Educational Robotics Intervention to Foster Computational Thinking in Preschoolers: Effects of Children’s Task Engagement
Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and strategies of problem-solving. Recent literature has pointed out the value of children acquiring computational thinking skills (i.e., unders...
Saved in:
Published in | Frontiers in psychology Vol. 13; p. 904761 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Frontiers Media S.A
21.06.2022
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and strategies of problem-solving. Recent literature has pointed out the value of children acquiring computational thinking skills (i.e., understanding and applying concepts, such as conditionals, iteration, or generalization), especially while learning STEM subjects. Robotics has been used as a tool to introduce computational thinking and STEM knowledge to children. As physical objects, robots have been proposed as developmentally appropriate for the early childhood setting, promoting motivation and allowing young learners to represent abstract ideas in a concrete setting. This study presents a novel educational robotics (ER) intervention using RoboTito, a robot programmable through tangible elements in its environment designed for kindergarteners. We used a quasi-experimental design with an active control group. In addition, we conducted a structured observation of the filmed material of the sessions to gather data on children’s attention and motivation throughout the activities. Fifty-one children (male = 33; mean age = 66 months, SD = 5.49 months) attending level 5 (kindergarten) at a Uruguayan public school participated in the study. Children in our experimental condition participated in an intervention programming RoboTito using tangible elements, while children in our control condition played with the robot through sensory-motor activities using a remote control and did not engage in programming. Motivational and attentional factors were assessed through video-recorded sessions of the ER activities. Four trained observers blind to the experimental conditions participated in the coding. Children’s interactions were assessed in four categories: task engagement, distractibility, oral participation, and objective fulfillment. Our results suggest children’s task engagement mediated their gains in CT after the intervention;
post-hoc
Tukey contrasts revealed non-significant pre-test to post-test gains for the control and low engagement groups, and significant for the high engagement group. Overall, we conclude task engagement played a central role in children’s learning gains and our robotics intervention was successful in promoting CT for engaged children. We discuss the practical implications of our results for early childhood education and developmentally appropriate ER targeted for young learners. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and strategies of problem-solving. Recent literature has pointed out the value of children acquiring computational thinking skills (i.e., understanding and applying concepts, such as conditionals, iteration, or generalization), especially while learning STEM subjects. Robotics has been used as a tool to introduce computational thinking and STEM knowledge to children. As physical objects, robots have been proposed as developmentally appropriate for the early childhood setting, promoting motivation and allowing young learners to represent abstract ideas in a concrete setting. This study presents a novel educational robotics (ER) intervention using RoboTito, a robot programmable through tangible elements in its environment designed for kindergarteners. We used a quasi-experimental design with an active control group. In addition, we conducted a structured observation of the filmed material of the sessions to gather data on children's attention and motivation throughout the activities. Fifty-one children (male = 33; mean age = 66 months, SD = 5.49 months) attending level 5 (kindergarten) at a Uruguayan public school participated in the study. Children in our experimental condition participated in an intervention programming RoboTito using tangible elements, while children in our control condition played with the robot through sensory-motor activities using a remote control and did not engage in programming. Motivational and attentional factors were assessed through video-recorded sessions of the ER activities. Four trained observers blind to the experimental conditions participated in the coding. Children's interactions were assessed in four categories: task engagement, distractibility, oral participation, and objective fulfillment. Our results suggest children's task engagement mediated their gains in CT after the intervention; post-hoc Tukey contrasts revealed non-significant pre-test to post-test gains for the control and low engagement groups, and significant for the high engagement group. Overall, we conclude task engagement played a central role in children's learning gains and our robotics intervention was successful in promoting CT for engaged children. We discuss the practical implications of our results for early childhood education and developmentally appropriate ER targeted for young learners.Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and strategies of problem-solving. Recent literature has pointed out the value of children acquiring computational thinking skills (i.e., understanding and applying concepts, such as conditionals, iteration, or generalization), especially while learning STEM subjects. Robotics has been used as a tool to introduce computational thinking and STEM knowledge to children. As physical objects, robots have been proposed as developmentally appropriate for the early childhood setting, promoting motivation and allowing young learners to represent abstract ideas in a concrete setting. This study presents a novel educational robotics (ER) intervention using RoboTito, a robot programmable through tangible elements in its environment designed for kindergarteners. We used a quasi-experimental design with an active control group. In addition, we conducted a structured observation of the filmed material of the sessions to gather data on children's attention and motivation throughout the activities. Fifty-one children (male = 33; mean age = 66 months, SD = 5.49 months) attending level 5 (kindergarten) at a Uruguayan public school participated in the study. Children in our experimental condition participated in an intervention programming RoboTito using tangible elements, while children in our control condition played with the robot through sensory-motor activities using a remote control and did not engage in programming. Motivational and attentional factors were assessed through video-recorded sessions of the ER activities. Four trained observers blind to the experimental conditions participated in the coding. Children's interactions were assessed in four categories: task engagement, distractibility, oral participation, and objective fulfillment. Our results suggest children's task engagement mediated their gains in CT after the intervention; post-hoc Tukey contrasts revealed non-significant pre-test to post-test gains for the control and low engagement groups, and significant for the high engagement group. Overall, we conclude task engagement played a central role in children's learning gains and our robotics intervention was successful in promoting CT for engaged children. We discuss the practical implications of our results for early childhood education and developmentally appropriate ER targeted for young learners. Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and strategies of problem-solving. Recent literature has pointed out the value of children acquiring computational thinking skills (i.e., understanding and applying concepts, such as conditionals, iteration, or generalization), especially while learning STEM subjects. Robotics has been used as a tool to introduce computational thinking and STEM knowledge to children. As physical objects, robots have been proposed as developmentally appropriate for the early childhood setting, promoting motivation and allowing young learners to represent abstract ideas in a concrete setting. This study presents a novel educational robotics (ER) intervention using RoboTito, a robot programmable through tangible elements in its environment designed for kindergarteners. We used a quasi-experimental design with an active control group. In addition, we conducted a structured observation of the filmed material of the sessions to gather data on children’s attention and motivation throughout the activities. Fifty-one children (male = 33; mean age = 66 months, SD = 5.49 months) attending level 5 (kindergarten) at a Uruguayan public school participated in the study. Children in our experimental condition participated in an intervention programming RoboTito using tangible elements, while children in our control condition played with the robot through sensory-motor activities using a remote control and did not engage in programming. Motivational and attentional factors were assessed through video-recorded sessions of the ER activities. Four trained observers blind to the experimental conditions participated in the coding. Children’s interactions were assessed in four categories: task engagement, distractibility, oral participation, and objective fulfillment. Our results suggest children’s task engagement mediated their gains in CT after the intervention; post-hoc Tukey contrasts revealed non-significant pre-test to post-test gains for the control and low engagement groups, and significant for the high engagement group. Overall, we conclude task engagement played a central role in children’s learning gains and our robotics intervention was successful in promoting CT for engaged children. We discuss the practical implications of our results for early childhood education and developmentally appropriate ER targeted for young learners. Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and strategies of problem-solving. Recent literature has pointed out the value of children acquiring computational thinking skills (i.e., understanding and applying concepts, such as conditionals, iteration, or generalization), especially while learning STEM subjects. Robotics has been used as a tool to introduce computational thinking and STEM knowledge to children. As physical objects, robots have been proposed as developmentally appropriate for the early childhood setting, promoting motivation and allowing young learners to represent abstract ideas in a concrete setting. This study presents a novel educational robotics (ER) intervention using RoboTito, a robot programmable through tangible elements in its environment designed for kindergarteners. We used a quasi-experimental design with an active control group. In addition, we conducted a structured observation of the filmed material of the sessions to gather data on children’s attention and motivation throughout the activities. Fifty-one children (male = 33; mean age = 66 months, SD = 5.49 months) attending level 5 (kindergarten) at a Uruguayan public school participated in the study. Children in our experimental condition participated in an intervention programming RoboTito using tangible elements, while children in our control condition played with the robot through sensory-motor activities using a remote control and did not engage in programming. Motivational and attentional factors were assessed through video-recorded sessions of the ER activities. Four trained observers blind to the experimental conditions participated in the coding. Children’s interactions were assessed in four categories: task engagement, distractibility, oral participation, and objective fulfillment. Our results suggest children’s task engagement mediated their gains in CT after the intervention; post-hoc Tukey contrasts revealed non-significant pre-test to post-test gains for the control and low engagement groups, and significant for the high engagement group. Overall, we conclude task engagement played a central role in children’s learning gains and our robotics intervention was successful in promoting CT for engaged children. We discuss the practical implications of our results for early childhood education and developmentally appropriate ER targeted for young learners. |
Author | Gerosa, Anaclara Koleszar, Víctor Tejera, Gonzalo Carboni, Alejandra Gómez-Sena, Leonel |
AuthorAffiliation | 4 Instituto de Fundamentos y Métodos, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay 1 Centro Interdisciplinario en Cognición para la Enseñanza y el Aprendizaje, Espacio Interdisciplinario, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay 2 Instituto de Computación, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay 3 Laboratorio de Neurociencias, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 1 Centro Interdisciplinario en Cognición para la Enseñanza y el Aprendizaje, Espacio Interdisciplinario, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay – name: 4 Instituto de Fundamentos y Métodos, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay – name: 2 Instituto de Computación, Facultad de Ingeniería, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay – name: 3 Laboratorio de Neurociencias, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de la República , Montevideo , Uruguay |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Anaclara surname: Gerosa fullname: Gerosa, Anaclara – sequence: 2 givenname: Víctor surname: Koleszar fullname: Koleszar, Víctor – sequence: 3 givenname: Gonzalo surname: Tejera fullname: Tejera, Gonzalo – sequence: 4 givenname: Leonel surname: Gómez-Sena fullname: Gómez-Sena, Leonel – sequence: 5 givenname: Alejandra surname: Carboni fullname: Carboni, Alejandra |
BookMark | eNp9kt9uFCEUxolpY2vtA3jHpTe7AsMA44WJ2Wx1kyY2zXpNGP7M0s7ACjNNGm98DV_PJ5HtVGO9KBdwcjjfj3PC9wochRgsAG8wWlaVaN65fb7vlgQRsmwQ5Qy_AKeYMbrAiIujf-ITcJ7zDSqLIoIQeQlOqlog1BB8Cr6vzaTV6GNQPbyObRy9znATRpvubDjk4RjhRcwlAVdx2E_jn-rtzodbHzroA7xKNutdjL1N-T1cO2f1mGF0cLXzvUk2_PrxM8OtyrdwHTrV2aHAX4Njp_pszx_PM_D1Yr1dfV5cfvm0WX28XGhKybgwznJUG2pYjQwylGLRWsN1QxTHCmGNSY1tUzbHGkpapoSuMXXKNLzRtq7OwGbmmqhu5D75QaV7GZWXD4mYOqlSmbu3kjrc1ITUrWOCtrgWmjBUUY4cElg4XVgfZtZ-agdrdBkjqf4J9OlN8DvZxTtZ-qtYVRXA20dAit8mm0c5-Kxt36tg45QlYYJzgjHmpRTPpTrFnJN1f5_BSB48IB88IA8ekLMHiob_p9F-_rHSje-fUf4Guna7eA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1080_2331186X_2024_2324487 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tsc_2024_101528 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2024_e29177 crossref_primary_10_1177_07356331241226459 crossref_primary_10_1177_07356331241236744 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10643_023_01563_y crossref_primary_10_3390_educsci13020134 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/s10643-012-0554-5 10.1145/1118178.1118215 10.3390/su10040905 10.1002/cae.22255 10.3102/0013189X12463051 10.2505/4/ss14_038_03_10 10.1007/s10643-021-01236-8 10.15388/infedu.2018.09 10.1016/j.caeo.2021.100043 10.1177/0735633120951871 10.1007/s40299-019-00438-4 10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.018 10.2224/sbp.7054 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.10.001 10.1177/0735633117743918 10.1007/978-981-13-6528-7_7 10.1002/cae.22295 10.1007/s11423-019-09694-z 10.1177/10567879221076077 10.1016/j.ijcci.2019.04.004 10.21585/ijcses.v3i4.76 10.1007/s10956-015-9581-5 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 10.1016/j.chb.2019.03.018 10.1504/IJMLO.2016.077867 10.28945/1887 10.4324/9780429453755-5 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.10.020 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.04.013 10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104425 10.3389/feduc.2021.657895 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735 10.1016/j.ijcci.2019.04.001 10.1007/s10639-018-9729-z 10.1007/s10956-017-9709-x 10.1016/j.ijcci.2021.100337 10.1080/02796015.2009.12087852 10.1007/s40299-019-00478-w 10.1177/0735633121992479 10.1177/1745691620904771 10.1007/s10639-015-9412-6 10.1016/B978-0-12-804036-2.00011-X 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103607 10.1080/08993408.2020.1789411 10.3102/00346543074001059 10.1007/s40751-017-0038-8 10.1017/9781108654555.023 10.3916/C59-2019-06 10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.047 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2022 Gerosa, Koleszar, Tejera, Gómez-Sena and Carboni. Copyright © 2022 Gerosa, Koleszar, Tejera, Gómez-Sena and Carboni. 2022 Gerosa, Koleszar, Tejera, Gómez-Sena and Carboni |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2022 Gerosa, Koleszar, Tejera, Gómez-Sena and Carboni. – notice: Copyright © 2022 Gerosa, Koleszar, Tejera, Gómez-Sena and Carboni. 2022 Gerosa, Koleszar, Tejera, Gómez-Sena and Carboni |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.904761 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1664-1078 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_4f195225bf684b158c2603470f0818fc PMC9253633 10_3389_fpsyg_2022_904761 |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Uruguay’s National Agency for Research and Innovation (ANII) grantid: FSED_2_2017_1_138793 – fundername: Interdisciplinary Cognition Center for Research and Learning grantid: CICEA-UDELAR |
GroupedDBID | 53G 5VS 9T4 AAFWJ AAKDD AAYXX ABIVO ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACXDI ADBBV ADRAZ AEGXH AFPKN AIAGR ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS BAWUL BCNDV CITATION DIK EBS EJD EMOBN F5P GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HYE KQ8 M48 M~E O5R O5S OK1 P2P PGMZT RNS RPM 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-dfe705d4d650d0d4418bed7c92a71a01c1251e9251f6942b6a8c514fad979ce53 |
IEDL.DBID | M48 |
ISSN | 1664-1078 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:23:58 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 14:10:15 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 01:51:17 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:35:09 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:07:50 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Language | English |
License | This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c442t-dfe705d4d650d0d4418bed7c92a71a01c1251e9251f6942b6a8c514fad979ce53 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 This article was submitted to Educational Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology Reviewed by: Spyros Vosinakis, University of the Aegean, Greece; Kan Kan Chan, City University of Macau, Macao SAR, China Edited by: Stamatios Papadakis, University of Crete, Greece |
OpenAccessLink | http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2022.904761 |
PMID | 35800921 |
PQID | 2687721117 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_4f195225bf684b158c2603470f0818fc pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9253633 proquest_miscellaneous_2687721117 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_904761 crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_904761 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2022-06-21 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2022-06-21 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 06 year: 2022 text: 2022-06-21 day: 21 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationTitle | Frontiers in psychology |
PublicationYear | 2022 |
Publisher | Frontiers Media S.A |
Publisher_xml | – name: Frontiers Media S.A |
References | Papadakis (ref38) 2022 Saxena (ref48) 2020; 29 Huang (ref27) 2021; 31 Hickmott (ref25) 2018; 4 Papadakis (ref37) 2021; 6 Bers (ref8) 2014; 72 Bers (ref6) 2008 Wing (ref62) 2011; 6 Weintrop (ref60) 2016; 25 Di Lieto (ref15) 2017; 71 Stoeckelmayr (ref52) 2011 (ref29) 2015 Muñoz-Repiso (ref35) 2019; 59 Critten (ref14) 2021 Janka (ref30) 2008 Sullivan (ref54) 2013; 12 (ref56) 2019 Zelazo (ref65) 2005 Tsarava (ref58) 2022; 179 Zapata Cáceres (ref64) 2020 Robertson (ref002) 2020; 3 Relkin (ref44) 2019 Kazakoff (ref32) 2013; 41 Zhang (ref66) 2019; 141 Perone (ref40) 2018 Sapounidis (ref47) 2016 Grover (ref24) 2013; 42 Yu (ref63) 2019; 21 Román-González (ref46) 2017; 72 Atmatzidou (ref2) 2018; 27 Fredricks (ref20) 2004; 74 Gerosa (ref21) 2019 Lei (ref33) 2018; 46 Swanson (ref55) 2019 Papadakis (ref39) 2016; 10 Raven (ref43) 1986 Caeli (ref12) 2020; 68 Doebel (ref17) 2020; 15 Gerosa (ref22) 2021; 2 Wing (ref61) 2006; 49 Ponitz (ref41) 2009; 38 Tran (ref57) 2019; 57 Pasquali (ref001) 2002; 2 Repenning (ref45) 2010 Jung (ref31) 2018; 10 Sullivan (ref53) 2017 Sharma (ref49) 2019; 21 Bell (ref5) 2018; 17 Eguchi (ref18) 2007 Bers (ref9) 2019; 138 González (ref23) 2018 Nam (ref36) 2019; 28 Angeli (ref1) 2020; 105 Bakala (ref3) 2021; 29 Fagerlund (ref19) 2021; 29 Horn (ref26) 2019; 1 Raven (ref42) 2000; 41 Sneider (ref50) 2014; 38 Ioannou (ref28) 2018; 23 Stewart (ref51) 2021; 59 Blair (ref10) 2016; 41 Bocconi (ref11) 2016 Diamond (ref16) 2013; 64 Zhong (ref67) 2021; 59 Bers (ref7) 2021 Cho (ref13) 2017; 95 Lyon (ref34) 2020; 28 Bakała (ref4) 2019 Voogt (ref59) 2015; 20 |
References_xml | – volume: 41 start-page: 245 year: 2013 ident: ref32 article-title: The effect of a classroom-based intensive robotics and programming workshop on sequencing ability in early childhood publication-title: Early Childhood Educ. J. doi: 10.1007/s10643-012-0554-5 – year: 2018 ident: ref23 – volume: 49 start-page: 33 year: 2006 ident: ref61 article-title: Computational thinking publication-title: Commun. ACM doi: 10.1145/1118178.1118215 – year: 2005 ident: ref65 – volume: 95 start-page: 1 year: 2017 ident: ref13 article-title: Possibility of improving computational thinking through activity based learning strategy for young children publication-title: J. Theor. Appl. Inf. Technol. – year: 2015 ident: ref29 – year: 2008 ident: ref30 – volume: 10 start-page: 905 year: 2018 ident: ref31 article-title: Systematic review of research trends in robotics education for young children publication-title: Sustainability doi: 10.3390/su10040905 – volume: 29 start-page: 12 year: 2021 ident: ref19 article-title: Computational thinking in programming with scratch in primary schools: a systematic review publication-title: Comput. Appl. Eng. Educ. doi: 10.1002/cae.22255 – volume: 42 start-page: 38 year: 2013 ident: ref24 article-title: Computational thinking in K–12: a review of the state of the field publication-title: Educ. Res. doi: 10.3102/0013189X12463051 – volume: 38 start-page: 10 year: 2014 ident: ref50 article-title: Exploring the science framework and NGSS: computational thinking in the science classroom publication-title: Sci. Scope doi: 10.2505/4/ss14_038_03_10 – start-page: 1 year: 2021 ident: ref14 article-title: Can pre-school children learn programming and coding through guided play activities? A case study in computational thinking publication-title: Early Childhood Educ. J. doi: 10.1007/s10643-021-01236-8 – volume: 17 start-page: 151 year: 2018 ident: ref5 article-title: Integrating computational thinking with a music education context publication-title: Inf. Educ. doi: 10.15388/infedu.2018.09 – volume: 2 start-page: 100043 year: 2021 ident: ref22 article-title: Cognitive abilities and computational thinking at age 5: evidence for associations to sequencing and symbolic number comparison publication-title: Comput. Educ. Open doi: 10.1016/j.caeo.2021.100043 – volume: 59 start-page: 95 year: 2021 ident: ref67 article-title: Troubleshooting to learn via scaffolds: effect on students’ ability and cognitive load in a robotics course publication-title: J. Educ. Comput. Res. doi: 10.1177/0735633120951871 – volume: 28 start-page: 387 year: 2019 ident: ref36 article-title: Connecting plans to action: the effects of a card-coded robotics curriculum and activities on Korean kindergartners publication-title: Asia Pac. Educ. Res. doi: 10.1007/s40299-019-00438-4 – volume: 71 start-page: 16 year: 2017 ident: ref15 article-title: Educational robotics intervention on executive functions in preschool children: a pilot study publication-title: Comput. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.018 – volume: 2 start-page: 95 year: 2002 ident: ref001 article-title: Matrizes progressivas do Raven infantil: Um estudo de validação para o Brasil publication-title: Aval. Psicol. – volume: 46 start-page: 517 year: 2018 ident: ref33 article-title: Relationships between student engagement and academic achievement: a meta-analysis publication-title: Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. doi: 10.2224/sbp.7054 – year: 2010 ident: ref45 – volume: 41 start-page: 85 year: 2016 ident: ref10 article-title: Moderating effects of executive functions and the teacher–child relationship on the development of mathematics ability in kindergarten publication-title: Learn. Instr. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.10.001 – volume: 57 start-page: 3 year: 2019 ident: ref57 article-title: Computational thinking equity in elementary classrooms: what third-grade students know and can do publication-title: J. Educ. Comput. Res. doi: 10.1177/0735633117743918 – start-page: 99 volume-title: Computational Thinking Education. year: 2019 ident: ref55 article-title: Introducing and assessing computational thinking in the secondary science classroom, doi: 10.1007/978-981-13-6528-7_7 – volume: 28 start-page: 1174 year: 2020 ident: ref34 article-title: Computational thinking in higher education: a review of the literature publication-title: Comput. Appl. Eng. Educ. doi: 10.1002/cae.22295 – volume: 68 start-page: 551 year: 2020 ident: ref12 article-title: Computational thinking in compulsory education: a survey study on initiatives and conceptions publication-title: Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. doi: 10.1007/s11423-019-09694-z – year: 2007 ident: ref18 – year: 2011 ident: ref52 – start-page: 1 year: 2022 ident: ref38 article-title: Can preschoolers learn computational thinking and coding skills with ScratchJr? A systematic literature review publication-title: Int. J. Educ. Reform. doi: 10.1177/10567879221076077 – volume: 21 start-page: 65 year: 2019 ident: ref49 article-title: Coding games and robots to enhance computational thinking: how collaboration and engagement moderate children’s attitudes? publication-title: Int. J. Child Comput. Interact. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2019.04.004 – volume: 3 start-page: 35 year: 2020 ident: ref002 article-title: The relationship between executive functions and computational thinking publication-title: Int. J. Comput. Sci. Educ. Sch. doi: 10.21585/ijcses.v3i4.76 – volume: 25 start-page: 127 year: 2016 ident: ref60 article-title: Defining computational thinking for mathematics and science classrooms publication-title: J. Sci. Educ. Technol. doi: 10.1007/s10956-015-9581-5 – volume: 64 start-page: 135 year: 2013 ident: ref16 article-title: Executive functions publication-title: Annu. Rev. Psychol. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 – year: 2019 ident: ref21 – year: 2016 ident: ref47 – volume: 6 start-page: 20 year: 2011 ident: ref62 article-title: Research notebook: computational thinking—what and why publication-title: Link Mag. – volume: 105 start-page: 105954 year: 2020 ident: ref1 article-title: Developing young children's computational thinking with educational robotics: An interaction effect between gender and scaffolding strategy publication-title: Comput. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.03.018 – volume: 10 start-page: 187 year: 2016 ident: ref39 article-title: Developing fundamental programming concepts and computational thinking with ScratchJr in preschool education: a case study publication-title: Int. J. Mob. Learn. Org. doi: 10.1504/IJMLO.2016.077867 – volume: 12 start-page: 203 year: 2013 ident: ref54 article-title: The wheels on the bot go round and round: robotics curriculum in pre-kindergarten publication-title: J. Inf. Technol. Educ. doi: 10.28945/1887 – start-page: 83 volume-title: STEM in Early Childhood Learners: How Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics Strengthen Learning. year: 2019 ident: ref44 article-title: Designing an assessment of computational thinking abilities for young children, doi: 10.4324/9780429453755-5 – volume: 72 start-page: 145 year: 2014 ident: ref8 article-title: Computational thinking and tinkering: exploration of an early childhood robotics curriculum publication-title: Comput. Educ. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.10.020 – volume: 138 start-page: 130 year: 2019 ident: ref9 article-title: Coding as a playground: promoting positive learning experiences in childhood classrooms publication-title: Comput. Educ. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.04.013 – volume: 179 start-page: 104425 year: 2022 ident: ref58 article-title: A cognitive definition of computational thinking in primary education publication-title: Comput. Educ. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104425 – volume: 6 start-page: 657895 year: 2021 ident: ref37 article-title: The impact of coding apps on young children computational thinking and coding skills. A literature review publication-title: Front. Educ. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2021.657895 – volume: 41 start-page: 1 year: 2000 ident: ref42 article-title: The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time publication-title: Cogn. Psychol. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735 – volume: 21 start-page: 17 year: 2019 ident: ref63 article-title: A review of computational toys and kits for young children publication-title: Int. J. Child Comput. Interact. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2019.04.001 – volume: 23 start-page: 2531 year: 2018 ident: ref28 article-title: Exploring the potentials of educational robotics in the development of computational thinking: a summary of current research and practical proposal for future work publication-title: Educ. Inf. Technol. doi: 10.1007/s10639-018-9729-z – volume: 27 start-page: 70 year: 2018 ident: ref2 article-title: How does the degree of guidance support students’ metacognitive and problem solving skills in educational robotics? publication-title: J. Sci. Educ. Technol. doi: 10.1007/s10956-017-9709-x – volume: 29 start-page: 100337 year: 2021 ident: ref3 article-title: Preschool children, robots, and computational thinking: a systematic review publication-title: Int. J. Child Comput. Interact. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2021.100337 – year: 2016 ident: ref11 – volume: 38 start-page: 102 year: 2009 ident: ref41 article-title: Kindergarten classroom quality, behavioral engagement, and reading achievement publication-title: Sch. Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2009.12087852 – volume-title: Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Raven’s Coloured Matrices year: 1986 ident: ref43 – year: 2019 ident: ref56 – year: 2020 ident: ref64 – volume: 29 start-page: 55 year: 2020 ident: ref48 article-title: Designing unplugged and plugged activities to cultivate computational thinking: an exploratory study in early childhood education publication-title: Asia Pac. Educ. Res. doi: 10.1007/s40299-019-00478-w – volume: 59 start-page: 1208 year: 2021 ident: ref51 article-title: Exploring factors that influence computational thinking skills in elementary students’ collaborative robotics publication-title: J. Educ. Comput. Res. doi: 10.1177/0735633121992479 – volume-title: Coding as a Playground: Programming and Computational Thinking in the Early Childhood Classroom. year: 2021 ident: ref7 – volume: 15 start-page: 942 year: 2020 ident: ref17 article-title: Rethinking executive function and its development publication-title: Perspect. Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1177/1745691620904771 – year: 2019 ident: ref4 – volume: 20 start-page: 715 year: 2015 ident: ref59 article-title: Computational thinking in compulsory education: towards an agenda for research and practice publication-title: Educ. Inf. Technol. doi: 10.1007/s10639-015-9412-6 – start-page: 291 volume-title: The Neurobiology of Brain and Behavioral Development. year: 2018 ident: ref40 article-title: Toward an understanding of the neural basis of executive function development, doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804036-2.00011-X – volume: 141 start-page: 103607 year: 2019 ident: ref66 article-title: A systematic review of learning computational thinking through scratch in K-9 publication-title: Comput. Educ. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103607 – volume-title: Blocks to Robots Learning with Technology in the Early Childhood Classroom. year: 2008 ident: ref6 – volume: 31 start-page: 83 year: 2021 ident: ref27 article-title: A critical review of literature on “unplugged” pedagogies in K-12 computer science and computational thinking education publication-title: Comput. Sci. Educ. doi: 10.1080/08993408.2020.1789411 – volume: 74 start-page: 59 year: 2004 ident: ref20 article-title: School engagement: potential of the concept, state of the evidence publication-title: Rev. Educ. Res. doi: 10.3102/00346543074001059 – start-page: 110 volume-title: Imagining, Playing, and Coding with KIBO: Using Robotics to Foster Computational Thinking in Young Children year: 2017 ident: ref53 – volume: 4 start-page: 48 year: 2018 ident: ref25 article-title: A scoping review of studies on computational thinking in K–12 mathematics classrooms publication-title: Digit. Exp. Math. Educ. doi: 10.1007/s40751-017-0038-8 – volume: 1 start-page: 663 year: 2019 ident: ref26 article-title: Tangible computing publication-title: Cambridge Handb. Comput. Educ. Res. doi: 10.1017/9781108654555.023 – volume: 59 start-page: 63 year: 2019 ident: ref35 article-title: Robótica para desarrollar el pensamiento computacional en Educación Infantil publication-title: Comunicar doi: 10.3916/C59-2019-06 – volume: 72 start-page: 678 year: 2017 ident: ref46 article-title: Which cognitive abilities underlie computational thinking? Criterion validity of the computational thinking test publication-title: Comput. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.047 |
SSID | ssj0000402002 |
Score | 2.4352767 |
Snippet | Computational thinking (CT) is a broadly used term in education to refer to the cognitive processes underlying the application of computer science concepts and... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Enrichment Source Index Database |
StartPage | 904761 |
SubjectTerms | cognitive development computational thinking early childhood preschool Psychology robotics task engagement |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1NT9wwELUqTlwqKFRsochIPSGlxI7jj94AsaJI9FCBxM2yHZsiUILIckC99G_07_FLmEmyq80FLlyTyZffxH5jj98Q8k0lrDeQwHm1xKkbX2Q6RZPpQiSRnOFlpzN7_kueXoqzq_JqqdQX5oT18sB9wx2IxAxwhNInqYVnpQ7AwAuh8oRibClg7wtj3lIw1fXBGBblvF_GhCjMHKT79uka4kHOv5tcKMlGA1Gn1z8imeMUyaUxZ7pGPg5kkR72L7lOPsT6E1ld9FlPG-TvIkED7H43vkHVZfpzKZGRzho6xZ0cD7Sv4DC3xoqdOE9Ob2qKiRidHCeQwR-0VzRuaZPo8bDX-_nf_5ZeuPaWntTXQ77MJrmcnlwcn2ZDPYUsCMFnWZWiystKVMDKqrwCIqR9rFQw3CnmchaQ7ESAhyVpBPfS6QB8KrnKKBNiWXwmK3VTxy1CQxEjUB-4ifZCO-FkiEAFmc_LpJ0TE5LPG9eGQWwca17cWQg6EA_b4WERD9vjMSH7i0vue6WN14yPELGFIYpkdwfAdezgOvYt15mQvTneFn4qXClxdWweW8ulVhgaMzUhauQIoyeOz9Q3fzp5bmjAQhbFl_d4xW2yil-NuWmc7ZCV2cNj_AosaOZ3O4d_ARKFCCs priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals |
Title | Educational Robotics Intervention to Foster Computational Thinking in Preschoolers: Effects of Children’s Task Engagement |
URI | https://www.proquest.com/docview/2687721117 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9253633 https://doaj.org/article/4f195225bf684b158c2603470f0818fc |
Volume | 13 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3fS-QwEA6iL77I6SnunUqEA0Go9keapAfHcS4ue4I-iAu-hSRN9uSk1e0K7n_vTNpdLMhxr22a0kwm833J9BtCvgmP9QY8TF7JcevGZJH0rohkxjzzukjzoDN7fcPHE3Z1n9-vkWV5q24Amw-pHdaTmswez16fFz_B4X8g44R4e-6fmsUUqF6anhUxE0iGNiAwCfTT6w7th4UZuVKbhcg5gwVIyPac8-NeepEqCPr3UGg_h_JdUBp9IlsdmqS_WvNvkzVX7ZDN1aK2-EwWqwwOaHdbmxplmenvd5mOdF7TEf7qMaNtiYdlayzpiRvp9KGimKkR9DoBLX6nreRxQ2tPh93P4CcNvdPNX3pZTbt0ml0yGV3eDcdRV24hsoyl86j0TsR5yUoAbWVcAk6SxpXCFqkWiY4Ti1jIgfUSzwuWGq6lBbjldVmIwro82yPrVV25fUJt5hwgI-hEGiY109w6QIqJiXMvtWYDEi-HVtlOixxLYjwq4CRoDRWsodAaqrXGgJyuHnlqhTj-1fgC7bVqiBra4UI9m6rOJRXzSQHoMzeeS2aSXFrgdhkTsUeZP28H5HhpbQU-hwcpunL1S6NSLgUy50QMiOhNg94b-3eqhz9BvRsGMONZ9uU_ev9KNvGjMDMtTQ7I-nz24g4BA83NUdg7OArz-w3vCAgD |
linkProvider | Scholars Portal |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Educational+Robotics+Intervention+to+Foster+Computational+Thinking+in+Preschoolers%3A+Effects+of+Children%27s+Task+Engagement&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+psychology&rft.au=Gerosa%2C+Anaclara&rft.au=Koleszar%2C+V%C3%ADctor&rft.au=Tejera%2C+Gonzalo&rft.au=G%C3%B3mez-Sena%2C+Leonel&rft.date=2022-06-21&rft.issn=1664-1078&rft.eissn=1664-1078&rft.volume=13&rft.spage=904761&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2022.904761&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |