Sensory Representations Supporting Memory Specificity: Age Effects on Behavioral and Neural Discriminability

Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inThe Journal of neuroscience Vol. 39; no. 12; pp. 2265 - 2275
Main Authors Bowman, Caitlin R., Chamberlain, Jordan D., Dennis, Nancy A.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Society for Neuroscience 20.03.2019
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0270-6474
1529-2401
1529-2401
DOI10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2022-18.2019

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects, the present study measured the extent of age differences in the specificity of memory representations using a false memory paradigm in which studied items were linked to retrieval items at multiple levels of similarity. Older adults showed poorer behavioral discrimination than young adults, driven primarily by false recognition of lures that differed from targets only in perceptual details. Patterns of activation across several regions within ventral visual cortex could be used to distinguish between targets and lures when they differed in both perceptual details and a semantic label. However, of ventral visual regions, only signals in the midline occipital cortex could be used to distinguish targets from lures when they differed only in perceptual details. Although there was an overall age deficit for this neural discrimination in this region, the positive relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability did not differ across age groups. In contrast, age moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability in lateral occipital and fusiform cortices, suggesting that activation patterns within these regions represent different types of information in each age group. Therefore, the quality of perceptual signals is a key contributor to memory discrimination across age groups, with evidence that age differences in the nature of representations emerges outside early visual cortex. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Age-related memory decline is due in part to older adults' difficulties in discriminating between old and new information. We tested whether this deficit arises from lack of specificity in the sensory representations underlying older adults' recognition judgments. Using pattern classification analyses in ventral visual cortices, we found that signals in a region early in the visual stream could distinguish between targets and lures at the highest level of similarity. The discriminability of targets and lures in this region was positively related to behavioral discriminability across age groups despite an overall age deficit in classification accuracy. Together, results showed that older adults' memory deficits are related to reduced discriminability of cognitive processes (old/new recognition) in portions of visual cortex.
AbstractList Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects, the present study measured the extent of age differences in the specificity of memory representations using a false memory paradigm in which studied items were linked to retrieval items at multiple levels of similarity. Older adults showed poorer behavioral discrimination than young adults, driven primarily by false recognition of lures that differed from targets only in perceptual details. Patterns of activation across several regions within ventral visual cortex could be used to distinguish between targets and lures when they differed in both perceptual details and a semantic label. However, of ventral visual regions, only signals in the midline occipital cortex could be used to distinguish targets from lures when they differed only in perceptual details. Although there was an overall age deficit for this neural discrimination in this region, the positive relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability did not differ across age groups. In contrast, age moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability in lateral occipital and fusiform cortices, suggesting that activation patterns within these regions represent different types of information in each age group. Therefore, the quality of perceptual signals is a key contributor to memory discrimination across age groups, with evidence that age differences in the nature of representations emerges outside early visual cortex.
Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects, the present study measured the extent of age differences in the specificity of memory representations using a false memory paradigm in which studied items were linked to retrieval items at multiple levels of similarity. Older adults showed poorer behavioral discrimination than young adults, driven primarily by false recognition of lures that differed from targets only in perceptual details. Patterns of activation across several regions within ventral visual cortex could be used to distinguish between targets and lures when they differed in both perceptual details and a semantic label. However, of ventral visual regions, only signals in the midline occipital cortex could be used to distinguish targets from lures when they differed only in perceptual details. Although there was an overall age deficit for this neural discrimination in this region, the positive relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability did not differ across age groups. In contrast, age moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability in lateral occipital and fusiform cortices, suggesting that activation patterns within these regions represent different types of information in each age group. Therefore, the quality of perceptual signals is a key contributor to memory discrimination across age groups, with evidence that age differences in the nature of representations emerges outside early visual cortex. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Age-related memory decline is due in part to older adults' difficulties in discriminating between old and new information. We tested whether this deficit arises from lack of specificity in the sensory representations underlying older adults' recognition judgments. Using pattern classification analyses in ventral visual cortices, we found that signals in a region early in the visual stream could distinguish between targets and lures at the highest level of similarity. The discriminability of targets and lures in this region was positively related to behavioral discriminability across age groups despite an overall age deficit in classification accuracy. Together, results showed that older adults' memory deficits are related to reduced discriminability of cognitive processes (old/new recognition) in portions of visual cortex.
Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects, the present study measured the extent of age differences in the specificity of memory representations using a false memory paradigm in which studied items were linked to retrieval items at multiple levels of similarity. Older adults showed poorer behavioral discrimination than young adults, driven primarily by false recognition of lures that differed from targets only in perceptual details. Patterns of activation across several regions within ventral visual cortex could be used to distinguish between targets and lures when they differed in both perceptual details and a semantic label. However, of ventral visual regions, only signals in the midline occipital cortex could be used to distinguish targets from lures when they differed only in perceptual details. Although there was an overall age deficit for this neural discrimination in this region, the positive relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability did not differ across age groups. In contrast, age moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability in lateral occipital and fusiform cortices, suggesting that activation patterns within these regions represent different types of information in each age group. Therefore, the quality of perceptual signals is a key contributor to memory discrimination across age groups, with evidence that age differences in the nature of representations emerges outside early visual cortex. Age-related memory decline is due in part to older adults' difficulties in discriminating between old and new information. We tested whether this deficit arises from lack of specificity in the sensory representations underlying older adults' recognition judgments. Using pattern classification analyses in ventral visual cortices, we found that signals in a region early in the visual stream could distinguish between targets and lures at the highest level of similarity. The discriminability of targets and lures in this region was positively related to behavioral discriminability across age groups despite an overall age deficit in classification accuracy. Together, results showed that older adults' memory deficits are related to reduced discriminability of cognitive processes (old/new recognition) in portions of visual cortex.
Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects, the present study measured the extent of age differences in the specificity of memory representations using a false memory paradigm in which studied items were linked to retrieval items at multiple levels of similarity. Older adults showed poorer behavioral discrimination than young adults, driven primarily by false recognition of lures that differed from targets only in perceptual details. Patterns of activation across several regions within ventral visual cortex could be used to distinguish between targets and lures when they differed in both perceptual details and a semantic label. However, of ventral visual regions, only signals in the midline occipital cortex could be used to distinguish targets from lures when they differed only in perceptual details. Although there was an overall age deficit for this neural discrimination in this region, the positive relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability did not differ across age groups. In contrast, age moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability in lateral occipital and fusiform cortices, suggesting that activation patterns within these regions represent different types of information in each age group. Therefore, the quality of perceptual signals is a key contributor to memory discrimination across age groups, with evidence that age differences in the nature of representations emerges outside early visual cortex.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Age-related memory decline is due in part to older adults' difficulties in discriminating between old and new information. We tested whether this deficit arises from lack of specificity in the sensory representations underlying older adults' recognition judgments. Using pattern classification analyses in ventral visual cortices, we found that signals in a region early in the visual stream could distinguish between targets and lures at the highest level of similarity. The discriminability of targets and lures in this region was positively related to behavioral discriminability across age groups despite an overall age deficit in classification accuracy. Together, results showed that older adults' memory deficits are related to reduced discriminability of cognitive processes (old/new recognition) in portions of visual cortex.Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely than young adults to retrieve specific sensory details necessary to distinguish between similar items. In male and female human subjects, the present study measured the extent of age differences in the specificity of memory representations using a false memory paradigm in which studied items were linked to retrieval items at multiple levels of similarity. Older adults showed poorer behavioral discrimination than young adults, driven primarily by false recognition of lures that differed from targets only in perceptual details. Patterns of activation across several regions within ventral visual cortex could be used to distinguish between targets and lures when they differed in both perceptual details and a semantic label. However, of ventral visual regions, only signals in the midline occipital cortex could be used to distinguish targets from lures when they differed only in perceptual details. Although there was an overall age deficit for this neural discrimination in this region, the positive relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability did not differ across age groups. In contrast, age moderated the relationship between neural and behavioral discriminability in lateral occipital and fusiform cortices, suggesting that activation patterns within these regions represent different types of information in each age group. Therefore, the quality of perceptual signals is a key contributor to memory discrimination across age groups, with evidence that age differences in the nature of representations emerges outside early visual cortex.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Age-related memory decline is due in part to older adults' difficulties in discriminating between old and new information. We tested whether this deficit arises from lack of specificity in the sensory representations underlying older adults' recognition judgments. Using pattern classification analyses in ventral visual cortices, we found that signals in a region early in the visual stream could distinguish between targets and lures at the highest level of similarity. The discriminability of targets and lures in this region was positively related to behavioral discriminability across age groups despite an overall age deficit in classification accuracy. Together, results showed that older adults' memory deficits are related to reduced discriminability of cognitive processes (old/new recognition) in portions of visual cortex.
Author Dennis, Nancy A.
Bowman, Caitlin R.
Chamberlain, Jordan D.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Caitlin R.
  orcidid: 0000-0002-5833-3591
  surname: Bowman
  fullname: Bowman, Caitlin R.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Jordan D.
  orcidid: 0000-0003-3520-1542
  surname: Chamberlain
  fullname: Chamberlain, Jordan D.
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Nancy A.
  orcidid: 0000-0003-2526-6334
  surname: Dennis
  fullname: Dennis, Nancy A.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30655350$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkVtP3DAQha0KVBbav4Ai9aUvob7F3lRVJbrdtiAKElueLccZL0ZZO9gJ0v77OuUi4KVPY2nOGZ-Zbx_t-OABoUOCj0hF2afT8-XV5cVqcXJEMaUlmedK6jdolrt1STkmO2iGqcSl4JLvof2UbjDGEhP5Fu0xLKqKVXiGuhX4FOK2uIQ-QgI_6MEFn4rV2PchDs6vi9-wmRSrHoyzzrhh-7k4XkOxtBbMkIrgi29wre9ciLortG-Lcxin53eXTHQb53Xjumx7h3at7hK8f6gH6OrH8s_iV3l28fNkcXxWGs7pUDZCQ6NJ01ommcUVJthQoxvetC1mtq61MLzGlnAwVFpGKLRaMw1aNGA5Ywfo6_3cfmw20Jq8VY6j-pxFx60K2qmXHe-u1TrcKZHNUsg84OPDgBhuR0iD2uRVoOu0hzAmRYmsWc0FJln64ZX0JozR5_UUzWhkTedznlWHzxM9RXkEkQVf7gUmhpQiWJXv_A9FDug6RbCauKsn7mrirshcTdyzXbyyP_7wH-NfHUu2BA
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0337_23_2023
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2019_107240
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2024_12_007
crossref_primary_10_1080_13825585_2022_2036682
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_44107_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2021_100926
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2022_119442
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_024_01245_1
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_023_01899_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2021_12_001
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_669481
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2022_01_011
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02052
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2021_05_021
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0959_23_2023
crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0142_20_2020
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2024_11_012
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1378_24_2024
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2020_01_006
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107658
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhaa210
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_82250
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psfr_2022_06_001
crossref_primary_10_1080_13825585_2022_2040411
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_021_00868_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2020_104556
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2025_104625
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2021_02_004
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1766_19_2020
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnsys_2021_687393
crossref_primary_10_1080_13825585_2022_2037500
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01593
crossref_primary_10_1080_09658211_2024_2319314
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01871
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnagi_2022_884993
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1763_21_2021
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhad219
crossref_primary_10_1080_13825585_2021_2019184
crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_023_04918_8
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhab157
Cites_doi 10.1080/09658211.2010.530271
10.1162/0898929052880048
10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790060110016
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5553-11.2012
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1498-18.2018
10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80448-1
10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01171-1
10.1037/h0046671
10.1126/science.1117645
10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.762
10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
10.1006/jmla.1997.2529
10.1162/jocn_a_00961
10.1162/jocn_a_00253
10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.08.019
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.267
10.1073/pnas.0405148101
10.1101/lm.045765.117
10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.078
10.1093/cercor/bhg133
10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3<109::AID-HBM7>3.0.CO;2-W
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4494-11.2012
10.1101/lm.045435.117
10.1371/journal.pone.0010773
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060
10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.120
10.1162/jocn_a_00740
10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00159-8
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021
10.1111/1467-9280.01441
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.079
10.1093/cercor/bhl156
10.1038/nn1252
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.014
10.1080/026432999380834
10.1080/09658211.2017.1387267
10.1093/cercor/bhm155
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.025
10.1300/J018v05n01_09
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-07.2007
10.1073/pnas.1016939108
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2019 the authors 0270-6474/19/392265-11$15.00/0.
Copyright Society for Neuroscience Mar 20, 2019
Copyright © 2019 the authors 0270-6474/19/392265-11$15.00/0 2019
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2019 the authors 0270-6474/19/392265-11$15.00/0.
– notice: Copyright Society for Neuroscience Mar 20, 2019
– notice: Copyright © 2019 the authors 0270-6474/19/392265-11$15.00/0 2019
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QR
7TK
7U7
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
P64
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2022-18.2019
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Toxicology Abstracts
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Engineering Research Database
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Toxicology Abstracts
Animal Behavior Abstracts
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList Virology and AIDS Abstracts

CrossRef
MEDLINE
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1529-2401
EndPage 2275
ExternalDocumentID PMC6433767
30655350
10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2022_18_2019
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: F32 AG054204
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
18M
2WC
34G
39C
53G
5GY
5RE
5VS
AAFWJ
AAJMC
AAYXX
ABBAR
ABIVO
ACGUR
ACNCT
ADBBV
ADCOW
ADHGD
AENEX
AFCFT
AFOSN
AFSQR
AHWXS
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BAWUL
BTFSW
CITATION
CS3
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
EJD
F5P
GX1
H13
HYE
H~9
KQ8
L7B
OK1
P0W
P2P
QZG
R.V
RHI
RPM
TFN
TR2
W8F
WH7
WOQ
X7M
YBU
YHG
YKV
YNH
YSK
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QR
7TK
7U7
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
P64
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-b6aeba1bdf373f05010c2cab4bdd03f99a6c490f14ec27f312edaa3aea6bef433
ISSN 0270-6474
1529-2401
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 13:56:25 EDT 2025
Fri Sep 05 12:05:56 EDT 2025
Mon Jun 30 16:43:05 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 07:01:59 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:15:41 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:06:29 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 12
Keywords functional MRI
aging
false memory
long-term memory
multivariate pattern analysis
Language English
License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0
Copyright © 2019 the authors 0270-6474/19/392265-11$15.00/0.
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c442t-b6aeba1bdf373f05010c2cab4bdd03f99a6c490f14ec27f312edaa3aea6bef433
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
Author contributions: C.R.B. wrote the first draft of the paper; C.R.B., J.D.C., and N.A.D. edited the paper; C.R.B. and N.A.D. designed research; C.R.B. performed research; C.R.B. and J.D.C. analyzed data; C.R.B. wrote the paper.
ORCID 0000-0003-2526-6334
0000-0002-5833-3591
0000-0003-3520-1542
OpenAccessLink https://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/39/12/2265.full.pdf
PMID 30655350
PQID 2202792884
PQPubID 2049535
PageCount 11
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6433767
proquest_miscellaneous_2179394601
proquest_journals_2202792884
pubmed_primary_30655350
crossref_citationtrail_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2022_18_2019
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2022_18_2019
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2019-03-20
20190320
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2019-03-20
PublicationDate_xml – month: 03
  year: 2019
  text: 2019-03-20
  day: 20
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: Baltimore
PublicationTitle The Journal of neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate J Neurosci
PublicationYear 2019
Publisher Society for Neuroscience
Publisher_xml – name: Society for Neuroscience
References 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.23
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.24
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.21
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.43
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.22
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.44
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.27
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.28
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.25
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.26
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.29
Bowman (2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.2) 2016; 28
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.41
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.20
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.42
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.40
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.12
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.34
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.13
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.35
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.10
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.32
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.11
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.33
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.16
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.38
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.17
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.39
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.14
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.36
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.15
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.37
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.18
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.19
McCormick-Huhn (2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.31) 2018; 26
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.9
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.7
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.8
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.5
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.6
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.3
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.30
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.4
2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.1
References_xml – ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.29
  doi: 10.1080/09658211.2010.530271
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.20
  doi: 10.1162/0898929052880048
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.16
  doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790060110016
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.40
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5553-11.2012
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.25
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1498-18.2018
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.36
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80448-1
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.39
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01171-1
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.11
  doi: 10.1037/h0046671
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.34
  doi: 10.1126/science.1117645
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.4
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.762
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.35
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.27
  doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2529
– volume: 28
  start-page: 1194
  year: 2016
  ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.2
  article-title: The neural basis of recollection rejection: increases in hippocampal-prefrontal connectivity in the absence of a shared recall-to-reject and target recollection network
  publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00961
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.6
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00253
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.14
  doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.08.019
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.8
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.267
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.32
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0405148101
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.23
  doi: 10.1101/lm.045765.117
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.42
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.19
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.078
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.7
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg133
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.9
  doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3<109::AID-HBM7>3.0.CO;2-W
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.33
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4494-11.2012
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.3
  doi: 10.1101/lm.045435.117
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.5
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010773
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.44
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.17
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.120
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.43
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00740
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.21
  doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00159-8
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.15
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.26
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01441
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.13
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.079
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.22
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl156
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.38
  doi: 10.1038/nn1252
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.12
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.18
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.014
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.1
  doi: 10.1080/026432999380834
– volume: 26
  start-page: 603
  year: 2018
  ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.31
  article-title: Repeated study of items with and without repeated context: aging effects on memory discriminability
  publication-title: Memory
  doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1387267
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.10
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.41
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.30
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.025
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.37
  doi: 10.1300/J018v05n01_09
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.24
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-07.2007
– ident: 2023041803352074000_39.12.2265.28
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016939108
SSID ssj0007017
Score 2.4800498
Snippet Older adults' difficulty in distinguishing between old and new information contributes to memory decline, which may occur because older adults are less likely...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage 2265
SubjectTerms Activation
Adolescent
Adult
Adults
Age
Age differences
Aging - physiology
Aging - psychology
Brain - physiology
Discrimination
Discrimination, Psychological - physiology
Female
Humans
Male
Memory Disorders - etiology
Memory Disorders - physiopathology
Mental Recall - physiology
Occipital lobe
Older people
Representations
Semantics
Signal quality
Somatosensory cortex
Visual cortex
Visual Cortex - physiology
Visual discrimination
Visual signals
Young Adult
Young adults
Title Sensory Representations Supporting Memory Specificity: Age Effects on Behavioral and Neural Discriminability
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30655350
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2202792884
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2179394601
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6433767
Volume 39
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEF6FcuGCgPIwFLRIiEvl1N5dv7hFIVUpJQiaSLlZa3tNKwUbJY5Q-Q38aGbW60fSitfFivyILc_n2Z2db74h5BXzA1_ANN8OUeRO5My1Exjm7SjKlAr9SMIcCdkWU_9kLk4X3mIw-NljLW2qZJj-uLGu5H-sCvvArlgl-w-Wbf8UdsBvsC9swcKw_Ssbn0MMijnyz5rOaqqIivUhtuosV5rQ_AGZtFd1m3kUi6i0wtPoizqcNEyOohFJXBndABTs0N4QXQpSZTSBdiv_21WU6blsTxWzy9KX383q6hib81wWHTdxfCGxD8lSmlZgEACDm2m5x2-RcbNufb9ZbjVLE1gNxW3mdEuTPd7pdPc5ahfHAgheRd2nZ6iMC2Y65-P2fXQteNRgkfU9Lqt7TVwbCjwtSXE6RUbk-fjdkGHdgqvZfFE3-DUJ_-nH-Hh-dhbPJovZLXKbQdCBjUDef-q05wNH929un9nUm8N9jm6-y_ZU51r8skvD7c1rZvfIXWNEOqrRdZ8MVPGA7I8KWZVfr-hrqinCOveyT5YGcHQHcLQDHK0BR3uAe0MBbtTAjZYF7eBGAW60hhvdhdtDMj-ezMYntmnXYadCsMpOfKkS6SZZzgOeOx5E-ilLZSKSLHN4HkXST0Xk5K5QKQty7jKVScmlkn6icsH5I7JXlIV6QmggnTRB9UMpuPCElBlzFc9lyoM0k25kEa95s3FqtOyxpcoyxpgWLBK3FonRIrEbxmgRixy1132r1Vz-eMVBY7jYfPnrmOGKYcTCUFjkZXsY_DIm22Shyg2cgyNfJHzHtcjj2s7tLSFM9zzuORYJthDQnoCa79tHissLrf0OAQTqLz39_WM9I3e67_GA7FWrjXoOk-cqeaEx_QtVIMlS
linkProvider Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sensory+Representations+Supporting+Memory+Specificity%3A+Age+Effects+on+Behavioral+and+Neural+Discriminability&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Bowman%2C+Caitlin+R&rft.au=Chamberlain%2C+Jordan+D&rft.au=Dennis%2C+Nancy+A&rft.date=2019-03-20&rft.pub=Society+for+Neuroscience&rft.issn=0270-6474&rft.eissn=1529-2401&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=2265&rft_id=info:doi/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.2022-18.2019&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon