Cooperation, involuntary defection, and Polymorphic Evolutionary Games
The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor. Biol., 242: 873–879). Current work is motivated by the subsequent accumulation of empirical evidence and the advances in the methodology of e...
Saved in:
Published in | BioSystems Vol. 206; p. 104428 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Ireland
Elsevier B.V
01.08.2021
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0303-2647 1872-8324 1872-8324 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428 |
Cover
Abstract | The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor. Biol., 242: 873–879). Current work is motivated by the subsequent accumulation of empirical evidence and the advances in the methodology of evolutionary games.
In recent decades it become clear that individuals in many animal species vary consistently in their behavioral responses to specific challenges—animal personality. Moreover, these differences have a hereditary component. Finally, investigations into the effects of neuropeptides on behavior suggest that the variations in animal personalities involve polymorphisms based on non-Mendelian heritability within the neuropeptide signaling systems.
The last observation suggests that animal personalities can be productively analyzed via Polymorphic Evolutionary Games, which allow us to add genetic model(s) to standard (phenotypic) evolutionary games.
Such an analysis of reciprocal cooperation is the subject of the current paper. The results indicate that there is a marked difference between models that assume Mendelian vs. non-Mendelian inheritance of the pertinent traits. Monomorphic and polymorphic-Mendelian models predict pure-strategy (single phenotype) ESS, whereas the non-Mendelian genetic model predicts a mixed strategy ESS exhibiting all three phenotypes.
•The two principal elements of pair-wise reciprocity are participants taking turns soliciting and donating help and involuntary defection.•High cost, infrequent reciprocity is maintained by low-cost, high-frequency reciprocity.•Empathy and the cognitive mechanisms used in defector-recognition are independently evolved preconditions for reciprocal cooperation.•The results show that unconditional altruism is consistent with “ignoring” persistent defection. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor. Biol., 242: 873–879). Current work is motivated by the subsequent accumulation of empirical evidence and the advances in the methodology of evolutionary games.
In recent decades it become clear that individuals in many animal species vary consistently in their behavioral responses to specific challenges—animal personality. Moreover, these differences have a hereditary component. Finally, investigations into the effects of neuropeptides on behavior suggest that the variations in animal personalities involve polymorphisms based on non-Mendelian heritability within the neuropeptide signaling systems.
The last observation suggests that animal personalities can be productively analyzed via Polymorphic Evolutionary Games, which allow us to add genetic model(s) to standard (phenotypic) evolutionary games.
Such an analysis of reciprocal cooperation is the subject of the current paper. The results indicate that there is a marked difference between models that assume Mendelian vs. non-Mendelian inheritance of the pertinent traits. Monomorphic and polymorphic-Mendelian models predict pure-strategy (single phenotype) ESS, whereas the non-Mendelian genetic model predicts a mixed strategy ESS exhibiting all three phenotypes.
•The two principal elements of pair-wise reciprocity are participants taking turns soliciting and donating help and involuntary defection.•High cost, infrequent reciprocity is maintained by low-cost, high-frequency reciprocity.•Empathy and the cognitive mechanisms used in defector-recognition are independently evolved preconditions for reciprocal cooperation.•The results show that unconditional altruism is consistent with “ignoring” persistent defection. The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor. Biol., 242: 873-879). Current work is motivated by the subsequent accumulation of empirical evidence and the advances in the methodology of evolutionary games. In recent decades it become clear that individuals in many animal species vary consistently in their behavioral responses to specific challenges-animal personality. Moreover, these differences have a hereditary component. Finally, investigations into the effects of neuropeptides on behavior suggest that the variations in animal personalities involve polymorphisms based on non-Mendelian heritability within the neuropeptide signaling systems. The last observation suggests that animal personalities can be productively analyzed via Polymorphic Evolutionary Games, which allow us to add genetic model(s) to standard (phenotypic) evolutionary games. Such an analysis of reciprocal cooperation is the subject of the current paper. The results indicate that there is a marked difference between models that assume Mendelian vs. non-Mendelian inheritance of the pertinent traits. Monomorphic and polymorphic-Mendelian models predict pure-strategy (single phenotype) ESS, whereas the non-Mendelian genetic model predicts a mixed strategy ESS exhibiting all three phenotypes.The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor. Biol., 242: 873-879). Current work is motivated by the subsequent accumulation of empirical evidence and the advances in the methodology of evolutionary games. In recent decades it become clear that individuals in many animal species vary consistently in their behavioral responses to specific challenges-animal personality. Moreover, these differences have a hereditary component. Finally, investigations into the effects of neuropeptides on behavior suggest that the variations in animal personalities involve polymorphisms based on non-Mendelian heritability within the neuropeptide signaling systems. The last observation suggests that animal personalities can be productively analyzed via Polymorphic Evolutionary Games, which allow us to add genetic model(s) to standard (phenotypic) evolutionary games. Such an analysis of reciprocal cooperation is the subject of the current paper. The results indicate that there is a marked difference between models that assume Mendelian vs. non-Mendelian inheritance of the pertinent traits. Monomorphic and polymorphic-Mendelian models predict pure-strategy (single phenotype) ESS, whereas the non-Mendelian genetic model predicts a mixed strategy ESS exhibiting all three phenotypes. The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor. Biol., 242: 873-879). Current work is motivated by the subsequent accumulation of empirical evidence and the advances in the methodology of evolutionary games. In recent decades it become clear that individuals in many animal species vary consistently in their behavioral responses to specific challenges-animal personality. Moreover, these differences have a hereditary component. Finally, investigations into the effects of neuropeptides on behavior suggest that the variations in animal personalities involve polymorphisms based on non-Mendelian heritability within the neuropeptide signaling systems. The last observation suggests that animal personalities can be productively analyzed via Polymorphic Evolutionary Games, which allow us to add genetic model(s) to standard (phenotypic) evolutionary games. Such an analysis of reciprocal cooperation is the subject of the current paper. The results indicate that there is a marked difference between models that assume Mendelian vs. non-Mendelian inheritance of the pertinent traits. Monomorphic and polymorphic-Mendelian models predict pure-strategy (single phenotype) ESS, whereas the non-Mendelian genetic model predicts a mixed strategy ESS exhibiting all three phenotypes. |
ArticleNumber | 104428 |
Author | Fishman, Michael A. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Michael A. surname: Fishman fullname: Fishman, Michael A. email: gromgoru@zahav.net.il organization: Maapilei Egoz 36, Tel Aviv, 6743732, Israel |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33894287$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkF1LwzAUhoNMdFP_gvTSCzvz1SW9EXTMDxjohV6HpD3FjLaZSTfYvze1G4I3mpvAyXPec_JM0Kh1LSCUEDwlmMxuVlNjXdiFDpowpZiSWOacyiM0JlLQVDLKR2iMGWYpnXFxiiYhrHA8mSQn6JQxmUdcjNHD3Lk1eN1Z114ntt26etN22u-SEioohrJuy-TV1bvG-fWHLZJFT_VPPfeoGwjn6LjSdYCL_X2G3h8Wb_OndPny-Dy_W6YF57hLCRY8zw3VvMwzgaWoMBYUMwK6nFVxoUxyCYxIY7imomQMMmNkbvKM68wIdoauhty1d58bCJ1qbCigrnULbhMUzYikDBM8i-jlHt2YBkq19raJ-6rD3yNwOwCFdyF4qFRhu28Rnde2VgSrXrZaqR_ZqpetBtkxQP4KOMz4R-v90ApR1taCV6Gw0BZQWh-tq9LZv0O-ALgYn34 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_energy_2022_124000 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tpb_2022_03_002 |
Cites_doi | 10.1163/156853905774539445 10.1016/j.geb.2007.08.008 10.1007/s10539-020-09776-w 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.03.004 10.1511/1998.31.334 10.1093/molbev/msv007 10.1007/s10519-016-9822-2 10.1371/journal.pone.0138430 10.3377/004.044.0215 10.1098/rstb.2010.0124 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.10.018 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.006 10.1007/BF02409749 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.010 10.1016/0040-5809(75)90025-8 10.1186/s12864-019-6172-5 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01731.x 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.04.013 10.1177/0269881120913145 10.1073/pnas.1114573108 10.1017/S0140525X00026479 10.1098/rsos.181493 10.1038/nature03701 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.11.012 10.1016/j.tree.2017.07.003 10.1007/s00265-018-2608-1 10.1098/rstb.2015.0014 10.1111/jne.12807 10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.05.004 10.1038/s41598-018-28112-7 10.1126/science.7466396 10.1093/scan/nsq083 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.12.240 10.1016/j.jtbi.2016.03.012 10.2298/ABS181206057B 10.1086/406755 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00230 10.1538/expanim.18-0148 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02165 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2021 Elsevier B.V. Copyright © 2021 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2021 Elsevier B.V. – notice: Copyright © 2021 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic PubMed |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Biology |
EISSN | 1872-8324 |
ExternalDocumentID | 33894287 10_1016_j_biosystems_2021_104428 S0303264721000836 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M -~X .GJ .~1 0R~ 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 23N 4.4 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JN AABVA AACTN AAEDT AAEDW AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALCJ AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AATCM AATLK AAXUO ABAOU ABFNM ABGRD ABGSF ABJNI ABMAC ABUDA ABXDB ABYKQ ABZDS ACAZW ACDAQ ACGFS ACIUM ACNCT ACRLP ADBBV ADEZE ADGUI ADQTV ADUVX AEBSH AEHWI AEKER AENEX AEQOU AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AGHFR AGRDE AGUBO AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIGVJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV ALCLG ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ARUGR ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CBWCG CS3 DOVZS DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA HLV HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IHE J1W KOM LW9 M41 MHUIS MO0 MVM N9A O-L O9- OAUVE OGGZJ OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SAB SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SPC SPCBC SSA SSP SSU SSW SSZ T5K WH7 WUQ XPP ZGI ZMT ZXP ~02 ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ACVFH ADCNI AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH NPM 7X8 EFKBS |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c440t-107499b2a4d957087f0072031ead6f2875848e318bb4a27d33e5bb89b954a5b73 |
IEDL.DBID | AIKHN |
ISSN | 0303-2647 1872-8324 |
IngestDate | Fri Sep 05 05:32:18 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 06:53:24 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:08:59 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:02:59 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:45:27 EST 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | Diploid genetics Evolutionary games Animal personalities Polymorphic |
Language | English |
License | Copyright © 2021 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c440t-107499b2a4d957087f0072031ead6f2875848e318bb4a27d33e5bb89b954a5b73 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 ObjectType-Review-3 content type line 23 |
PMID | 33894287 |
PQID | 2518230106 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2518230106 pubmed_primary_33894287 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_biosystems_2021_104428 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biosystems_2021_104428 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_biosystems_2021_104428 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2021-08-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-08-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 08 year: 2021 text: 2021-08-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Ireland |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Ireland |
PublicationTitle | BioSystems |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Biosystems |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
Publisher | Elsevier B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier B.V |
References | Reuter, Frenzel, Walter, Markett, Montag (bib32) 2011; 6 Hofbauer, Sigmund (bib13) 1998 Nishina (bib27) 2019; 13 Sellis (bib34) 2011; 108 Reichert, Quinn (bib31) 2017; 32 Laine, van Oers (bib22) 2017 Purcell, Pruitt (bib30) 2019; 20 Ostner (bib29) 2018 Fishman (bib11) 2008; 63 Kaiser, Müller (bib17) 2021; 36 Ohta, Kojima (bib28) 2019; 68 Fishman (bib12) 2016; 398 Smith (bib37) 1982 Jacobs (bib15) 2009; 44 Sih (bib36) 2012; 15 Arahori (bib2) 2017; 8 Selten, Hammerstein (bib35) 1984; 7 Brask (bib6) 2019; 6 Wilson, Weiss, Humle, Morimura, Udono, Idani, Matsuzawa, Hirata, Inoue-Murayama (bib45) 2017; 47 Lang (bib23) 1993 Bašić (bib4) 2019; 71 Trumble, Jaeggi, Gurven (bib42) 2015; 370 Zhao (bib46) 2019; 103 Kosfeld (bib20) 2005; 435 Mesterton-Gibbons, Adams (bib25) 1998; 86 Robinson, Bosch, Levkowitz, Busch, Jarman, Ludwig (bib33) 2019; 31 Alexander (bib1) 1987 Karlin, Lessard (bib19) 1986; vol. 22 Fishman (bib10) 2006; 242 Neto, Antunes, Lopes, Ferreira, Rilling, Prata (bib26) 2020; 34 Karlin (bib18) 1975; 7 Inoue-Murayama (bib14) 2018; 8 Carter, Leffer (bib7) 2015; 10 Staes (bib38) 2015; 75 Teixeira (bib39) 2015; 32 Ziegler, Crockford (bib47) 2017; 91 Weissing (bib43) 1996; 34 Davies, Krebs, West (bib8) 2012 Kaburu, Newton-Fisher (bib16) 2015; 99 Tolomeo, Chiao, Lei, Chew, Ebstein (bib40) 2020; 115 Wilkinson (bib44) 2019; 73 Dingemanse, Réale (bib9) 2005; 142 Kulahci, Ghazanfar, Rubenstein (bib21) 2018; 136 Axelrod, Hamilton (bib3) 1981; 211 Trivers (bib41) 1971; 46 Laskowski, Bell (bib24) 2014; 90 Bergmüller, Schürch, Hamilton (bib5) 2010; 365 Trivers (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib41) 1971; 46 Bergmüller (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib5) 2010; 365 Wilson (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib45) 2017; 47 Alexander (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib1) 1987 Brask (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib6) 2019; 6 Kaiser (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib17) 2021; 36 Jacobs (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib15) 2009; 44 Smith (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib37) 1982 Reichert (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib31) 2017; 32 Teixeira (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib39) 2015; 32 Purcell (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib30) 2019; 20 Neto (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib26) 2020; 34 Carter (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib7) 2015; 10 Ostner (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib29) 2018 Tolomeo (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib40) 2020; 115 Inoue-Murayama (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib14) 2018; 8 Laine (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib22) 2017 Sellis (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib34) 2011; 108 Davies (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib8) 2012 Reuter (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib32) 2011; 6 Nishina (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib27) 2019; 13 Fishman (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib11) 2008; 63 Hofbauer (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib13) 1998 Weissing (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib43) 1996; 34 Axelrod (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib3) 1981; 211 Kosfeld (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib20) 2005; 435 Kulahci (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib21) 2018; 136 Ziegler (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib47) 2017; 91 Mesterton-Gibbons (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib25) 1998; 86 Robinson (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib33) 2019; 31 Karlin (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib18) 1975; 7 Laskowski (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib24) 2014; 90 Fishman (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib12) 2016; 398 Wilkinson (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib44) 2019; 73 Karlin (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib19) 1986; vol. 22 Bašić (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib4) 2019; 71 Ohta (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib28) 2019; 68 Kaburu (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib16) 2015; 99 Dingemanse (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib9) 2005; 142 Arahori (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib2) 2017; 8 Trumble (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib42) 2015; 370 Selten (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib35) 1984; 7 Lang (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib23) 1993 Staes (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib38) 2015; 75 Fishman (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib10) 2006; 242 Sih (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib36) 2012; 15 Zhao (10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib46) 2019; 103 |
References_xml | – start-page: 55 year: 2017 end-page: 72 ident: bib22 article-title: The quantitative and molecular genetics of individual differences in animal personality publication-title: Personality in Nonhuman Animals – volume: 75 start-page: 84 year: 2015 end-page: 90 ident: bib38 article-title: Chimpanzee sociability is associated with vasopressin (Avpr1a) but not oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variation publication-title: Horm. Behav. – start-page: 65 year: 1998 ident: bib13 article-title: Evolutionary Games and Population Dynamics – volume: 8 start-page: 1 year: 2018 end-page: 15 ident: bib14 article-title: Common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) personality, subjective well-being, hair cortisol level and AVPR1a, OPRM1, and DAT genotypes publication-title: Sci. Rep. – volume: vol. 22 year: 1986 ident: bib19 publication-title: Theoretical Studies on Sex Ratio Evolution – volume: 398 start-page: 130 year: 2016 end-page: 135 ident: bib12 article-title: Polymorphic evolutionary games publication-title: J. Theor. Biol. – volume: 7 start-page: 115 year: 1984 end-page: 116 ident: bib35 article-title: Gaps in Harley's argument on evolutionarily stable learning rules and in the logic of “tit for tat” publication-title: Behav. Brain Sci. – volume: 63 start-page: 77 year: 2008 end-page: 90 ident: bib11 article-title: Asymmetric evolutionary games with non-linear pure strategy payoffs publication-title: Game. Econ. Behav. – volume: 46 start-page: 35 year: 1971 end-page: 57 ident: bib41 article-title: The evolution of reciprocal altruism publication-title: Q. Rev. Biol. – volume: 103 start-page: 8 year: 2019 end-page: 13 ident: bib46 article-title: OXTR rs53576 polymorphism impacts interpersonal adaptability: dispositional forgiveness as a mediator publication-title: Psychoneuroendocrinology – volume: 7 start-page: 364 year: 1975 end-page: 398 ident: bib18 article-title: General two-locus selection models: some objectives, results and interpretations publication-title: Theor. Popul. Biol. – volume: 242 start-page: 873 year: 2006 end-page: 879 ident: bib10 article-title: Involuntary defection and the evolutionary origins of empathy publication-title: J. Theor. Biol. – year: 2012 ident: bib8 article-title: An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology – volume: 20 start-page: 1 year: 2019 end-page: 10 ident: bib30 article-title: Are personalities genetically determined? Inferences from subsocial spiders publication-title: BMC Genom. – volume: 211 start-page: 1390 year: 1981 end-page: 1396 ident: bib3 article-title: The evolution of cooperation publication-title: Science – volume: 44 start-page: 271 year: 2009 end-page: 282 ident: bib15 article-title: Animal personality and biological markets: rise of the individual publication-title: Afr. Zool. – year: 1987 ident: bib1 article-title: The Biology of Moral Systems – volume: 6 start-page: 662 year: 2011 end-page: 668 ident: bib32 article-title: Investigating the genetic basis of altruism: the role of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism publication-title: Soc. Cognit. Affect Neurosci. – volume: 13 start-page: 230 year: 2019 ident: bib27 article-title: Association of polymorphism of arginine-vasopressin receptor 1A (AVPR1a) gene with trust and reciprocity publication-title: Front. Hum. Neurosci. – volume: 32 start-page: 773 year: 2017 end-page: 785 ident: bib31 article-title: Cognition in contests: mechanisms, ecology, and evolution publication-title: Trends Ecol. Evol. – volume: 108 start-page: 20666 year: 2011 end-page: 20671 ident: bib34 article-title: Heterozygote advantage as a natural consequence of adaptation in diploids publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. – volume: 10 year: 2015 ident: bib7 article-title: Social grooming in bats: are vampire bats exceptional? publication-title: PloS One – volume: 15 start-page: 278 year: 2012 end-page: 289 ident: bib36 article-title: Ecological implications of behavioural syndromes publication-title: Ecol. Lett. – year: 1993 ident: bib23 article-title: Algebra – volume: 34 start-page: 891 year: 2020 end-page: 900 ident: bib26 article-title: Oxytocin and vasopressin modulation of prisoner's dilemma strategies publication-title: J. Psychopharmacol. – volume: 370 start-page: 20150014 year: 2015 ident: bib42 article-title: Evolving the neuroendocrine physiology of human and primate cooperation and collective action publication-title: Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. – year: 1982 ident: bib37 article-title: Evolution and the Theory of Games (Ch-2,4) – volume: 34 start-page: 533 year: 1996 end-page: 555 ident: bib43 article-title: Genetic versus phenotypic models of selection: can genetics be neglected in a long-term perspective? publication-title: J. Math. Biol. – volume: 99 start-page: 61 year: 2015 end-page: 71 ident: bib16 article-title: Egalitarian despots: hierarchy steepness, reciprocity and the grooming-trade model in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 115 start-page: 251 year: 2020 end-page: 272 ident: bib40 article-title: A novel role of CD38 and oxytocin as tandem molecular moderators of human social behavior publication-title: Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. – volume: 71 start-page: 159 year: 2019 end-page: 165 ident: bib4 article-title: The influence of rs53576 and rs2254298 oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms on plasma oxytocin levels and measures of empathy publication-title: Arch. Biol. Sci. – volume: 73 start-page: 7 year: 2019 ident: bib44 article-title: Kinship, association, and social complexity in bats publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. – volume: 47 start-page: 215 year: 2017 end-page: 226 ident: bib45 article-title: Chimpanzee personality and the arginine vasopressin receptor 1A genotype publication-title: Behav. Genet. – volume: 435 start-page: 673 year: 2005 end-page: 676 ident: bib20 article-title: Oxytocin increases trust in humans publication-title: Nature – volume: 136 start-page: 217 year: 2018 end-page: 226 ident: bib21 article-title: Consistent individual variation across interaction networks indicates social personalities in lemurs publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 36 start-page: 1 year: 2021 end-page: 25 ident: bib17 article-title: What is an animal personality? publication-title: Biol. Philos. – volume: 68 start-page: 127 year: 2019 end-page: 136 ident: bib28 article-title: Hatano rats selectively bred for high-and low-avoidance learning: an overview publication-title: Exp. Anim. – volume: 91 start-page: 107 year: 2017 end-page: 121 ident: bib47 article-title: Neuroendocrine control in social relationships in non-human primates: field based evidence publication-title: Horm. Behav. – volume: 365 start-page: 2751 year: 2010 end-page: 2764 ident: bib5 article-title: Evolutionary causes and consequences of consistent individual variation in cooperative behaviour publication-title: Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. – volume: 142 start-page: 1159 year: 2005 end-page: 1184 ident: bib9 article-title: Natural selection and animal personality publication-title: Behaviour – volume: 8 start-page: 2165 year: 2017 ident: bib2 article-title: Microsatellite polymorphisms adjacent to the oxytocin receptor gene in domestic cats: association with personality? publication-title: Front. Psychol. – start-page: 97 year: 2018 end-page: 110 ident: bib29 article-title: Primate social cognition: evidence from primate field studies publication-title: Evolution of Primate Social Cognition – volume: 31 year: 2019 ident: bib33 article-title: Social creatures: model animal systems for studying the neuroendocrine mechanisms of social behaviour publication-title: J. Neuroendocrinol. – volume: 32 start-page: 1186 year: 2015 end-page: 1196 ident: bib39 article-title: Long-term balancing selection in LAD1 maintains a missense trans-species polymorphism in humans, chimpanzees, and bonobos publication-title: Mol. Biol. Evol. – volume: 90 start-page: 287 year: 2014 end-page: 295 ident: bib24 article-title: Strong personalities, not social niches, drive individual differences in social behaviours in sticklebacks publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 86 start-page: 334 year: 1998 end-page: 341 ident: bib25 article-title: Animal contests as evolutionary games: paradoxical behavior can be understood in the context of evolutionary stable strategies. The trick is to discover which game the animal is playing publication-title: Am. Sci. – volume: 6 start-page: 181493 year: 2019 ident: bib6 article-title: Evolution of non-kin cooperation: social assortment by cooperative phenotype in guppies publication-title: R. Soc. Open Sci. – volume: 142 start-page: 1159 issue: 9–10 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib9 article-title: Natural selection and animal personality publication-title: Behaviour doi: 10.1163/156853905774539445 – volume: 63 start-page: 77 issue: 1 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib11 article-title: Asymmetric evolutionary games with non-linear pure strategy payoffs publication-title: Game. Econ. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2007.08.008 – volume: 36 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2021 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib17 article-title: What is an animal personality? publication-title: Biol. Philos. doi: 10.1007/s10539-020-09776-w – volume: 91 start-page: 107 year: 2017 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib47 article-title: Neuroendocrine control in social relationships in non-human primates: field based evidence publication-title: Horm. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.03.004 – volume: vol. 22 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib19 – volume: 86 start-page: 334 issue: 4 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib25 article-title: Animal contests as evolutionary games: paradoxical behavior can be understood in the context of evolutionary stable strategies. The trick is to discover which game the animal is playing publication-title: Am. Sci. doi: 10.1511/1998.31.334 – volume: 32 start-page: 1186 issue: 5 year: 2015 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib39 article-title: Long-term balancing selection in LAD1 maintains a missense trans-species polymorphism in humans, chimpanzees, and bonobos publication-title: Mol. Biol. Evol. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msv007 – volume: 47 start-page: 215 issue: 2 year: 2017 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib45 article-title: Chimpanzee personality and the arginine vasopressin receptor 1A genotype publication-title: Behav. Genet. doi: 10.1007/s10519-016-9822-2 – volume: 10 issue: 10 year: 2015 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib7 article-title: Social grooming in bats: are vampire bats exceptional? publication-title: PloS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138430 – volume: 44 start-page: 271 issue: 2 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib15 article-title: Animal personality and biological markets: rise of the individual publication-title: Afr. Zool. doi: 10.3377/004.044.0215 – start-page: 97 year: 2018 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib29 article-title: Primate social cognition: evidence from primate field studies – year: 1987 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib1 – volume: 365 start-page: 2751 issue: 1553 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib5 article-title: Evolutionary causes and consequences of consistent individual variation in cooperative behaviour publication-title: Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0124 – volume: 99 start-page: 61 year: 2015 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib16 article-title: Egalitarian despots: hierarchy steepness, reciprocity and the grooming-trade model in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes publication-title: Anim. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.10.018 – volume: 75 start-page: 84 year: 2015 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib38 article-title: Chimpanzee sociability is associated with vasopressin (Avpr1a) but not oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variation publication-title: Horm. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.006 – volume: 34 start-page: 533 issue: 5 year: 1996 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib43 article-title: Genetic versus phenotypic models of selection: can genetics be neglected in a long-term perspective? publication-title: J. Math. Biol. doi: 10.1007/BF02409749 – volume: 90 start-page: 287 year: 2014 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib24 article-title: Strong personalities, not social niches, drive individual differences in social behaviours in sticklebacks publication-title: Anim. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.010 – year: 1982 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib37 – volume: 7 start-page: 364 issue: 3 year: 1975 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib18 article-title: General two-locus selection models: some objectives, results and interpretations publication-title: Theor. Popul. Biol. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(75)90025-8 – volume: 20 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib30 article-title: Are personalities genetically determined? Inferences from subsocial spiders publication-title: BMC Genom. doi: 10.1186/s12864-019-6172-5 – volume: 15 start-page: 278 issue: 3 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib36 article-title: Ecological implications of behavioural syndromes publication-title: Ecol. Lett. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01731.x – volume: 115 start-page: 251 year: 2020 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib40 article-title: A novel role of CD38 and oxytocin as tandem molecular moderators of human social behavior publication-title: Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.04.013 – volume: 34 start-page: 891 issue: 8 year: 2020 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib26 article-title: Oxytocin and vasopressin modulation of prisoner's dilemma strategies publication-title: J. Psychopharmacol. doi: 10.1177/0269881120913145 – volume: 108 start-page: 20666 issue: 51 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib34 article-title: Heterozygote advantage as a natural consequence of adaptation in diploids publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114573108 – volume: 7 start-page: 115 issue: 1 year: 1984 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib35 article-title: Gaps in Harley's argument on evolutionarily stable learning rules and in the logic of “tit for tat” publication-title: Behav. Brain Sci. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00026479 – volume: 6 start-page: 181493 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib6 article-title: Evolution of non-kin cooperation: social assortment by cooperative phenotype in guppies publication-title: R. Soc. Open Sci. doi: 10.1098/rsos.181493 – volume: 435 start-page: 673 issue: 7042 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib20 article-title: Oxytocin increases trust in humans publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/nature03701 – year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib23 – volume: 136 start-page: 217 year: 2018 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib21 article-title: Consistent individual variation across interaction networks indicates social personalities in lemurs publication-title: Anim. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.11.012 – volume: 32 start-page: 773 issue: 10 year: 2017 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib31 article-title: Cognition in contests: mechanisms, ecology, and evolution publication-title: Trends Ecol. Evol. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2017.07.003 – volume: 73 start-page: 7 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib44 article-title: Kinship, association, and social complexity in bats publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2608-1 – volume: 370 start-page: 20150014 issue: 1683 year: 2015 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib42 article-title: Evolving the neuroendocrine physiology of human and primate cooperation and collective action publication-title: Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0014 – volume: 31 issue: 12 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib33 article-title: Social creatures: model animal systems for studying the neuroendocrine mechanisms of social behaviour publication-title: J. Neuroendocrinol. doi: 10.1111/jne.12807 – volume: 242 start-page: 873 issue: 4 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib10 article-title: Involuntary defection and the evolutionary origins of empathy publication-title: J. Theor. Biol. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.05.004 – volume: 8 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2018 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib14 article-title: Common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) personality, subjective well-being, hair cortisol level and AVPR1a, OPRM1, and DAT genotypes publication-title: Sci. Rep. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28112-7 – volume: 211 start-page: 1390 issue: 4489 year: 1981 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib3 article-title: The evolution of cooperation publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.7466396 – start-page: 65 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib13 – volume: 6 start-page: 662 issue: 5 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib32 article-title: Investigating the genetic basis of altruism: the role of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism publication-title: Soc. Cognit. Affect Neurosci. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq083 – year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib8 – volume: 103 start-page: 8 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib46 article-title: OXTR rs53576 polymorphism impacts interpersonal adaptability: dispositional forgiveness as a mediator publication-title: Psychoneuroendocrinology doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.12.240 – volume: 398 start-page: 130 year: 2016 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib12 article-title: Polymorphic evolutionary games publication-title: J. Theor. Biol. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2016.03.012 – start-page: 55 year: 2017 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib22 article-title: The quantitative and molecular genetics of individual differences in animal personality – volume: 71 start-page: 159 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib4 article-title: The influence of rs53576 and rs2254298 oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms on plasma oxytocin levels and measures of empathy publication-title: Arch. Biol. Sci. doi: 10.2298/ABS181206057B – volume: 46 start-page: 35 issue: 1 year: 1971 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib41 article-title: The evolution of reciprocal altruism publication-title: Q. Rev. Biol. doi: 10.1086/406755 – volume: 13 start-page: 230 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib27 article-title: Association of polymorphism of arginine-vasopressin receptor 1A (AVPR1a) gene with trust and reciprocity publication-title: Front. Hum. Neurosci. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00230 – volume: 68 start-page: 127 issue: 2 year: 2019 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib28 article-title: Hatano rats selectively bred for high-and low-avoidance learning: an overview publication-title: Exp. Anim. doi: 10.1538/expanim.18-0148 – volume: 8 start-page: 2165 year: 2017 ident: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428_bib2 article-title: Microsatellite polymorphisms adjacent to the oxytocin receptor gene in domestic cats: association with personality? publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02165 |
SSID | ssj0000581 |
Score | 2.296674 |
SecondaryResourceType | review_article |
Snippet | The subject of involuntary defection in the context of cycles of interaction approach to direct reciprocal cooperation was introduced some time ago (J. Theor.... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 104428 |
SubjectTerms | Animal personalities Diploid genetics Evolutionary games Polymorphic |
Title | Cooperation, involuntary defection, and Polymorphic Evolutionary Games |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104428 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33894287 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2518230106 |
Volume | 206 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1Ra9swED7ShEJfRrt1XdoueNDHealtybLoUwlNs4WWsa00b0ayFHApdihJIS_97b2z5JZBC4U-GQsJi-9Opzvp8x3AUSxZYouYhQUXJmSxZaHiTIWFZXOqbhsXTSami8t0csV-zfisA6P2XxiiVXrb72x6Y619y9CjOVyU5fAvqif6HgxDmMaRSDegFycy5V3onf6cTi6fDTJvapVSfyJ0CU_ocTQvXdYuZzLl7o4juvNkVJr95V3qNS-02Y3G2_DBu5HBqZvpDnRs9RE2XWHJ9ScYj-p6YZ1wvwdlRTaoWqq7dWCsI19hs6pM8Lu-xeAfsS6L4OzeqyH1Oyf27C5cjc_-jSahr5gQFowdL0NiV0qpY8WM5OI4E3PKDI7rFvUlnWNwhO5GRqeeWjMVC5MklmudSS1RPlyL5DN0q7qyXyCYp8IYmSmdacMwdFWRQmeBC5SgSRDzPogWobzw6cSpqsVt3vLGbvJnbHPCNnfY9iF6GrlwKTXeMOakFUL-n3rkaPnfMPpbK7ccVw9diajK1ivsxCO6acS4uA97TqBPc8LgXVJAuf-ubx_AFr05zuAhdJd3K_sV_ZilHsDGj4do4LWVntM_19NHz5vzfg |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV3dS8MwED_mRPRF_HZ-VvDRuq1NlhafZDjn1xDcwLeQNBlUpB0yBV_8271rWkVQGPiaJjT87nK56_16B3AcxCy0ScD8hAvjs8AyX3Gm_MSyMXW3DZKiEtPdoNMfsetH_liDbvUvDNEqS9vvbHphrcuRZolmc5KmzQdUT_Q9GIYwhSPRmYN5xkNBvL7Tj2-eR4sXnUppNtG5REnncSQvneauYjJV7g7alPFk1Jj99zvqLx-0uIt6K7BcOpHeudvnKtRstgYLrq3k-zr0unk-sU60J16akQXKpurl3TPWUa9wWGXGu8-fMfRHpNPEu3grlZDmXRJ3dgNGvYtht--X_RL8hLHW1CduZRzrQDETc9GKxJjqguOpRW3pjDE0Qmcjom-eWjMVCBOGlmsdxTpG6XAtwk2oZ3lmt8Ebd4QxcaR0pA3DwFW1FboKXKD8TIiIN0BUCMmkLCZOPS2eZcUae5Lf2ErCVjpsG9D-WjlxBTVmWHNWCUH-UA6Jdn-G1UeV3CSeHUqIqMzmrziJtynPiFFxA7acQL_2hKF7TOHkzr_efQiL_eHdrby9GtzswhI9cezBPahPX17tPno0U31QaOwnRo3ypg |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Cooperation%2C+involuntary+defection%2C+and+Polymorphic+Evolutionary+Games&rft.jtitle=BioSystems&rft.au=Fishman%2C+Michael+A&rft.date=2021-08-01&rft.issn=1872-8324&rft.eissn=1872-8324&rft.volume=206&rft.spage=104428&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.biosystems.2021.104428&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0303-2647&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0303-2647&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0303-2647&client=summon |