Girls’ comparative advantage in reading can largely explain the gender gap in math-related fields
Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Us...
Saved in:
Published in | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 116; no. 31; pp. 15435 - 15440 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
National Academy of Sciences
30.07.2019
|
Series | From the Cover |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0027-8424 1091-6490 1091-6490 |
DOI | 10.1073/pnas.1905779116 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Using individual-level data on 300,000 15-y-old students in 64 countries, we show that the difference between a student performance in reading and math is 80% of a standard deviation (SD) larger for girls than boys, a magnitude considered as very large. When this difference is controlled for, the gender gap in students’ intentions to pursue math-intensive studies and careers is reduced by around 75%, while gender gaps in self-concept in math, declared interest for math or attitudes toward math entirely disappear. These latter variables are also much less able to explain the gender gap in intentions to study math than is students’ difference in performance betweenmath and reading. These results are in line with choice models in which educational decisions involve intraindividual comparisons of achievement and self-beliefs in different subjects as well as cultural norms regarding gender. To directly show that intraindividual comparisons of achievement impact students’ intended careers, we use differences across schools in teaching resources dedicated to math and reading as exogenous variations of students’ comparative advantage for math. Results confirm that the comparative advantage in math with respect to reading at the time of making educational choices plays a key role in the process leading to women’s under-representation in math-intensive fields. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Using individual-level data on 300,000 15-y-old students in 64 countries, we show that the difference between a student performance in reading and math is 80% of a standard deviation (SD) larger for girls than boys, a magnitude considered as very large. When this difference is controlled for, the gender gap in students' intentions to pursue math-intensive studies and careers is reduced by around 75%, while gender gaps in self-concept in math, declared interest for math or attitudes toward math entirely disappear. These latter variables are also much less able to explain the gender gap in intentions to study math than is students' difference in performance between math and reading. These results are in line with choice models in which educational decisions involve intraindividual comparisons of achievement and self-beliefs in different subjects as well as cultural norms regarding gender. To directly show that intraindividual comparisons of achievement impact students' intended careers, we use differences across schools in teaching resources dedicated to math and reading as exogenous variations of students' comparative advantage for math. Results confirm that the comparative advantage in math with respect to reading at the time of making educational choices plays a key role in the process leading to women's underrepresentation in math-intensive fields.Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Using individual-level data on 300,000 15-y-old students in 64 countries, we show that the difference between a student performance in reading and math is 80% of a standard deviation (SD) larger for girls than boys, a magnitude considered as very large. When this difference is controlled for, the gender gap in students' intentions to pursue math-intensive studies and careers is reduced by around 75%, while gender gaps in self-concept in math, declared interest for math or attitudes toward math entirely disappear. These latter variables are also much less able to explain the gender gap in intentions to study math than is students' difference in performance between math and reading. These results are in line with choice models in which educational decisions involve intraindividual comparisons of achievement and self-beliefs in different subjects as well as cultural norms regarding gender. To directly show that intraindividual comparisons of achievement impact students' intended careers, we use differences across schools in teaching resources dedicated to math and reading as exogenous variations of students' comparative advantage for math. Results confirm that the comparative advantage in math with respect to reading at the time of making educational choices plays a key role in the process leading to women's underrepresentation in math-intensive fields. Women remain strongly underrepresented in math-related fields. This phenomenon is problematic because it contributes to gender inequalities in the labor market and can reflect a loss of talent. The current state of the art is that students’ abilities are not able to explain gender differences in educational and career choices. Relying on the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) data, we show that female students who are good at math are much more likely than male students to be even better in reading. As a consequence, the difference between 15-y-old students’ math and reading abilities, which is likely to be determined by earlier socialization processes, can explain up to 80% of the gender gap in intentions to pursue math-studies and careers. Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Using individual-level data on 300,000 15-y-old students in 64 countries, we show that the difference between a student performance in reading and math is 80% of a standard deviation (SD) larger for girls than boys, a magnitude considered as very large. When this difference is controlled for, the gender gap in students’ intentions to pursue math-intensive studies and careers is reduced by around 75%, while gender gaps in self-concept in math, declared interest for math or attitudes toward math entirely disappear. These latter variables are also much less able to explain the gender gap in intentions to study math than is students’ difference in performance between math and reading. These results are in line with choice models in which educational decisions involve intraindividual comparisons of achievement and self-beliefs in different subjects as well as cultural norms regarding gender. To directly show that intraindividual comparisons of achievement impact students’ intended careers, we use differences across schools in teaching resources dedicated to math and reading as exogenous variations of students’ comparative advantage for math. Results confirm that the comparative advantage in math with respect to reading at the time of making educational choices plays a key role in the process leading to women’s underrepresentation in math-intensive fields. Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Using individual-level data on 300,000 15-y-old students in 64 countries, we show that the difference between a student performance in reading and math is 80% of a standard deviation (SD) larger for girls than boys, a magnitude considered as very large. When this difference is controlled for, the gender gap in students' intentions to pursue math-intensive studies and careers is reduced by around 75%, while gender gaps in self-concept in math, declared interest for math or attitudes toward math entirely disappear. These latter variables are also much less able to explain the gender gap in intentions to study math than is students' difference in performance between math and reading. These results are in line with choice models in which educational decisions involve intraindividual comparisons of achievement and self-beliefs in different subjects as well as cultural norms regarding gender. To directly show that intraindividual comparisons of achievement impact students' intended careers, we use differences across schools in teaching resources dedicated to math and reading as exogenous variations of students' comparative advantage for math. Results confirm that the comparative advantage in math with respect to reading at the time of making educational choices plays a key role in the process leading to women's underrepresentation in math-intensive fields. Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in math-related fields. This latter result is however no longer true once gender differences in reading performance are also taken into account. Using individual-level data on 300,000 15-y-old students in 64 countries, we show that the difference between a student performance in reading and math is 80% of a standard deviation (SD) larger for girls than boys, a magnitude considered as very large. When this difference is controlled for, the gender gap in students’ intentions to pursue math-intensive studies and careers is reduced by around 75%, while gender gaps in self-concept in math, declared interest for math or attitudes toward math entirely disappear. These latter variables are also much less able to explain the gender gap in intentions to study math than is students’ difference in performance betweenmath and reading. These results are in line with choice models in which educational decisions involve intraindividual comparisons of achievement and self-beliefs in different subjects as well as cultural norms regarding gender. To directly show that intraindividual comparisons of achievement impact students’ intended careers, we use differences across schools in teaching resources dedicated to math and reading as exogenous variations of students’ comparative advantage for math. Results confirm that the comparative advantage in math with respect to reading at the time of making educational choices plays a key role in the process leading to women’s under-representation in math-intensive fields. |
Author | Breda, Thomas Napp, Clotilde |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Thomas surname: Breda fullname: Breda, Thomas – sequence: 2 givenname: Clotilde surname: Napp fullname: Napp, Clotilde |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31308226$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp1kUFv1DAQhS1URLeFMydQJC5c0tqOEzsXJFSVglSJC5ytWXuS9cpxgp1dtTf-Bn-vvwRH2xaoxGkO872nN_NOyFEYAxLymtEzRmV1PgVIZ6yltZQtY80zsmK0ZWUjWnpEVpRyWSrBxTE5SWlLKW1rRV-Q44pVVHHerIi5ctGnu5-_CjMOE0SY3R4LsHsIM_RYuFBEBOtCXxgIhYfYo78t8GbykHfzBoseg8VY9DAt9ADzpozoYUZbdA69TS_J8w58wlf385R8_3T57eJzef316svFx-vSiKqey7WsRdeCNDmbVVKp2thOSVZ3DbW0k6ZTramN6RgK2QlY162yds1BSGyoMNUp-XDwnXbrAa3BMEfweopugHirR3D6301wG92Pe900ikleZYP39wZx_LHDNOvBJYPeQ8BxlzTntZKCK84y-u4Juh13MeTzMtWoRlFaLYZv_070GOXh_xmoD4CJY0oRO23cnDsYl4DOa0b10rNeetZ_es668ye6B-v_K94cFNs0j_ERz1mFYpWqfgPYpbbL |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2305629120 crossref_primary_10_1080_0144929X_2022_2150565 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4749346 crossref_primary_10_3390_su142416780 crossref_primary_10_1257_pandp_20241025 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijedudev_2021_102503 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijedudev_2022_102703 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_023_39079_z crossref_primary_10_1080_0969594X_2023_2251715 crossref_primary_10_1080_13803611_2024_2426593 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2023_102302 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejed_12730 crossref_primary_10_1111_issj_12468 crossref_primary_10_1080_13645579_2023_2251321 crossref_primary_10_1177_14034948231219832 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2008704117 crossref_primary_10_1177_0890207020962326 crossref_primary_10_1080_03055698_2025_2479605 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4161554 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4811770 crossref_primary_10_1086_712330 crossref_primary_10_1186_s40594_021_00324_w crossref_primary_10_1111_mbe_12312 crossref_primary_10_1080_17461391_2021_1938692 crossref_primary_10_4000_rfp_10890 crossref_primary_10_3390_economies11020032 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0238230 crossref_primary_10_3390_educsci14020165 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_intell_2020_101467 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10672_020_09353_z crossref_primary_10_1016_j_learninstruc_2024_101906 crossref_primary_10_1111_bjdp_12423 crossref_primary_10_3389_feduc_2023_1155492 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_childyouth_2020_105650 crossref_primary_10_1007_s42330_023_00285_y crossref_primary_10_1515_cti_2021_0030 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_5167706 crossref_primary_10_3917_bupsy_pr1_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_joep_2021_102361 crossref_primary_10_1515_spp_2023_0017 crossref_primary_10_1515_zfsoz_2024_2025 crossref_primary_10_3917_bupsy_584_0041 crossref_primary_10_1080_03055698_2022_2117540 crossref_primary_10_1257_pandp_20201126 crossref_primary_10_52589_BJCE_FOB1S51H crossref_primary_10_1126_science_aba7377 crossref_primary_10_3389_feduc_2024_1389574 crossref_primary_10_1257_pandp_20221036 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_evolhumbehav_2023_01_010 crossref_primary_10_1177_01430343221149689 crossref_primary_10_1515_zfsoz_2020_0005 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jebo_2022_08_032 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00148_025_01087_2 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10672_024_09501_9 crossref_primary_10_1080_10511970_2022_2032506 crossref_primary_10_15366_jospoe2019_9_001 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4783528 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_econedurev_2023_102479 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_5048608 crossref_primary_10_1080_13803611_2021_1991810 crossref_primary_10_1080_19345747_2021_1966860 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jebo_2022_09_003 crossref_primary_10_1108_IJCED_09_2023_0084 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_5062351 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2020_06_002 crossref_primary_10_1162_qss_a_00114 crossref_primary_10_1002_jnr_25056 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_labeco_2021_101969 crossref_primary_10_1080_21528586_2024_2311399 crossref_primary_10_1186_s40536_023_00190_8 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_024_09872_3 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11218_021_09663_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psyneuen_2020_104721 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijer_2024_102440 crossref_primary_10_4000_osp_11737 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2002861117 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2013.03.008 10.1126/science.1154094 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x 10.3102/0002831211435229 10.3386/w23525 10.1177/1529100614541236 10.1177/0956797612458937 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00530 10.3386/w23769 10.1257/jel.20160995 10.1177/0956797617741719 10.1037/11706-004 10.1016/j.econedurev.2018.05.005 10.1111/0019-8676.00134 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.56 10.1126/science.1261375 10.1126/science.aar2307 10.1086/321299 10.1080/14616696.2018.1442929 10.3102/00028312023001129 10.1093/oxfordjournals.esr.a018272 10.1073/pnas.1418878112 10.1787/eag-2016-en 10.1086/209867 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2013.07.002 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02007.x 10.1126/science.aaf4372 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright National Academy of Sciences Jul 30, 2019 2019 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Jul 30, 2019 – notice: 2019 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1073/pnas.1905779116 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Immunology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Engineering Research Database AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Virology and AIDS Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Technology Research Database Nucleic Acids Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Entomology Abstracts Genetics Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Immunology Abstracts Engineering Research Database Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef Virology and AIDS Abstracts MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) Mathematics Education |
EISSN | 1091-6490 |
EndPage | 15440 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC6681723 31308226 10_1073_pnas_1905779116 26848138 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Women and Science Chair grantid: N.A. |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 0R~ 123 29P 2AX 2FS 2WC 4.4 53G 5RE 5VS 85S AACGO AAFWJ AANCE ABBHK ABOCM ABPLY ABPPZ ABTLG ABXSQ ABZEH ACGOD ACHIC ACIWK ACNCT ACPRK ADQXQ ADULT AENEX AEUPB AEXZC AFFNX AFOSN AFRAH ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AQVQM BKOMP CS3 D0L DCCCD DIK DU5 E3Z EBS EJD F5P FRP GX1 H13 HH5 HYE IPSME JAAYA JBMMH JENOY JHFFW JKQEH JLS JLXEF JPM JSG JST KQ8 L7B LU7 N9A N~3 O9- OK1 PNE PQQKQ R.V RHI RNA RNS RPM RXW SA0 SJN TAE TN5 UKR W8F WH7 WOQ WOW X7M XSW Y6R YBH YKV YSK ZCA ~02 ~KM AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF DOOOF ECM EIF NPM RHF VQA YIF YIN 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c435t-b754f9a7c308d87885cdf8715f60d0f7cf89c5ccf1e47f4ab598ddb2a47e604c3 |
ISSN | 0027-8424 1091-6490 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:36:33 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 13:15:02 EDT 2025 Mon Jun 30 08:22:19 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:31:21 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:40:06 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:25 EDT 2025 Thu May 29 13:25:30 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 31 |
Keywords | students’ achievement math-intensive fields comparative advantage gender gap |
Language | English |
License | Published under the PNAS license. |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c435t-b754f9a7c308d87885cdf8715f60d0f7cf89c5ccf1e47f4ab598ddb2a47e604c3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Author contributions: T.B. and C.N. designed research, performed research, contributed new reagents/analytic tools, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. 2T.B. and C.N. contributed equally to this work. Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved June 10, 2019 (received for review April 4, 2019) |
ORCID | 0000-0002-6356-7771 |
PMID | 31308226 |
PQID | 2268680033 |
PQPubID | 42026 |
PageCount | 6 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6681723 proquest_miscellaneous_2258742821 proquest_journals_2268680033 pubmed_primary_31308226 crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_1905779116 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1905779116 jstor_primary_26848138 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2019-07-30 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-07-30 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 07 year: 2019 text: 2019-07-30 day: 30 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: Washington |
PublicationSeriesTitle | From the Cover |
PublicationTitle | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
Publisher | National Academy of Sciences |
Publisher_xml | – name: National Academy of Sciences |
References | Black D. A. (e_1_3_3_8_2) 2008; 43 Eccles J. S. (e_1_3_3_32_2) 1983 Nagy G. (e_1_3_3_33_2) 2008 e_1_3_3_17_2 e_1_3_3_16_2 e_1_3_3_19_2 e_1_3_3_18_2 e_1_3_3_13_2 e_1_3_3_15_2 e_1_3_3_34_2 e_1_3_3_14_2 e_1_3_3_35_2 e_1_3_3_11_2 e_1_3_3_30_2 e_1_3_3_10_2 e_1_3_3_31_2 Carnevale A. P. (e_1_3_3_12_2) 2011 e_1_3_3_6_2 e_1_3_3_5_2 e_1_3_3_7_2 Xie Y. (e_1_3_3_27_2) 2003 e_1_3_3_28_2 e_1_3_3_9_2 e_1_3_3_29_2 e_1_3_3_24_2 e_1_3_3_23_2 e_1_3_3_26_2 e_1_3_3_25_2 e_1_3_3_2_2 e_1_3_3_20_2 e_1_3_3_1_2 e_1_3_3_4_2 e_1_3_3_22_2 e_1_3_3_3_2 e_1_3_3_21_2 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_3_3_26_2 doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2013.03.008 – ident: e_1_3_3_31_2 doi: 10.1126/science.1154094 – ident: e_1_3_3_19_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x – ident: e_1_3_3_6_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_20_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_22_2 doi: 10.3102/0002831211435229 – ident: e_1_3_3_1_2 doi: 10.3386/w23525 – ident: e_1_3_3_14_2 doi: 10.1177/1529100614541236 – volume: 43 start-page: 630 year: 2008 ident: e_1_3_3_8_2 article-title: Gender wage disparities among the highly educated publication-title: J. Hum. Resour. – ident: e_1_3_3_4_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_24_2 doi: 10.1177/0956797612458937 – ident: e_1_3_3_29_2 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00530 – ident: e_1_3_3_23_2 doi: 10.3386/w23769 – ident: e_1_3_3_9_2 doi: 10.1257/jel.20160995 – ident: e_1_3_3_17_2 doi: 10.1177/0956797617741719 – start-page: 115 volume-title: Gender and Occupational Outcomes: Longitudinal Assessments of Individual, Social, and Cultural Influences year: 2008 ident: e_1_3_3_33_2 doi: 10.1037/11706-004 – ident: e_1_3_3_34_2 doi: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2018.05.005 – ident: e_1_3_3_11_2 doi: 10.1111/0019-8676.00134 – ident: e_1_3_3_21_2 doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.56 – ident: e_1_3_3_2_2 doi: 10.1126/science.1261375 – ident: e_1_3_3_25_2 doi: 10.1126/science.aar2307 – ident: e_1_3_3_28_2 doi: 10.1086/321299 – ident: e_1_3_3_35_2 doi: 10.1080/14616696.2018.1442929 – ident: e_1_3_3_10_2 – volume-title: STEM: Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics year: 2011 ident: e_1_3_3_12_2 – volume-title: Women in Science: Career Processes and Outcomes year: 2003 ident: e_1_3_3_27_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_30_2 doi: 10.3102/00028312023001129 – ident: e_1_3_3_16_2 doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.esr.a018272 – ident: e_1_3_3_15_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418878112 – ident: e_1_3_3_5_2 doi: 10.1787/eag-2016-en – ident: e_1_3_3_7_2 doi: 10.1086/209867 – ident: e_1_3_3_13_2 doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2013.07.002 – ident: e_1_3_3_18_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02007.x – start-page: 75 volume-title: Achievement and Achievement Motivation year: 1983 ident: e_1_3_3_32_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_3_2 doi: 10.1126/science.aaf4372 |
SSID | ssj0009580 |
Score | 2.5667698 |
Snippet | Gender differences in math performance are now small in developed countries and they cannot explain on their own the strong underrepresentation of women in... Women remain strongly underrepresented in math-related fields. This phenomenon is problematic because it contributes to gender inequalities in the labor market... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref jstor |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 15435 |
SubjectTerms | Adolescent Attitude Career Choice Comparative advantage Developed countries Education Female Gender aspects Gender differences Girls Humans Male Mathematics Norms Policy Reading Schools Self Concept Sex differences Sex Factors Social Sciences Students Women |
Title | Girls’ comparative advantage in reading can largely explain the gender gap in math-related fields |
URI | https://www.jstor.org/stable/26848138 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31308226 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2268680033 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2258742821 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6681723 |
Volume | 116 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3dbtMwFLbKkNBuEAMGgYGMxMVQlZImTuxcThVsQlrVi03aXeTYzlYppFWTXsAVr8H78CQ8Ccc_SdOqSLCbqEqcpPH5cnwcf-c7CL0XKRdUMOnTsAh9wiPppyqGF08qSRSNhcj1RPFymlxcky838c1g8KvHWlo3-Uh835tXch-rwj6wq86S_Q_LdheFHfAb7AtbsDBs_8nG5_NVWbd0hdTxya2St1nbbzQhxySsGKa8ZngNS039Lr9paf-SO5LjraknN7zlS90aYtg73-S4QDBqGG51P4SddUNe3RIMpu0XxbNNfopzGvXQH86mvWrHKyX5DjNJf4vmS1Mob1Iumnkp7VPqPlS1TkyxGTATTTftf6YwmVHtiktP5XvvH-n75xDGTGKzqkfKumSIaPyE2KKinc-2CZoOnG4csS4YYkIrgOLGcy03FOwdLMC76QrHFa9HEBbFlKbusj3oLL8a7ERjLesT7oh2mzBgdjlJEgZxYPQAPQwpNWSB85txT_qZ2UQo92ytwBSNPu7c-xA9am-0FSZZpuy-OdAulbcXG109QY_dpAafWYQeoYGqnqKjtuPxqdM2__AMCQPZ3z9-4h5YcQdWPK-wAysGsGIHVuzAigEF2IIVA1h16z5YsQXrc3T9-dPV5MJ3dT58AaZq_JzGpEg5FfDoklHGYiELmMjHRRLIoKCiYKkAr1GMFaEF4XmcMinzkBOqkoCI6BgdVItKvUSY8kivhEOfQ-wpBE1jKqkIwPHwIA9p6KFR262ZcCL4uhZLmRkyBo0ybZJsYxIPnXYnLK3-y9-bHhs7de20kBIbR8xDJ63hMuc96gxszBKmSyl66F13GHy7XrDjlVqsdZuYURKycOyhF9bO3cVboHiIbiGga6B147ePVPM7ox_v4Prq3me-RoebV_wEHTSrtXoDsXmTvzXQ_wNKeufH |
linkProvider | Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Girls%E2%80%99+comparative+advantage+in+reading+can+largely+explain+the+gender+gap+in+math-related+fields&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Breda%2C+Thomas&rft.au=Napp%2C+Clotilde&rft.series=From+the+Cover&rft.date=2019-07-30&rft.pub=National+Academy+of+Sciences&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft.volume=116&rft.issue=31&rft.spage=15435&rft.epage=15440&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.1905779116&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F31308226&rft.externalDocID=PMC6681723 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |