Unpredictable singleton distractors in visual search can be subject to second-order suppression
Recent evidence suggests that attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant distractions can be avoided via suppression, thereby improving performance in visual search. Initial evidence suggested it is only possible to suppress salient distractors with constant and predictable features (first-order...
Saved in:
Published in | Attention, perception & psychophysics Vol. 87; no. 3; pp. 832 - 847 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
New York
Springer US
01.04.2025
Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Recent evidence suggests that attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant distractions can be avoided via suppression, thereby improving performance in visual search. Initial evidence suggested it is only possible to suppress salient distractors with constant and predictable features (first-order suppression). We show that previous failures to find evidence for second-order suppression of unpredictable feature singletons may have been due to low feature variability: If it is probable that the salient distractor colour is the target colour on another trial, suppressing this item might hinder performance. We first validated a new multiframe letter-probe paradigm, where observers counted the search displays with a target shape and always reported as many letter probes as possible from the final display. When target and singleton colours were constant (Experiment 1), a singleton suppression effect was observed, with probe letters at the singleton distractor location reported less frequently than those at non-singleton distractor locations. When two randomly swapped target/singleton colours were employed (Experiment 2), no suppression effect was observed, replicating previous findings. Critically, when target-colour items and the singleton could have one of eight different random colours (Experiment 3), a robust suppression effect reappeared. These observations demonstrate that first-order suppression is not universal, and that second-order suppression can be applied to singleton distractors under some circumstances. Suppression effects were observed for displays with and without targets, suggesting that they are not a product of direct target-singleton competition. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Recent evidence suggests that attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant distractions can be avoided via suppression, thereby improving performance in visual search. Initial evidence suggested it is only possible to suppress salient distractors with constant and predictable features (first-order suppression). We show that previous failures to find evidence for second-order suppression of unpredictable feature singletons may have been due to low feature variability: If it is probable that the salient distractor colour is the target colour on another trial, suppressing this item might hinder performance. We first validated a new multiframe letter-probe paradigm, where observers counted the search displays with a target shape and always reported as many letter probes as possible from the final display. When target and singleton colours were constant (Experiment 1), a singleton suppression effect was observed, with probe letters at the singleton distractor location reported less frequently than those at non-singleton distractor locations. When two randomly swapped target/singleton colours were employed (Experiment 2), no suppression effect was observed, replicating previous findings. Critically, when target-colour items and the singleton could have one of eight different random colours (Experiment 3), a robust suppression effect reappeared. These observations demonstrate that first-order suppression is not universal, and that second-order suppression can be applied to singleton distractors under some circumstances. Suppression effects were observed for displays with and without targets, suggesting that they are not a product of direct target-singleton competition.Recent evidence suggests that attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant distractions can be avoided via suppression, thereby improving performance in visual search. Initial evidence suggested it is only possible to suppress salient distractors with constant and predictable features (first-order suppression). We show that previous failures to find evidence for second-order suppression of unpredictable feature singletons may have been due to low feature variability: If it is probable that the salient distractor colour is the target colour on another trial, suppressing this item might hinder performance. We first validated a new multiframe letter-probe paradigm, where observers counted the search displays with a target shape and always reported as many letter probes as possible from the final display. When target and singleton colours were constant (Experiment 1), a singleton suppression effect was observed, with probe letters at the singleton distractor location reported less frequently than those at non-singleton distractor locations. When two randomly swapped target/singleton colours were employed (Experiment 2), no suppression effect was observed, replicating previous findings. Critically, when target-colour items and the singleton could have one of eight different random colours (Experiment 3), a robust suppression effect reappeared. These observations demonstrate that first-order suppression is not universal, and that second-order suppression can be applied to singleton distractors under some circumstances. Suppression effects were observed for displays with and without targets, suggesting that they are not a product of direct target-singleton competition. Recent evidence suggests that attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant distractions can be avoided via suppression, thereby improving performance in visual search. Initial evidence suggested it is only possible to suppress salient distractors with constant and predictable features (first-order suppression). We show that previous failures to find evidence for second-order suppression of unpredictable feature singletons may have been due to low feature variability: If it is probable that the salient distractor colour is the target colour on another trial, suppressing this item might hinder performance. We first validated a new multiframe letter-probe paradigm, where observers counted the search displays with a target shape and always reported as many letter probes as possible from the final display. When target and singleton colours were constant (Experiment 1), a singleton suppression effect was observed, with probe letters at the singleton distractor location reported less frequently than those at non-singleton distractor locations. When two randomly swapped target/singleton colours were employed (Experiment 2), no suppression effect was observed, replicating previous findings. Critically, when target-colour items and the singleton could have one of eight different random colours (Experiment 3), a robust suppression effect reappeared. These observations demonstrate that first-order suppression is not universal, and that second-order suppression can be applied to singleton distractors under some circumstances. Suppression effects were observed for displays with and without targets, suggesting that they are not a product of direct target-singleton competition. |
Author | Drisdelle, Brandi Lee Eimer, Martin Zivony, Alon |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Brandi Lee surname: Drisdelle fullname: Drisdelle, Brandi Lee email: brandi.drisdelle@bristol.ac.uk organization: School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Beacon House, School of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London – sequence: 2 givenname: Alon surname: Zivony fullname: Zivony, Alon organization: School of Psychology, University of Sheffield, School of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London – sequence: 3 givenname: Martin surname: Eimer fullname: Eimer, Martin organization: School of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/40011337$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kU9P3DAQxa0KVP60X4BDFakXLike21knp6pCQCshcSlSb5ZjTxavsvbWkyDx7TEs3dIeONny_ObNG78jthdTRMZOgH-RumnPCKQCVXPR1Fxy0dbyHTuETsladvLX3u4u4IAdEa04X8iF5u_ZgeIcQEp9yMxt3GT0wU22H7GiEJcjTilWPtCUrZtSpirE6j7QbMeK0GZ3Vzkbq77Qc79CN1VTKgWXoq9T9pjL-6aIEoUUP7D9wY6EH1_OY3Z7efHz_Ht9fXP14_zbde2UWEw1Ns3Qc-mFbgGGYrJvWi-5dY0WA_Zeu2JeALRoXQ_KC4XayWFwyqFydpDH7OtWdzP3a_QOY3E_mk0Oa5sfTLLB_FuJ4c4s070B6BaNaHRROH1RyOn3jDSZdSCH42gjppmMBA2tbnknC_r5P3SV5hzLfoXqeMc1KFGoT68t7bz8-fwCiC3gciLKOOwQ4OYpYbNN2JSEzXPC5mm23DZRgeMS89_Zb3Q9AtTvqt4 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/xge0000970 10.1080/13506285.2019.1583298 10.1073/pnas.1523471113 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4161-13.2014 10.1111/psyp.13878 10.3758/s13414-023-02712-6 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.01.010 10.3758/BF03208805 10.1162/jocn_a_01827 10.3758/s13423-012-0280-4 10.3758/APP.72.6.1455 10.1037/xlm0001194 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102269 10.1037/xhp0001002 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 10.3758/BF03211619 10.1037/xhp0001067 10.1037/xge0001398 10.1037/xhp0000484 10.1177/0956797619878813 10.1080/13506285.2019.1600090 10.1037/xhp0001135.supp 10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.001 10.1037/xge0001350 10.1162/jocn_a_01901 10.3758/s13414-017-1477-4 10.1007/s42113-022-00160-3 10.1177/0956797615597913 10.1080/13506285.2021.1925798 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.011 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.03.004 10.1037/amp0000917 10.1080/13506285.2011.603709 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.847 10.3758/s13423-017-1320-x 10.3758/s13414-018-1562-3 10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1 10.3758/s13423-016-1065-y 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.023 10.1037/xhp0000594 10.3758/BF03211656 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.027 10.1037/xhp0000084 10.1080/13506285.2016.1145159 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105291 10.1037/a0026578 10.1177/0956797619886809 10.1080/13506285.2020.1848949 10.1037/xhp0000521 10.1037/xhp0000796 10.1162/jocn_a_01279 10.1162/jocn_a_01986 10.3758/BF03193146 10.3758/s13414-022-02524-0 10.1093/cercor/bhab341 10.1111/psyp.12675 10.1111/psyp.12606 10.1037/xhp0000221 10.1037/xhp0000948 10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8 10.1037/a0027885 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s) 2025 2025. The Author(s). Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Apr 2025 The Author(s) 2025 2025 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s) 2025 – notice: 2025. The Author(s). – notice: Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Apr 2025 – notice: The Author(s) 2025 2025 |
DBID | C6C AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 4T- 4U- K9. 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.3758/s13414-025-03028-3 |
DatabaseName | Springer Nature OA Free Journals CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Docstoc University Readers ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) University Readers Docstoc MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: C6C name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals url: http://www.springeropen.com/ sourceTypes: Publisher – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1943-393X |
EndPage | 847 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC11965257 40011337 10_3758_s13414_025_03028_3 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -~X 0-V 06D 0R~ 0VY 186 199 1N0 203 23N 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2VQ 30V 4.4 406 408 40E 53G 6J9 7X7 875 88E 8AO 8FI 8FJ 8G5 8TC 8UJ 95. 96X AAAVM AABHQ AACDK AAFWJ AAGAY AAHNG AAIAL AAJBT AAJKR AANZL AAPKM AARHV AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYQN AAYTO AAYZH AAZMS ABAKF ABDBE ABDZT ABECU ABFTV ABHLI ABIVO ABJOX ABJUD ABKCH ABLLD ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABPPZ ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABULA ABUWG ABXPI ACAOD ACBXY ACDTI ACGFS ACHSB ACHXU ACKIV ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACOKC ACPIV ACZOJ ADBBV ADHIR ADHKG ADKNI ADKPE ADRFC ADTPH ADURQ ADYFF ADZKW AEBTG AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEKMD AEMSY AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETCA AETEA AEVLU AEXYK AFDYV AFKRA AFLOW AFQWF AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGJBK AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHKAY AHMBA AHPBZ AHSBF AHYZX AI. AIAKS AIGIU AIIXL AILAN AITGF AJBLW AJRNO AJZVZ ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AN0 AOCGG ARALO ARMRJ ASPBG AVWKF AXYYD AYFIA AYQZM AZFZN AZQEC BAWUL BENPR BGNMA BNQBC BPHCQ BVXVI C1A C6C CCPQU CJNVE CSCUP D0L DDRTE DIK DNIVK DPUIP DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMOBN ESBYG F5P FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FYUFA GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ7 GUQSH H13 HMCUK HMJXF HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ IKXTQ IRVIT ITM IWAJR J-C JBSCW JZLTJ KOV LLZTM M0P M1P M2M M2O M2R M4Y MQGED N2Q N9A NB0 NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O9- O93 O9G O9I O9J OHT P9L PF- PHGZT PQEDU PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ PT4 R9I ROL RPV RSV S16 S1Z S27 S3B SBS SBU SCLPG SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 TN5 TSG TUC TUS U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UPT UTJUX UZXMN VC2 VFIZW VH1 W48 WH7 WK8 XJT XKC ZMTXR ZOVNA AAYXX ABBRH ADXHL AFDZB AFOHR AGQPQ ATHPR CITATION PHGZM CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM UMC 4T- 4U- ABRTQ K9. 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-e55fb03d27811f670b58d30ac572febd7c0062118eacb14d24e7c3ffc4ce4caf3 |
IEDL.DBID | U2A |
ISSN | 1943-3921 1943-393X |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:37:50 EDT 2025 Tue Aug 05 10:45:22 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 20:55:23 EDT 2025 Sun May 11 01:40:31 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 05:15:06 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 03:05:12 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 3 |
Keywords | Attentional suppression Visual search Letter-probe paradigm Second-order suppression Attentional capture Visual attention |
Language | English |
License | 2025. The Author(s). Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c426t-e55fb03d27811f670b58d30ac572febd7c0062118eacb14d24e7c3ffc4ce4caf3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-025-03028-3 |
PMID | 40011337 |
PQID | 3190907142 |
PQPubID | 976350 |
PageCount | 16 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11965257 proquest_miscellaneous_3171878093 proquest_journals_3190907142 pubmed_primary_40011337 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_025_03028_3 springer_journals_10_3758_s13414_025_03028_3 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2025-04-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2025-04-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 04 year: 2025 text: 2025-04-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | New York |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: New York – name: United States – name: Austin |
PublicationTitle | Attention, perception & psychophysics |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Atten Percept Psychophys |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Atten Percept Psychophys |
PublicationYear | 2025 |
Publisher | Springer US Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer US – name: Springer Nature B.V |
References | N Gaspelin (3028_CR21) 2015; 26 CL Folk (3028_CR17) 1992; 18 A Grubert (3028_CR26) 2016; 42 C Huang (3028_CR27) 2022; 48 BY Won (3028_CR61) 2018; 44 AB Leber (3028_CR31) 2016; 23 J Theeuwes (3028_CR48) 1991; 49 S Aubin (3028_CR4) 2016; 53 J Theeuwes (3028_CR50) 2022; 26 J Theeuwes (3028_CR49) 1992; 51 DB Vatterott (3028_CR56) 2012; 19 HR Liesefeld (3028_CR34) 2021; 150 3028_CR52 BL Drisdelle (3028_CR12) 2018; 127 BT Stilwell (3028_CR45) 2022; 34 X Ma (3028_CR37) 2023; 85 BL Drisdelle (3028_CR11) 2023; 152 J Jonides (3028_CR29) 1988; 43 R Sawaki (3028_CR43) 2010; 72 N Gaspelin (3028_CR22) 2017; 79 OJ Adams (3028_CR1) 2023; 85 BT Stilwell (3028_CR46) 2021; 47 B Wang (3028_CR57) 2018; 80 J Tam (3028_CR47) 2022; 34 M Maheux (3028_CR38) 2017; 125 BL Drisdelle (3028_CR10) 2021; 58 D van Moorselaar (3028_CR53) 2021; 137 D van Moorselaar (3028_CR54) 2023; 35 M Oxner (3028_CR41) 2023; 152 JM Gaspar (3028_CR18) 2016; 113 CL Folk (3028_CR16) 2015; 41 J Moher (3028_CR39) 2020; 31 JT Arita (3028_CR3) 2012; 38 N Gaspelin (3028_CR24) 2018; 44 CL Folk (3028_CR15) 1998; 24 JD Cosman (3028_CR7) 2018; 28 3028_CR40 HR Liesefeld (3028_CR32) 2021; 29 M Sauter (3028_CR42) 2018; 80 SJ Luck (3028_CR35) 2021; 29 O Ferrante (3028_CR14) 2018; 102 DB Vatterott (3028_CR55) 2018; 80 M De Tommaso (3028_CR8) 2019; 27 M Turatto (3028_CR51) 2018; 25 S Chang (3028_CR6) 2019; 30 3028_CR30 BA Anderson (3028_CR2) 2021; 76 BL Drisdelle (3028_CR9) 2017; 54 BY Won (3028_CR62) 2019; 45 HR Liesefeld (3028_CR33) 2022; 32 B Wang (3028_CR58) 2020; 46 R Sawaki (3028_CR44) 2011; 19 F Faul (3028_CR13) 2007; 39 BE Wirth (3028_CR60) 2023; 49 EC Blakley (3028_CR5) 2022; 214 T Graves (3028_CR25) 2015; 23 JL Irons (3028_CR28) 2012; 38 N Gaspelin (3028_CR23) 2018; 30 N Gaspelin (3028_CR20) 2019; 27 3028_CR63 JM Gaspar (3028_CR19) 2014; 34 X Ma (3028_CR36) 2023; 49 |
References_xml | – volume: 150 start-page: 710 issue: 4 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR34 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/xge0000970 – volume: 27 start-page: 214 issue: 3–4 year: 2019 ident: 3028_CR8 publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2019.1583298 – volume: 113 start-page: 3693 issue: 13 year: 2016 ident: 3028_CR18 publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America doi: 10.1073/pnas.1523471113 – volume: 34 start-page: 5658 issue: 16 year: 2014 ident: 3028_CR19 publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4161-13.2014 – volume: 58 issue: 9 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR10 publication-title: Psychophysiology doi: 10.1111/psyp.13878 – volume: 85 start-page: 1409 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR37 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-023-02712-6 – volume: 137 start-page: 232 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR53 publication-title: Cortex doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.01.010 – volume: 43 start-page: 346 issue: 4 year: 1988 ident: 3028_CR29 publication-title: Perception & : Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03208805 – volume: 34 start-page: 787 issue: 5 year: 2022 ident: 3028_CR45 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01827 – volume: 19 start-page: 871 issue: 5 year: 2012 ident: 3028_CR56 publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0280-4 – volume: 72 start-page: 1455 issue: 6 year: 2010 ident: 3028_CR43 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/APP.72.6.1455 – ident: 3028_CR30 doi: 10.1037/xlm0001194 – ident: 3028_CR63 doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102269 – volume: 48 start-page: 443 issue: 5 year: 2022 ident: 3028_CR27 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0001002 – volume: 18 start-page: 1030 issue: 4 year: 1992 ident: 3028_CR17 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 – volume: 49 start-page: 83 issue: 1 year: 1991 ident: 3028_CR48 publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211619 – volume: 49 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR36 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0001067 – volume: 152 start-page: 2504 issue: 9 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR11 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi: 10.1037/xge0001398 – volume: 44 start-page: 626 issue: 4 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR24 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0000484 – volume: 30 start-page: 1724 issue: 12 year: 2019 ident: 3028_CR6 publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797619878813 – volume: 27 start-page: 227 issue: 3–4 year: 2019 ident: 3028_CR20 publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2019.1600090 – volume: 49 start-page: 1145 issue: 8 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR60 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0001135.supp – volume: 26 start-page: 860 issue: 10 year: 2022 ident: 3028_CR50 publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.001 – volume: 152 start-page: 1705 issue: 6 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR41 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/xge0001350 – volume: 34 start-page: 2100 issue: 11 year: 2022 ident: 3028_CR47 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01901 – volume: 80 start-page: 622 issue: 3 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR42 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1477-4 – ident: 3028_CR52 doi: 10.1007/s42113-022-00160-3 – volume: 26 start-page: 1740 issue: 11 year: 2015 ident: 3028_CR21 publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797615597913 – volume: 29 start-page: 600 issue: 9 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR32 publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2021.1925798 – ident: 3028_CR40 – volume: 125 start-page: 28 year: 2017 ident: 3028_CR38 publication-title: Biological Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.011 – volume: 127 start-page: 52 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR12 publication-title: International Journal of Psychophysiology doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.03.004 – volume: 76 start-page: 1410 issue: 9 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR2 publication-title: American Psychologist doi: 10.1037/amp0000917 – volume: 19 start-page: 956 issue: 7 year: 2011 ident: 3028_CR44 publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2011.603709 – volume: 24 start-page: 847 issue: 3 year: 1998 ident: 3028_CR15 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.847 – volume: 25 start-page: 1028 issue: 3 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR51 publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1320-x – volume: 80 start-page: 1763 issue: 7 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR57 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1562-3 – volume: 79 start-page: 45 issue: 1 year: 2017 ident: 3028_CR22 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1 – volume: 23 start-page: 1873 issue: 6 year: 2016 ident: 3028_CR31 publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1065-y – volume: 28 start-page: 414 issue: 3 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR7 publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.023 – volume: 45 start-page: 125 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: 3028_CR62 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0000594 – volume: 51 start-page: 599 issue: 6 year: 1992 ident: 3028_CR49 publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211656 – volume: 102 start-page: 67 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR14 publication-title: Cortex doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.027 – volume: 41 start-page: 1153 issue: 4 year: 2015 ident: 3028_CR16 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0000084 – volume: 23 start-page: 1098 issue: 9–10 year: 2015 ident: 3028_CR25 publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2016.1145159 – volume: 214 year: 2022 ident: 3028_CR5 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105291 – volume: 38 start-page: 758 issue: 3 year: 2012 ident: 3028_CR28 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0026578 – volume: 31 start-page: 31 issue: 1 year: 2020 ident: 3028_CR39 publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797619886809 – volume: 29 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR35 publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2020.1848949 – volume: 44 start-page: 1128 issue: 7 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR61 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0000521 – volume: 46 start-page: 1051 issue: 10 year: 2020 ident: 3028_CR58 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000796 – volume: 30 start-page: 1265 issue: 9 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR23 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01279 – volume: 35 start-page: 1032 issue: 6 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR54 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01986 – volume: 39 start-page: 175 issue: 2 year: 2007 ident: 3028_CR13 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 – volume: 85 start-page: 613 issue: 3 year: 2023 ident: 3028_CR1 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-022-02524-0 – volume: 32 start-page: 2398 issue: 11 year: 2022 ident: 3028_CR33 publication-title: Cerebral Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab341 – volume: 54 start-page: 83 issue: 1 year: 2017 ident: 3028_CR9 publication-title: Psychophysiology doi: 10.1111/psyp.12675 – volume: 53 start-page: 611 issue: 5 year: 2016 ident: 3028_CR4 publication-title: Psychophysiology doi: 10.1111/psyp.12606 – volume: 42 start-page: 1215 issue: 8 year: 2016 ident: 3028_CR26 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0000221 – volume: 47 start-page: 1313 issue: 10 year: 2021 ident: 3028_CR46 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/xhp0000948 – volume: 80 start-page: 485 issue: 2 year: 2018 ident: 3028_CR55 publication-title: Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8 – volume: 38 start-page: 580 issue: 3 year: 2012 ident: 3028_CR3 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0027885 |
SSID | ssj0063670 |
Score | 2.443603 |
Snippet | Recent evidence suggests that attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant distractions can be avoided via suppression, thereby improving performance in... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Publisher |
StartPage | 832 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Attention Behavioral Science and Psychology Cognitive Psychology Color Perception Evidence Experiments Female Humans Inhibition, Psychological Male Orientation Pattern Recognition, Visual Psychology Reaction Time Young Adult |
Title | Unpredictable singleton distractors in visual search can be subject to second-order suppression |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13414-025-03028-3 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/40011337 https://www.proquest.com/docview/3190907142 https://www.proquest.com/docview/3171878093 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC11965257 |
Volume | 87 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LT9wwEB4VuHBBpS2wha5ciRu1yNrO67jaQmlRObESnKw4tsVevCs2i8S_Z8ZJtloQh15yiEdOMjOZmc_jGQOcekOwQZQ8kS7nSvmSV6nL-KgSTtW5LRJLQPHvTXY1VX_u0ruuKGzZ73bvU5LRUhOuxKD2fEmtxxSn41dRMUXB5RbspITdUYunYtzb34xaksVcspIcvf-oLZV5Z45Nd_Qmxny7VfJVvjS6ocuPsNfFj2zcCnwfPrjwCXbXZuz5M-hpWDxS-qWhqihGawEom3lgNvbIjcfrsFlgT7PlCmdqVZ0hh5lB6pWhhRnWzHEAobLlsTcn3l90O2bDF5heXtxOrnh3jAKv0f023KWpN4m0gopKPTLHpIWVSVWnufDO2LymQkoEGmiDzUhZoVxeS-9rVaO4Ki8PYDvMgzsCZqQpM5RunXmLkVdRZYg4pPFFlphKynwAZz039aLtlqERZRDvdct7jbzXkfdaDuCkZ7ju_pylRpOQlFRVJQbwfT2MOk-JjCq4-Ypo0KPmRVLiFIetfNaPUxTkxlcpNiS3JqB-2psjYfYQ-2qPqLsimrAB_OiF_O-93v-Mr_9Hfgy7Iiog7f45ge3mceW-YWDTmCHsjH_dX18MYWuSTfD68_f1MOr2C6du9QY |
linkProvider | Springer Nature |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LT-MwEB6xcIAL4r1dXkbiBhZp7LyOqKIqUDhRiZsVx7boxa1ouhL_fmectLsFcdhrbDnOzGQenpnPAJdOU9gQFzwSNuNSuoKXiU15t4ytrDKTR4YCxafndDCSD6_JawuTQ70w_-TvBbqyNzMCHJOcLl1FcYxzLn7AhsRImcr3emlvoXVTAiILGWQpONr8btMg880aq0boi2f5tUDyU5Y0GJ_-Dmy3XiO7bdi8C2vW78HWUnl97IMa-ek7JV1q6oVidAKAHJl4ZgIybrhUh409-z2ezXGlRsAZ0pVpnD3XdBzD6gkOYIBseEDkxOfTtk7WH8Cof_fSG_D28gReodGtuU0SpyNhYmoldUgcneRGRGWVZLGz2mQVtU9ieIGaV3eliaXNKuFcJStkUunEIaz7ibc_gWmhixR5WqXOoL-VlynGGUK7PI10KUTWgasFNdW0wchQGFsQ7VVDe4W0V4H2SnTgZEFw1f4vM4WKICqolyruwMVyGCWd0helt5M5zUE7muVRgUscNfxZvk6Saxu2kq9wbjmBULRXR_z4LaBpdwlTERVXB64XTP67r-8_49f_TT-HzcHL01AN758fj2ErDsJI9T8nsF6_z-0puja1Pgsy_Qcz2PBu |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LT-MwEB7xkBAXtOzyKK_1StzAIo2d1xEVKt7iQCVuVhzbohe3oinS_vudcZouBXHgGltO8s14POPxfAY4dprChrjgkbAZl9IVvExsyrtlbGWVmTwyFCjeP6RXA3nznDy_q-IPp93blGRT00AsTb4-GxtHU1ygg3s2IRoyyekqVlTSOOdiGVYxUgmJ2l7aa21xSvRkIa8sBUdPoNuUzXwxxuLS9Mnf_Hxs8kPuNCxJ_R-wMfMl2Xkj_E1Ysv4nrM9N2t9foAZ-_EqpmJoqpBjtC6CcRp6ZwJcbrtphQ8_ehpMpjtSoPUO0mcbeU02bNKweYQPiY3jg6cTn49npWb8Fg_7lU--Kz65U4BUuxTW3SeJ0JExMBaYOwdFJbkRUVkkWO6tNVlFRJQYdaI91V5pY2qwSzlWyQtGVTmzDih95uwtMC12kKOkqdQa9sLxMMfoQ2uVppEshsg6ctGiqccOcoTDiIOxVg71C7FXAXokOHLSAq9ksmig0D1FBFVZxB_7Mm1H_KalRejuaUh9cXbM8KnCInUY-89dJcnjDp-QLkpt3IG7txRY_fAkc211iWkRz1oHTVsj_v-vr39j7XvffsPZ40Vd31w-3-7AeB12kQ0EHsFK_Tu0h-ju1Pgoq_Q_4jPi1 |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Unpredictable+singleton+distractors+in+visual+search+can+be+subject+to+second%E2%80%91order+suppression&rft.jtitle=Attention%2C+perception+%26+psychophysics&rft.au=Drisdelle%2C+Brandi+Lee&rft.au=Zivony%2C+Alon&rft.au=Eimer%2C+Martin&rft.date=2025-04-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.issn=1943-3921&rft.eissn=1943-393X&rft.volume=87&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=832&rft.epage=847&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13414-025-03028-3&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1943-3921&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1943-3921&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1943-3921&client=summon |