A spatial version of the Stroop task for examining proactive and reactive control independently from non-conflict processes

Conflict-induced control refers to humans’ ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word Stroop task) based on experience with conflict created by distracting information (e.g., an incongruent color word), and to do so either in a...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inAttention, perception & psychophysics Vol. 86; no. 4; pp. 1259 - 1286
Main Authors Spinelli, Giacomo, Lupker, Stephen J.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published New York Springer US 01.05.2024
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Conflict-induced control refers to humans’ ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word Stroop task) based on experience with conflict created by distracting information (e.g., an incongruent color word), and to do so either in a proactive (preparatory) or a reactive (stimulus-driven) fashion. Interest in conflict-induced control has grown recently, as has the awareness that effects attributed to those processes might be affected by conflict-unrelated processes (e.g., the learning of stimulus-response associations). This awareness has resulted in the recommendation to move away from traditional interference paradigms with small stimulus/response sets and towards paradigms with larger sets (at least four targets, distractors, and responses), paradigms that allow better control of non-conflict processes. Using larger sets, however, is not always feasible. Doing so in the Stroop task, for example, would require either multiple arbitrary responses that are difficult for participants to learn (e.g., manual responses to colors) or non-arbitrary responses that can be difficult for researchers to collect (e.g., vocal responses in online experiments). Here, we present a spatial version of the Stroop task that solves many of those problems. In this task, participants respond to one of six directions indicated by an arrow, each requiring a specific, non-arbitrary manual response, while ignoring the location where the arrow is displayed. We illustrate the usefulness of this task by showing the results of two experiments in which evidence for proactive and reactive control was obtained while controlling for the impact of non-conflict processes.
AbstractList Conflict-induced control refers to humans' ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word Stroop task) based on experience with conflict created by distracting information (e.g., an incongruent color word), and to do so either in a proactive (preparatory) or a reactive (stimulus-driven) fashion. Interest in conflict-induced control has grown recently, as has the awareness that effects attributed to those processes might be affected by conflict-unrelated processes (e.g., the learning of stimulus-response associations). This awareness has resulted in the recommendation to move away from traditional interference paradigms with small stimulus/response sets and towards paradigms with larger sets (at least four targets, distractors, and responses), paradigms that allow better control of non-conflict processes. Using larger sets, however, is not always feasible. Doing so in the Stroop task, for example, would require either multiple arbitrary responses that are difficult for participants to learn (e.g., manual responses to colors) or non-arbitrary responses that can be difficult for researchers to collect (e.g., vocal responses in online experiments). Here, we present a spatial version of the Stroop task that solves many of those problems. In this task, participants respond to one of six directions indicated by an arrow, each requiring a specific, non-arbitrary manual response, while ignoring the location where the arrow is displayed. We illustrate the usefulness of this task by showing the results of two experiments in which evidence for proactive and reactive control was obtained while controlling for the impact of non-conflict processes.
Abstract Conflict-induced control refers to humans’ ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word Stroop task) based on experience with conflict created by distracting information (e.g., an incongruent color word), and to do so either in a proactive (preparatory) or a reactive (stimulus-driven) fashion. Interest in conflict-induced control has grown recently, as has the awareness that effects attributed to those processes might be affected by conflict-unrelated processes (e.g., the learning of stimulus-response associations). This awareness has resulted in the recommendation to move away from traditional interference paradigms with small stimulus/response sets and towards paradigms with larger sets (at least four targets, distractors, and responses), paradigms that allow better control of non-conflict processes. Using larger sets, however, is not always feasible. Doing so in the Stroop task, for example, would require either multiple arbitrary responses that are difficult for participants to learn (e.g., manual responses to colors) or non-arbitrary responses that can be difficult for researchers to collect (e.g., vocal responses in online experiments). Here, we present a spatial version of the Stroop task that solves many of those problems. In this task, participants respond to one of six directions indicated by an arrow, each requiring a specific, non-arbitrary manual response, while ignoring the location where the arrow is displayed. We illustrate the usefulness of this task by showing the results of two experiments in which evidence for proactive and reactive control was obtained while controlling for the impact of non-conflict processes.
Conflict-induced control refers to humans' ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word Stroop task) based on experience with conflict created by distracting information (e.g., an incongruent color word), and to do so either in a proactive (preparatory) or a reactive (stimulus-driven) fashion. Interest in conflict-induced control has grown recently, as has the awareness that effects attributed to those processes might be affected by conflict-unrelated processes (e.g., the learning of stimulus-response associations). This awareness has resulted in the recommendation to move away from traditional interference paradigms with small stimulus/response sets and towards paradigms with larger sets (at least four targets, distractors, and responses), paradigms that allow better control of non-conflict processes. Using larger sets, however, is not always feasible. Doing so in the Stroop task, for example, would require either multiple arbitrary responses that are difficult for participants to learn (e.g., manual responses to colors) or non-arbitrary responses that can be difficult for researchers to collect (e.g., vocal responses in online experiments). Here, we present a spatial version of the Stroop task that solves many of those problems. In this task, participants respond to one of six directions indicated by an arrow, each requiring a specific, non-arbitrary manual response, while ignoring the location where the arrow is displayed. We illustrate the usefulness of this task by showing the results of two experiments in which evidence for proactive and reactive control was obtained while controlling for the impact of non-conflict processes.Conflict-induced control refers to humans' ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word Stroop task) based on experience with conflict created by distracting information (e.g., an incongruent color word), and to do so either in a proactive (preparatory) or a reactive (stimulus-driven) fashion. Interest in conflict-induced control has grown recently, as has the awareness that effects attributed to those processes might be affected by conflict-unrelated processes (e.g., the learning of stimulus-response associations). This awareness has resulted in the recommendation to move away from traditional interference paradigms with small stimulus/response sets and towards paradigms with larger sets (at least four targets, distractors, and responses), paradigms that allow better control of non-conflict processes. Using larger sets, however, is not always feasible. Doing so in the Stroop task, for example, would require either multiple arbitrary responses that are difficult for participants to learn (e.g., manual responses to colors) or non-arbitrary responses that can be difficult for researchers to collect (e.g., vocal responses in online experiments). Here, we present a spatial version of the Stroop task that solves many of those problems. In this task, participants respond to one of six directions indicated by an arrow, each requiring a specific, non-arbitrary manual response, while ignoring the location where the arrow is displayed. We illustrate the usefulness of this task by showing the results of two experiments in which evidence for proactive and reactive control was obtained while controlling for the impact of non-conflict processes.
Author Lupker, Stephen J.
Spinelli, Giacomo
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Giacomo
  orcidid: 0000-0002-3913-8109
  surname: Spinelli
  fullname: Spinelli, Giacomo
  email: giacomospinelli@hotmail.it
  organization: Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Stephen J.
  surname: Lupker
  fullname: Lupker, Stephen J.
  email: lupker@uwo.ca
  organization: Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38691237$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9UU1v1DAUtFAR_YA_0AOyxKWXUH_FiU-oqqBFqtRDQeJmeZ2XrdvETm3viqp_HoddFuihB_s9682M52kO0Z4PHhA6puQjb-r2NFEuqKgIm0-rWKVeoQOqBK-44j_2dj2j--gwpTtCJJcNeYP2eSsVZbw5QE9nOE0mOzPgNcTkgsehx_kW8E2OIUw4m3SP-xAx_DSj884v8RSDsdmtARvf4Qjbhw2-UAbsfAcTlMvn4RH3MYy4GK_KuB-czTPdQkqQ3qLXvRkSvNvWI_T9y-dv55fV1fXF1_Ozq8oKJnPFSmG9EAo6wiknda0s7RjIDuqmE0aIjjZyYQVV9aIFwRaq5m1PFafMWAb8CH3a6E6rxQidLcaiGfQU3Wjiow7G6f8n3t3qZVhrSonibd0UhZOtQgwPK0hZjy5ZGAbjIaySLqYIbRpRywL98Ax6F1bRl_1mlGRcinZGsQ3KxpBShH7nhhI9h6s34eoSrv4drlaF9P7fPXaUP2kWAN8AUhn5JcS_f78g-wtHprQY
Cites_doi 10.1037/xlm0000934
10.2466/pms.1977.45.1.11
10.1037/a0023437
10.1371/journal.pone.0294957
10.1080/17470218.2016.1206130
10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.422
10.1037/xge0000933
10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.004
10.3758/s13428-021-01588-4
10.1037/xlm0000820
10.1080/17470210902752096
10.1162/jocn.2009.21365
10.1177/17470218221114769
10.3758/s13423-022-02104-1
10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
10.1002/hbm.460010305
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.019
10.3758/s13423-012-0373-0
10.3758/s13421-017-0721-4
10.1006/nimg.1997.0285
10.1037/xlm0001005
10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.08.012
10.1177/1747021820912477
10.3758/s13428-014-0458-y
10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514
10.3758/s13421-019-00980-y
10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.010
10.3758/s13421-016-0591-1
10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
10.1037/xlm0000670
10.1037/h0027448
10.3758/s13423-018-1520-z
10.3758/BF03194070
10.1073/pnas.97.4.1944
10.1037/0033-295x.114.4.1076
10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002
10.1177/17470218211032548
10.1037/a0032426
10.3758/s13428-023-02215-0
10.1037/h0054651
10.1002/hbm.20129
10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
10.1076/jcen.24.4.534.1033
10.3758/MC.36.8.1484
10.3758/BF03196526
10.3758/BF03211844
10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.01.003
10.1037/xlm0000844
10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.570
10.1162/jocn_a_01768
10.3758/BF03198265
10.3758/BF03202722
10.1080/17470218.2016.1182193
10.3758/s13423-022-02138-5
10.1177/1747021818787155
10.1371/journal.pone.0082320
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.007
10.3758/BF03193850
10.1163/22134468-bja10019
10.1080/02724980042000048
10.3758/s13421-021-01251-5
10.3758/BF03210959
10.1016/j.neulet.2019.134683
10.1037/bul0000192
10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.978
10.3758/BF03197535
10.3758/BF03201181
10.1037/xhp0000996
10.1177/1747021820906397
10.1037/xhp0000720
10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
10.1037/xge0000783
10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.03.004
10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1
10.1037/xlm0001095
10.1037/xhp0000656
10.1176/jnp.9.1.55
10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01786
10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.515
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00327R
10.1006/nimg.1995.1034
10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0778-16.2016
10.1037/a0019957
10.1037/xhp0000580
10.3758/s13414-022-02447-w
10.1007/s00426-021-01554-x
10.3758/s13414-021-02403-0
10.1037/cep0000106
10.1016/j.neucom.2005.12.100
10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
10.1037/a0029145
10.1037/a0035032
10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00094-9
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01327
10.1037/xhp0000989
10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.024
10.1037/xlm0001058
10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
10.1037/xhp0000801
10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.016
10.15626/MP.2020.2577
10.1177/17470218231182854
10.21105/joss.03041
10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00282.x
10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01683
10.1037/abn0000184
10.1037/xlm0001144
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2024
2024. The Author(s).
Copyright Springer Nature B.V. May 2024
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2024
– notice: 2024. The Author(s).
– notice: Copyright Springer Nature B.V. May 2024
DBID C6C
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
4T-
4U-
K9.
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.3758/s13414-024-02892-9
DatabaseName SpringerOpen (Open Access)
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
CrossRef
Docstoc
University Readers
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
CrossRef
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
University Readers
Docstoc
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)


MEDLINE
CrossRef
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: C6C
  name: SpringerOpen (Open Access)
  url: http://www.springeropen.com/
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1943-393X
EndPage 1286
ExternalDocumentID 10_3758_s13414_024_02892_9
38691237
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Università degli Studi di Milano - Bicocca
GroupedDBID ---
-55
-5G
-BR
-EM
-~C
-~X
0-V
06D
0R~
0VY
186
199
1N0
203
23N
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2VQ
30V
3V.
4.4
406
408
40E
53G
6J9
7X7
875
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8G5
8TC
8UJ
95.
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AABYN
AACDK
AAEOY
AAFGU
AAFWJ
AAGAY
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANQY
AANZL
AARHV
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFGW
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJOX
ABJUD
ABKAS
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPPZ
ABQBU
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABUWG
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBMV
ACBRV
ACBXY
ACBYP
ACDPM
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIGE
ACIPQ
ACKIV
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACOKC
ACTTH
ACVWB
ACWMK
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADMDM
ADOXG
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEEQQ
AEFQL
AEFTE
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AESTI
AETCA
AETEA
AEVLU
AEVTX
AEXYK
AFDYV
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFNRJ
AFQWF
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGGBP
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AI.
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AIMYW
AITGF
AJBLW
AJDOV
AJRNO
AJZVZ
AKQUC
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AN0
AOCGG
AQQCX
ARALO
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
BAWUL
BENPR
BGNMA
BNQBC
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C1A
C6C
CCPQU
CJNVE
CSCUP
D0L
DDRTE
DIK
DNIVK
DPUIP
DWQXO
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FYUFA
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ6
GQ7
GUQSH
HMCUK
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
IKXTQ
IRVIT
ITM
IWAJR
J-C
JBSCW
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M0P
M1P
M2M
M2O
M2R
M4Y
MQGED
N2Q
N9A
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9I
O9J
OHT
OK1
P9L
PF-
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
R9I
ROL
RPV
RSV
S16
S1Z
S27
S3B
SBS
SBU
SCLPG
SHX
SISQX
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TN5
TSG
TUC
TUS
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UNUBA
UOJIU
UPT
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
VH1
W48
WH7
WK8
XJT
XKC
Z7W
Z81
Z83
Z92
ZA5
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
AAYZH
AFQWV
ALIPV
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
H13
NPM
PQEDU
SJYHP
AAYXX
CITATION
4T-
4U-
K9.
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-2c422f449ed03130559c1d2e6de57d4a44d176bc4195b8e42b9538f19312ac2e3
IEDL.DBID AGYKE
ISSN 1943-3921
1943-393X
IngestDate Tue Sep 17 21:28:37 EDT 2024
Sat Oct 26 04:49:18 EDT 2024
Sun Nov 17 11:16:21 EST 2024
Thu Sep 26 15:51:46 EDT 2024
Sat Nov 02 12:24:21 EDT 2024
Wed May 22 06:53:36 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Keywords Conflict-induced control
Reactive control
Proactive control
Stroop
Proportion-congruent effect
Language English
License 2024. The Author(s).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c426t-2c422f449ed03130559c1d2e6de57d4a44d176bc4195b8e42b9538f19312ac2e3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0002-3913-8109
OpenAccessLink https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-024-02892-9
PMID 38691237
PQID 3056236486
PQPubID 976350
PageCount 28
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11093857
proquest_miscellaneous_3050177456
proquest_journals_3056236486
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02892_9
pubmed_primary_38691237
springer_journals_10_3758_s13414_024_02892_9
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2024-05-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-05-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 05
  year: 2024
  text: 2024-05-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace New York
PublicationPlace_xml – name: New York
– name: United States
– name: Austin
PublicationTitle Attention, perception & psychophysics
PublicationTitleAbbrev Atten Percept Psychophys
PublicationTitleAlternate Atten Percept Psychophys
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher Springer US
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer US
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Dishon-Berkovits, Algom (CR35) 2000; 28
Hutcheon, Spieler (CR52) 2017; 70
Hutchison (CR53) 2011; 37
Algom, Fitousi, Chajut (CR3) 2022; 50
Spinelli, Lupker (CR100) 2022; 48
Abrahamse, Duthoo, Notebaert, Risko (CR1) 2013; 39
Bugg, Gonthier (CR18) 2020; 73
Spinelli, Krishna, Perry, Lupker (CR104) 2020; 46
Viviani, Visalli, Montefinese, Vallesi, Ambrosini (CR113) 2023
Crump, Milliken (CR32) 2009; 62
Hazeltine, Mordkoff (CR47) 2014; 5
Taylor, Kornblum, Lauber, Minoshima, Koeppe (CR110) 1997; 6
Kornblum, Stelmach, Requin (CR60) 1992
Hommel, Proctor, Vu (CR49) 2004; 68
Jiménez, Méndez, Abrahamse, Braem (CR56) 2021; 47
Tang, Bugg, Snijder, Conway, Braver (CR109) 2023; 76
Bugg, Jacoby, Toth (CR21) 2008; 36
Weidler, Pratt, Bugg (CR114) 2022; 48
Miller, Cohen (CR75) 2001; 24
Schmidt (CR90) 2021; 9
Chiu, Jiang, Egner (CR26) 2017; 37
Schmidt, Lemercier (CR92) 2019; 72
MacLeod (CR73) 1991; 109
Redding, Gerjets (CR82) 1977; 45
Cohen-Shikora, Suh, Bugg (CR29) 2019; 45
Bugg (CR15) 2014; 40
Kane, Engle (CR57) 2003; 132
Kim, Cho (CR58) 2014; 149
Schmidt, De Houwer, Besner (CR94) 2010; 19
Arechar, Rand (CR4) 2021; 53
CR59
Gonthier, Ambrosi, Blaye (CR44) 2021; 47
Ravnkilde, Videbech, Rosenberg, Gjedde, Gade (CR81) 2002; 24
George, Ketter, Parekh, Rosinsky, Ring, Casey, Trimble, Horwitz, Herscovitch, Post (CR41) 1994; 1
CR55
Parris, Hasshim, Wadsley, Augustinova, Ferrand (CR76) 2021; 86
Lupker, Brown, Colombo (CR70) 1997; 23
Chiu, Egner (CR25) 2019; 99
Jacoby, Lindsay, Hessels (CR54) 2003; 10
Hedge, Powell, Sumner (CR48) 2018; 50
Gullifer, Titone (CR45) 2021; 150
Blais, Robidoux, Risko, Besner (CR9) 2007; 114
George, Ketter, Parekh, Rosinsky, Ring, Pazzaglia, Marangell, Callahan, Post (CR42) 1997; 9
Crump, Gong, Milliken (CR31) 2006; 13
Lupker, Kinoshita, Coltheart, Taylor (CR71) 2003; 49
Tzelgov, Henik, Berger (CR111) 1992; 20
Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, Lang (CR38) 2009; 41
Hutcheon (CR51) 2022; 84
Huang, Su, Ma (CR50) 2020; 716
Funes, Lupiáñez, Humphreys (CR40) 2010; 114
Schmidt (CR89) 2019; 26
CR61
Schmidt (CR85) 2013; 20
Lu, Proctor (CR67) 1995; 2
Spinelli, Lupker (CR98) 2020; 48
Hasshim, Parris (CR46) 2021; 74
Logan, Zbrodoff (CR65) 1979; 7
MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, Carter (CR72) 2000; 288
CR78
CR77
CR116
Schmidt (CR87) 2013; 142
Bugg, Dey (CR17) 2018; 44
Weissman, Egner, Hawks, Link (CR115) 2015; 156
Welford (CR117) 1980
Bugg, Suh, Colvett, Lehmann (CR22) 2020; 46
Schmidt, Besner (CR91) 2008; 34
Schmidt, Giesen, Rothermund (CR95) 2020; 73
Bugg, Crump (CR16) 2012; 3
Puccioni, Vallesi (CR79) 2012; 6
Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, Cohen (CR11) 2001; 108
de Leeuw (CR33) 2015; 47
Draheim, Tsukahara, Martin, Mashburn, Engle (CR37) 2021; 150
Laird, McMillan, Lancaster, Kochunov, Turkeltaub, Pardo, Fox (CR62) 2005; 25
CR2
Schmidt (CR86) 2013; 8
Schmidt (CR88) 2017; 71
Braem, Bugg, Schmidt, Crump, Weissman, Notebaert, Egner (CR12) 2019; 23
Lupker (CR69) 1979; 7
Melara, Algom (CR74) 2003; 110
Bugg, Hutchison (CR19) 2013; 39
Lim, Cho (CR63) 2018; 190
Borgmann, Risko, Stolz, Besner (CR10) 2007; 14
Logan (CR64) 1988; 95
CR83
CR80
Carter, Macdonald, Botvinick, Ross, Stenger, Noll, Cohen (CR23) 2000; 97
Augustinova, Parris, Ferrand (CR5) 2019; 10
Bugg, Jacoby, Chanani (CR20) 2011; 37
Forrin, MacLeod (CR39) 2017; 45
Spinelli, Perry, Lupker (CR106) 2019; 45
Stroop (CR107) 1935; 18
Schmidt, Crump, Cheesman, Besner (CR93) 2007; 16
Cochrane, Pratt (CR27) 2022; 84
Crump, Brosowsky, Milliken (CR30) 2017; 70
Logan, Zbrodoff (CR66) 1998; 24
Spinelli, Lupker (CR99) 2021; 47
Gonthier, Braver, Bugg (CR43) 2016; 44
Spinelli, Morton, Lupker (CR105) 2022; 29
De Pisapia, Braver (CR34) 2006; 69
Visalli, Ambrosini, Viviani, Sambataro, Tenconi, Vallesi (CR112) 2023; 18
Tafuro, Vallesi, Ambrosini (CR108) 2020; 133
Blais, Bunge (CR8) 2010; 22
Draheim, Mashburn, Martin, Engle (CR36) 2019; 145
Cochrane, Pratt (CR28) 2022; 29
Braver, Kizhner, Tang, Freund, Etzel (CR14) 2021; 33
Braver (CR13) 2012; 16
Simon (CR97) 1969; 81
Sharma, McKenna (CR96) 1998; 26
Bejjani, Tan, Egner (CR7) 2020; 46
Carter, Mintun, Cohen (CR24) 1995; 2
CR102
CR101
Bejjani, Egner (CR6) 2021; 47
Spinelli, Goldsmith, Lupker, Morton (CR103) 2022; 48
Sabri, Melara, Algom (CR84) 2001; 27
Lu, Proctor (CR68) 2001; 54
MJ Crump (2892_CR31) 2006; 13
AR Laird (2892_CR62) 2005; 25
YC Chiu (2892_CR25) 2019; 99
S Kim (2892_CR58) 2014; 149
ND Forrin (2892_CR39) 2017; 45
C Gonthier (2892_CR43) 2016; 44
S Kornblum (2892_CR60) 1992
(2892_CR117) 1980
BA Parris (2892_CR76) 2021; 86
MS George (2892_CR41) 1994; 1
MS George (2892_CR42) 1997; 9
SJ Lupker (2892_CR69) 1979; 7
JR Simon (2892_CR97) 1969; 81
F Faul (2892_CR38) 2009; 41
G Spinelli (2892_CR103) 2022; 48
CS Carter (2892_CR23) 2000; 97
JR Schmidt (2892_CR89) 2019; 26
G Spinelli (2892_CR104) 2020; 46
SF Taylor (2892_CR110) 1997; 6
J Bugg (2892_CR15) 2014; 40
2892_CR55
BA Cochrane (2892_CR28) 2022; 29
MJ Kane (2892_CR57) 2003; 132
B Ravnkilde (2892_CR81) 2002; 24
JR Schmidt (2892_CR94) 2010; 19
SJ Lupker (2892_CR70) 1997; 23
MM Botvinick (2892_CR11) 2001; 108
G Spinelli (2892_CR98) 2020; 48
JM Bugg (2892_CR17) 2018; 44
CH Lu (2892_CR68) 2001; 54
2892_CR59
JR Schmidt (2892_CR92) 2019; 72
C Draheim (2892_CR36) 2019; 145
MJ Funes (2892_CR40) 2010; 114
2892_CR61
D Algom (2892_CR3) 2022; 50
L Jiménez (2892_CR56) 2021; 47
C Blais (2892_CR8) 2010; 22
BA Cochrane (2892_CR27) 2022; 84
MJ Crump (2892_CR32) 2009; 62
DH Weissman (2892_CR115) 2015; 156
AW MacDonald (2892_CR72) 2000; 288
TS Braver (2892_CR14) 2021; 33
AA Arechar (2892_CR4) 2021; 53
JM Bugg (2892_CR16) 2012; 3
G Spinelli (2892_CR106) 2019; 45
2892_CR77
JR Schmidt (2892_CR86) 2013; 8
G Spinelli (2892_CR99) 2021; 47
EL Abrahamse (2892_CR1) 2013; 39
CS Carter (2892_CR24) 1995; 2
TG Hutcheon (2892_CR52) 2017; 70
JW Gullifer (2892_CR45) 2021; 150
MJ Crump (2892_CR30) 2017; 70
BJ Weidler (2892_CR114) 2022; 48
C Bejjani (2892_CR7) 2020; 46
CE Lim (2892_CR63) 2018; 190
JR Schmidt (2892_CR95) 2020; 73
ER Cohen-Shikora (2892_CR29) 2019; 45
N De Pisapia (2892_CR34) 2006; 69
C Gonthier (2892_CR44) 2021; 47
S Braem (2892_CR12) 2019; 23
M Dishon-Berkovits (2892_CR35) 2000; 28
2892_CR78
2892_CR83
2892_CR80
B Hommel (2892_CR49) 2004; 68
GD Logan (2892_CR64) 1988; 95
C Draheim (2892_CR37) 2021; 150
JR Schmidt (2892_CR90) 2021; 9
J Stroop (2892_CR107) 1935; 18
TG Hutcheon (2892_CR51) 2022; 84
G Spinelli (2892_CR105) 2022; 29
RD Melara (2892_CR74) 2003; 110
2892_CR102
JM Bugg (2892_CR18) 2020; 73
C Bejjani (2892_CR6) 2021; 47
2892_CR101
Y Huang (2892_CR50) 2020; 716
CH Lu (2892_CR67) 1995; 2
O Puccioni (2892_CR79) 2012; 6
A Tafuro (2892_CR108) 2020; 133
JM Bugg (2892_CR20) 2011; 37
JM Bugg (2892_CR19) 2013; 39
M Sabri (2892_CR84) 2001; 27
GM Redding (2892_CR82) 1977; 45
A Visalli (2892_CR112) 2023; 18
JR Schmidt (2892_CR91) 2008; 34
R Tang (2892_CR109) 2023; 76
CM MacLeod (2892_CR73) 1991; 109
EK Miller (2892_CR75) 2001; 24
JR Schmidt (2892_CR93) 2007; 16
G Viviani (2892_CR113) 2023
2892_CR116
YC Chiu (2892_CR26) 2017; 37
KW Borgmann (2892_CR10) 2007; 14
GD Logan (2892_CR66) 1998; 24
JR Schmidt (2892_CR85) 2013; 20
JR Schmidt (2892_CR88) 2017; 71
2892_CR2
TS Braver (2892_CR13) 2012; 16
D Sharma (2892_CR96) 1998; 26
J Tzelgov (2892_CR111) 1992; 20
JR de Leeuw (2892_CR33) 2015; 47
SJ Lupker (2892_CR71) 2003; 49
C Blais (2892_CR9) 2007; 114
KA Hutchison (2892_CR53) 2011; 37
G Spinelli (2892_CR100) 2022; 48
N Hasshim (2892_CR46) 2021; 74
LL Jacoby (2892_CR54) 2003; 10
JR Schmidt (2892_CR87) 2013; 142
C Hedge (2892_CR48) 2018; 50
E Hazeltine (2892_CR47) 2014; 5
JM Bugg (2892_CR22) 2020; 46
M Augustinova (2892_CR5) 2019; 10
GD Logan (2892_CR65) 1979; 7
JM Bugg (2892_CR21) 2008; 36
References_xml – volume: 46
  start-page: 2007
  year: 2020
  end-page: 2033
  ident: CR104
  article-title: Working memory load dissociates contingency learning and item-specific proportion-congruent effects
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000934
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– volume: 45
  start-page: 11
  year: 1977
  end-page: 17
  ident: CR82
  article-title: Stroop effect: Interference and facilitation with verbal and manual responses
  publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills
  doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.45.1.11
  contributor:
    fullname: Gerjets
– volume: 37
  start-page: 851
  year: 2011
  end-page: 860
  ident: CR53
  article-title: The interactive effects of listwide control, item-based control, and working memory capacity on Stroop performance
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0023437
  contributor:
    fullname: Hutchison
– volume: 18
  year: 2023
  ident: CR112
  article-title: On the relationship between emotions and cognitive control: Evidence from an observational study on emotional priming Stroop task
  publication-title: Plos one
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294957
  contributor:
    fullname: Vallesi
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1792
  year: 2017
  end-page: 1807
  ident: CR30
  article-title: Reproducing the location-based context-specific proportion congruent effect for frequency unbiased items: A reply to Hutcheon and Spieler (2016)
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1206130
  contributor:
    fullname: Milliken
– volume: 109
  start-page: 163
  year: 1991
  end-page: 203
  ident: CR73
  article-title: Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
  contributor:
    fullname: MacLeod
– ident: CR80
– ident: CR77
– volume: 110
  start-page: 422
  year: 2003
  end-page: 471
  ident: CR74
  article-title: Driven by information: a tectonic theory of Stroop effects
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.422
  contributor:
    fullname: Algom
– volume: 150
  start-page: 414
  year: 2021
  end-page: 430
  ident: CR45
  article-title: Engaging proactive control: Influences of diverse language experiences using insights from machine learning
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000933
  contributor:
    fullname: Titone
– volume: 142
  start-page: 119
  year: 2013
  end-page: 126
  ident: CR87
  article-title: The Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model: Dissociating contingency and conflict adaptation in the item-specific proportion congruent paradigm
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.004
  contributor:
    fullname: Schmidt
– volume: 53
  start-page: 2591
  year: 2021
  end-page: 2595
  ident: CR4
  article-title: Turking in the time of COVID
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01588-4
  contributor:
    fullname: Rand
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1550
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1562
  ident: CR99
  article-title: Proactive control in the Stroop task: A conflict-frequency manipulation free of item-specific, contingency-learning, and color-word correlation confounds
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000820
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– ident: CR101
– volume: 62
  start-page: 1523
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1532
  ident: CR32
  article-title: Short article: The flexibility of context-specific control: Evidence for context-driven generalization of item-specific control settings
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470210902752096
  contributor:
    fullname: Milliken
– volume: 22
  start-page: 2758
  year: 2010
  end-page: 2767
  ident: CR8
  article-title: Behavioral and neural evidence for item-specific performance monitoring
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21365
  contributor:
    fullname: Bunge
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1457
  year: 2023
  end-page: 1480
  ident: CR109
  article-title: The Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control (DMCC) project: Validation of an online behavioural task battery
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/17470218221114769
  contributor:
    fullname: Braver
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1821
  year: 2022
  end-page: 1830
  ident: CR28
  article-title: The item-specific proportion congruency effect transfers to non-category members based on broad visual similarity
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02104-1
  contributor:
    fullname: Pratt
– volume: 288
  start-page: 1835
  year: 2000
  end-page: 1838
  ident: CR72
  article-title: Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
  contributor:
    fullname: Carter
– volume: 132
  start-page: 47
  year: 2003
  end-page: 70
  ident: CR57
  article-title: Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
  contributor:
    fullname: Engle
– volume: 1
  start-page: 194
  year: 1994
  end-page: 209
  ident: CR41
  article-title: Regional brain activity when selecting a response despite interference: An H2 15O PET study of the Stroop and an emotional Stroop
  publication-title: Human Brain Mapping
  doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010305
  contributor:
    fullname: Post
– volume: 99
  start-page: 33
  year: 2019
  end-page: 41
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Cortical and subcortical contributions to context-control learning
  publication-title: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.019
  contributor:
    fullname: Egner
– volume: 20
  start-page: 615
  year: 2013
  end-page: 630
  ident: CR85
  article-title: Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0373-0
  contributor:
    fullname: Schmidt
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1206
  year: 2017
  end-page: 1222
  ident: CR39
  article-title: Relative speed of processing determines color–word contingency learning
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0721-4
  contributor:
    fullname: MacLeod
– volume: 6
  start-page: 81
  year: 1997
  end-page: 92
  ident: CR110
  article-title: Isolation of specific interference processing in the Stroop task: PET activation studies
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0285
  contributor:
    fullname: Koeppe
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1660
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1685
  ident: CR44
  article-title: Learning-based before intentional cognitive control: Developmental evidence for a dissociation between implicit and explicit control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001005
  contributor:
    fullname: Blaye
– volume: 190
  start-page: 267
  year: 2018
  end-page: 276
  ident: CR63
  article-title: Determining the scope of control underlying the congruency sequence effect: roles of stimulus-response mapping and response mode
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.08.012
  contributor:
    fullname: Cho
– volume: 73
  start-page: 1444
  year: 2020
  end-page: 1459
  ident: CR18
  article-title: List-level control in the flanker task
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021820912477
  contributor:
    fullname: Gonthier
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  end-page: 12
  ident: CR33
  article-title: jsPsych: A JavaScript library for creating behavioral experiments in a Web browser
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0458-y
  contributor:
    fullname: de Leeuw
– volume: 34
  start-page: 514
  year: 2008
  end-page: 523
  ident: CR91
  article-title: The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514
  contributor:
    fullname: Besner
– volume: 48
  start-page: 426
  year: 2020
  end-page: 435
  ident: CR98
  article-title: Item-specific control of attention in the Stroop task: Contingency learning is not the whole story in the item-specific proportion-congruent effect
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-00980-y
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– volume: 16
  start-page: 421
  year: 2007
  end-page: 435
  ident: CR93
  article-title: Contingency learning without awareness: Evidence for implicit control
  publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.010
  contributor:
    fullname: Besner
– year: 1980
  ident: CR117
  publication-title: Reaction Times
  contributor:
    fullname: Welford
– volume: 44
  start-page: 778
  year: 2016
  end-page: 788
  ident: CR43
  article-title: Dissociating proactive and reactive control in the Stroop task
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0591-1
  contributor:
    fullname: Bugg
– volume: 24
  start-page: 167
  year: 2001
  end-page: 202
  ident: CR75
  article-title: An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function
  publication-title: Annual Review of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
  contributor:
    fullname: Cohen
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1703
  year: 2019
  end-page: 1723
  ident: CR29
  article-title: Assessing the temporal learning account of the list-wide proportion congruence effect
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000670
  contributor:
    fullname: Bugg
– volume: 81
  start-page: 174
  year: 1969
  end-page: 176
  ident: CR97
  article-title: Reactions toward the source of stimulation
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/h0027448
  contributor:
    fullname: Simon
– volume: 26
  start-page: 753
  year: 2019
  end-page: 771
  ident: CR89
  article-title: Evidence against conflict monitoring and adaptation: An updated review
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1520-z
  contributor:
    fullname: Schmidt
– volume: 14
  start-page: 313
  year: 2007
  end-page: 319
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Simon says: Reliability and the role of working memory and attentional control in the Simon task
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194070
  contributor:
    fullname: Besner
– volume: 97
  start-page: 1944
  year: 2000
  end-page: 1948
  ident: CR23
  article-title: Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1944
  contributor:
    fullname: Cohen
– volume: 114
  start-page: 1076
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1086
  ident: CR9
  article-title: Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: A computational model
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.114.4.1076
  contributor:
    fullname: Besner
– volume: 23
  start-page: 769
  year: 2019
  end-page: 783
  ident: CR12
  article-title: Measuring adaptive control in conflict tasks
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002
  contributor:
    fullname: Egner
– volume: 74
  start-page: 1657
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1668
  ident: CR46
  article-title: The role of contingency and correlation in the Stroop task
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/17470218211032548
  contributor:
    fullname: Parris
– volume: 39
  start-page: 1552
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1562
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Attention modulation by proportion congruency: The asymmetrical list shifting effect
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0032426
  contributor:
    fullname: Risko
– year: 2023
  ident: CR113
  publication-title: The Stroop legacy: A cautionary tale on methodological issues and a proposed spatial solution
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-023-02215-0
  contributor:
    fullname: Ambrosini
– volume: 18
  start-page: 643
  year: 1935
  end-page: 662
  ident: CR107
  article-title: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/h0054651
  contributor:
    fullname: Stroop
– volume: 25
  start-page: 6
  year: 2005
  end-page: 21
  ident: CR62
  article-title: A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task
  publication-title: Human Brain Mapping
  doi: 10.1002/hbm.20129
  contributor:
    fullname: Fox
– ident: CR55
– volume: 68
  start-page: 1
  year: 2004
  end-page: 17
  ident: CR49
  article-title: A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task
  publication-title: Psychological Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
  contributor:
    fullname: Vu
– ident: CR83
– ident: CR102
– volume: 24
  start-page: 534
  year: 2002
  end-page: 547
  ident: CR81
  article-title: Putative tests of frontal lobe function: a PET-study of brain activation during Stroop's Test and verbal fluency
  publication-title: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
  doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.4.534.1033
  contributor:
    fullname: Gade
– volume: 36
  start-page: 1484
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1494
  ident: CR21
  article-title: Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/MC.36.8.1484
  contributor:
    fullname: Toth
– volume: 10
  start-page: 638
  year: 2003
  end-page: 644
  ident: CR54
  article-title: Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196526
  contributor:
    fullname: Hessels
– volume: 28
  start-page: 1437
  year: 2000
  end-page: 1449
  ident: CR35
  article-title: The Stroop effect: It is not the robust phenomenon that you have thought it to be
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211844
  contributor:
    fullname: Algom
– volume: 156
  start-page: 8
  year: 2015
  end-page: 21
  ident: CR115
  article-title: The congruency sequence effect emerges when the distracter precedes the target
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.01.003
  contributor:
    fullname: Link
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1686
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1704
  ident: CR56
  article-title: It is harder than you think: On the boundary conditions of exploiting congruency cues
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000844
  contributor:
    fullname: Braem
– volume: 23
  start-page: 570
  year: 1997
  end-page: 590
  ident: CR70
  article-title: Strategic control in a naming task: Changing routes or changing deadlines?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.570
  contributor:
    fullname: Colombo
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1990
  year: 2021
  end-page: 2015
  ident: CR14
  article-title: The dual mechanisms of cognitive control project
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01768
  contributor:
    fullname: Etzel
– start-page: 743
  year: 1992
  end-page: 777
  ident: CR60
  article-title: Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility
  publication-title: Tutorials in motor behavior
  contributor:
    fullname: Requin
– volume: 7
  start-page: 485
  year: 1979
  end-page: 495
  ident: CR69
  article-title: The semantic nature of response competition in the picture-word interference task
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03198265
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– volume: 20
  start-page: 727
  year: 1992
  end-page: 735
  ident: CR111
  article-title: Controlling Stroop effects by manipulating expectations for color words
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03202722
  contributor:
    fullname: Berger
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1292
  year: 2017
  end-page: 1304
  ident: CR52
  article-title: Limits on the generalizability of context-driven control
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1182193
  contributor:
    fullname: Spieler
– volume: 29
  start-page: 2133
  year: 2022
  end-page: 2145
  ident: CR105
  article-title: Both task-irrelevant and task-relevant information trigger reactive conflict adaptation in the item-specific proportion-congruent paradigm
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02138-5
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1119
  year: 2019
  end-page: 1130
  ident: CR92
  article-title: Context-specific proportion congruent effects: Compound-cue contingency learning in disguise
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021818787155
  contributor:
    fullname: Lemercier
– ident: CR61
– volume: 8
  year: 2013
  ident: CR86
  article-title: Temporal learning and list-level proportion congruency: conflict adaptation or learning when to respond?
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082320
  contributor:
    fullname: Schmidt
– volume: 114
  start-page: 338
  year: 2010
  end-page: 347
  ident: CR40
  article-title: Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.007
  contributor:
    fullname: Humphreys
– volume: 13
  start-page: 316
  year: 2006
  end-page: 321
  ident: CR31
  article-title: The context-specific proportion congruent Stroop effect: Location as a contextual cue
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193850
  contributor:
    fullname: Milliken
– volume: 9
  start-page: 161
  year: 2021
  end-page: 197
  ident: CR90
  article-title: When data transformations are appropriate or even necessary: A response to Cohen-Shikora, Suh and Bugg (2019)
  publication-title: Timing & Time Perception
  doi: 10.1163/22134468-bja10019
  contributor:
    fullname: Schmidt
– volume: 54
  start-page: 95
  year: 2001
  end-page: 136
  ident: CR68
  article-title: Influence of irrelevant information on human performance: Effects of SR association strength and relative timing
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A
  doi: 10.1080/02724980042000048
  contributor:
    fullname: Proctor
– volume: 50
  start-page: 883
  year: 2022
  end-page: 897
  ident: CR3
  article-title: Can the Stroop effect serve as the gold standard of conflict monitoring and control? A conceptual critique
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-021-01251-5
  contributor:
    fullname: Chajut
– volume: 2
  start-page: 174
  year: 1995
  end-page: 207
  ident: CR67
  article-title: The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03210959
  contributor:
    fullname: Proctor
– volume: 716
  year: 2020
  ident: CR50
  article-title: The Stroop effect: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis in healthy young adults
  publication-title: Neuroscience Letters
  doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2019.134683
  contributor:
    fullname: Ma
– volume: 145
  start-page: 508
  year: 2019
  end-page: 535
  ident: CR36
  article-title: Reaction time in differential and developmental research: A review and commentary on the problems and alternatives
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: 10.1037/bul0000192
  contributor:
    fullname: Engle
– volume: 24
  start-page: 978
  year: 1998
  end-page: 992
  ident: CR66
  article-title: Stroop-type interference: Congruity effects in color naming with typewritten responses
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.978
  contributor:
    fullname: Zbrodoff
– volume: 7
  start-page: 166
  year: 1979
  end-page: 174
  ident: CR65
  article-title: When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03197535
  contributor:
    fullname: Zbrodoff
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1033
  year: 1998
  end-page: 1040
  ident: CR96
  article-title: Differential components of the manual and vocal Stroop tasks
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03201181
  contributor:
    fullname: McKenna
– volume: 48
  start-page: 497
  year: 2022
  end-page: 530
  ident: CR100
  article-title: Conflict-monitoring theory in overtime: Is temporal learning a viable explanation for the congruency sequence effect?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000996
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– ident: CR116
– volume: 73
  start-page: 739
  year: 2020
  end-page: 761
  ident: CR95
  article-title: Contingency learning as binding? Testing an exemplar view of the colour-word contingency learning effect
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021820906397
  contributor:
    fullname: Rothermund
– volume: 46
  start-page: 369
  year: 2020
  end-page: 387
  ident: CR7
  article-title: Performance feedback promotes proactive but not reactive adaptation of conflict-control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000720
  contributor:
    fullname: Egner
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1149
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1160
  ident: CR38
  article-title: Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
  contributor:
    fullname: Lang
– ident: CR78
– volume: 150
  start-page: 242
  year: 2021
  end-page: 275
  ident: CR37
  article-title: A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000783
  contributor:
    fullname: Engle
– volume: 95
  start-page: 492
  year: 1988
  end-page: 527
  ident: CR64
  article-title: Toward an instance theory of automatization
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
  contributor:
    fullname: Logan
– volume: 149
  start-page: 60
  year: 2014
  end-page: 68
  ident: CR58
  article-title: Congruency sequence effect without feature integration and contingency learning
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.03.004
  contributor:
    fullname: Cho
– volume: 50
  start-page: 1166
  year: 2018
  end-page: 1186
  ident: CR48
  article-title: The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1
  contributor:
    fullname: Sumner
– volume: 48
  start-page: 906
  year: 2022
  end-page: 927
  ident: CR103
  article-title: Bilingualism and executive attention: Evidence from studies of proactive and reactive control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001095
  contributor:
    fullname: Morton
– volume: 45
  start-page: 995
  year: 2019
  end-page: 1014
  ident: CR106
  article-title: Adaptation to conflict frequency without contingency and temporal learning: Evidence from the picture-word interference task
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000656
  contributor:
    fullname: Lupker
– volume: 9
  start-page: 55
  year: 1997
  end-page: 63
  ident: CR42
  article-title: Blunted left cingulate activation in mood disorder subjects during a response interference task (the Stroop)
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
  doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.1.55
  contributor:
    fullname: Post
– volume: 10
  start-page: 1786
  year: 2019
  ident: CR5
  article-title: The loci of Stroop interference and facilitation effects with manual and vocal responses
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01786
  contributor:
    fullname: Ferrand
– volume: 27
  start-page: 515
  year: 2001
  end-page: 537
  ident: CR84
  article-title: A confluence of contexts: Asymmetric versus global failures of selective attention to Stroop dimensions
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.515
  contributor:
    fullname: Algom
– volume: 6
  start-page: 327
  year: 2012
  ident: CR79
  article-title: High cognitive reserve is associated with a reduced age-related deficit in spatial conflict resolution
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00327R
  contributor:
    fullname: Vallesi
– volume: 2
  start-page: 264
  year: 1995
  end-page: 272
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Interference and facilitation effects during selective attention: an H215O PET study of Stroop task performance
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1034
  contributor:
    fullname: Cohen
– volume: 3
  start-page: 367
  year: 2012
  ident: CR16
  article-title: In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: A review of the literature on proportion congruent effects
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
  contributor:
    fullname: Crump
– volume: 37
  start-page: 1028
  year: 2017
  end-page: 1038
  ident: CR26
  article-title: The caudate nucleus mediates learning of stimulus–control state associations
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0778-16.2016
  contributor:
    fullname: Egner
– ident: CR2
– volume: 37
  start-page: 844
  year: 2011
  end-page: 859
  ident: CR20
  article-title: Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0019957
  contributor:
    fullname: Chanani
– volume: 44
  start-page: 1905
  year: 2018
  end-page: 1932
  ident: CR17
  article-title: When stimulus-driven control settings compete: On the dominance of categories as cues for control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000580
  contributor:
    fullname: Dey
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1248
  year: 2022
  end-page: 1263
  ident: CR51
  article-title: What is cued by faces in the face-based context-specific proportion congruent manipulation?
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-022-02447-w
  contributor:
    fullname: Hutcheon
– volume: 86
  start-page: 1029
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1053
  ident: CR76
  article-title: The loci of Stroop effects: a critical review of methods and evidence for levels of processing contributing to color-word Stroop effects and the implications for the loci of attentional selection
  publication-title: Psychological Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01554-x
  contributor:
    fullname: Ferrand
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1
  year: 2022
  end-page: 9
  ident: CR27
  article-title: The item-specific proportion congruency effect can be contaminated by short-term repetition priming
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-021-02403-0
  contributor:
    fullname: Pratt
– volume: 71
  start-page: 52
  year: 2017
  end-page: 62
  ident: CR88
  article-title: Time-out for conflict monitoring theory: Preventing rhythmic biases eliminates the list-level proportion congruent effect
  publication-title: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale
  doi: 10.1037/cep0000106
  contributor:
    fullname: Schmidt
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1322
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1326
  ident: CR34
  article-title: A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions
  publication-title: Neurocomputing
  doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2005.12.100
  contributor:
    fullname: Braver
– volume: 16
  start-page: 106
  year: 2012
  end-page: 113
  ident: CR13
  article-title: The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
  contributor:
    fullname: Braver
– volume: 39
  start-page: 433
  year: 2013
  end-page: 449
  ident: CR19
  article-title: Converging evidence for control of color–word Stroop interference at the item level
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0029145
  contributor:
    fullname: Hutchison
– volume: 40
  start-page: 567
  year: 2014
  end-page: 587
  ident: CR15
  article-title: Conflict-triggered top-down control: Default mode, last resort, or no such thing?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0035032
  contributor:
    fullname: Bugg
– volume: 49
  start-page: 556
  year: 2003
  end-page: 575
  ident: CR71
  article-title: Mixing costs and mixing benefits in naming words, pictures, and sums
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00094-9
  contributor:
    fullname: Taylor
– volume: 5
  start-page: 1327
  year: 2014
  ident: CR47
  article-title: Resolved but not forgotten: Stroop conflict dredges up the past
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01327
  contributor:
    fullname: Mordkoff
– volume: 48
  start-page: 312
  year: 2022
  end-page: 330
  ident: CR114
  article-title: How is location defined? Implications for learning and transfer of location-specific control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000989
  contributor:
    fullname: Bugg
– volume: 133
  start-page: 188
  year: 2020
  end-page: 200
  ident: CR108
  article-title: Cognitive brakes in interference resolution: A mouse-tracking and EEG co-registration study
  publication-title: Cortex
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.024
  contributor:
    fullname: Ambrosini
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1599
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1621
  ident: CR6
  article-title: Evaluating the learning of stimulus-control associations through incidental memory of reinforcement events
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001058
  contributor:
    fullname: Egner
– volume: 108
  start-page: 624
  year: 2001
  end-page: 652
  ident: CR11
  article-title: Conflict monitoring and cognitive control
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
  contributor:
    fullname: Cohen
– ident: CR59
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1029
  year: 2020
  end-page: 1050
  ident: CR22
  article-title: What can be learned in a context-specific proportion congruence paradigm? Implications for reproducibility
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000801
  contributor:
    fullname: Lehmann
– volume: 19
  start-page: 235
  year: 2010
  end-page: 250
  ident: CR94
  article-title: Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: A resource dependent process
  publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.016
  contributor:
    fullname: Besner
– volume: 40
  start-page: 567
  year: 2014
  ident: 2892_CR15
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0035032
  contributor:
    fullname: J Bugg
– volume: 9
  start-page: 55
  year: 1997
  ident: 2892_CR42
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
  doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.1.55
  contributor:
    fullname: MS George
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1599
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR6
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001058
  contributor:
    fullname: C Bejjani
– volume: 73
  start-page: 1444
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR18
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021820912477
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 27
  start-page: 515
  year: 2001
  ident: 2892_CR84
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.515
  contributor:
    fullname: M Sabri
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1686
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR56
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000844
  contributor:
    fullname: L Jiménez
– volume: 50
  start-page: 883
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR3
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-021-01251-5
  contributor:
    fullname: D Algom
– start-page: 743
  volume-title: Tutorials in motor behavior
  year: 1992
  ident: 2892_CR60
  contributor:
    fullname: S Kornblum
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1119
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR92
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021818787155
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 13
  start-page: 316
  year: 2006
  ident: 2892_CR31
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193850
  contributor:
    fullname: MJ Crump
– volume: 149
  start-page: 60
  year: 2014
  ident: 2892_CR58
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.03.004
  contributor:
    fullname: S Kim
– volume: 95
  start-page: 492
  year: 1988
  ident: 2892_CR64
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
  contributor:
    fullname: GD Logan
– volume: 71
  start-page: 52
  year: 2017
  ident: 2892_CR88
  publication-title: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale
  doi: 10.1037/cep0000106
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1248
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR51
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-022-02447-w
  contributor:
    fullname: TG Hutcheon
– ident: 2892_CR78
  doi: 10.15626/MP.2020.2577
– ident: 2892_CR102
  doi: 10.1177/17470218231182854
– volume: 29
  start-page: 2133
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR105
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02138-5
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– volume: 39
  start-page: 1552
  year: 2013
  ident: 2892_CR1
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0032426
  contributor:
    fullname: EL Abrahamse
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1149
  year: 2009
  ident: 2892_CR38
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
  contributor:
    fullname: F Faul
– volume: 37
  start-page: 1028
  year: 2017
  ident: 2892_CR26
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0778-16.2016
  contributor:
    fullname: YC Chiu
– volume: 142
  start-page: 119
  year: 2013
  ident: 2892_CR87
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.004
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  ident: 2892_CR33
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0458-y
  contributor:
    fullname: JR de Leeuw
– volume: 9
  start-page: 161
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR90
  publication-title: Timing & Time Perception
  doi: 10.1163/22134468-bja10019
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 37
  start-page: 851
  year: 2011
  ident: 2892_CR53
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0023437
  contributor:
    fullname: KA Hutchison
– volume: 6
  start-page: 327
  year: 2012
  ident: 2892_CR79
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00327R
  contributor:
    fullname: O Puccioni
– volume-title: The Stroop legacy: A cautionary tale on methodological issues and a proposed spatial solution
  year: 2023
  ident: 2892_CR113
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-023-02215-0
  contributor:
    fullname: G Viviani
– volume: 48
  start-page: 312
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR114
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000989
  contributor:
    fullname: BJ Weidler
– volume: 16
  start-page: 106
  year: 2012
  ident: 2892_CR13
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010
  contributor:
    fullname: TS Braver
– volume: 36
  start-page: 1484
  year: 2008
  ident: 2892_CR21
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/MC.36.8.1484
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 132
  start-page: 47
  year: 2003
  ident: 2892_CR57
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
  contributor:
    fullname: MJ Kane
– volume: 81
  start-page: 174
  year: 1969
  ident: 2892_CR97
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/h0027448
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Simon
– volume: 22
  start-page: 2758
  year: 2010
  ident: 2892_CR8
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21365
  contributor:
    fullname: C Blais
– volume: 7
  start-page: 485
  year: 1979
  ident: 2892_CR69
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03198265
  contributor:
    fullname: SJ Lupker
– volume: 716
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR50
  publication-title: Neuroscience Letters
  doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2019.134683
  contributor:
    fullname: Y Huang
– volume: 48
  start-page: 906
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR103
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001095
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1457
  year: 2023
  ident: 2892_CR109
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/17470218221114769
  contributor:
    fullname: R Tang
– volume: 26
  start-page: 753
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR89
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1520-z
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 114
  start-page: 338
  year: 2010
  ident: 2892_CR40
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.007
  contributor:
    fullname: MJ Funes
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1550
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR99
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000820
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1206
  year: 2017
  ident: 2892_CR39
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0721-4
  contributor:
    fullname: ND Forrin
– volume: 50
  start-page: 1166
  year: 2018
  ident: 2892_CR48
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1
  contributor:
    fullname: C Hedge
– volume: 150
  start-page: 414
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR45
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000933
  contributor:
    fullname: JW Gullifer
– ident: 2892_CR59
– volume: 68
  start-page: 1
  year: 2004
  ident: 2892_CR49
  publication-title: Psychological Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
  contributor:
    fullname: B Hommel
– volume: 24
  start-page: 978
  year: 1998
  ident: 2892_CR66
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.978
  contributor:
    fullname: GD Logan
– volume: 73
  start-page: 739
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR95
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021820906397
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 86
  start-page: 1029
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR76
  publication-title: Psychological Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01554-x
  contributor:
    fullname: BA Parris
– volume: 54
  start-page: 95
  year: 2001
  ident: 2892_CR68
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A
  doi: 10.1080/02724980042000048
  contributor:
    fullname: CH Lu
– volume: 53
  start-page: 2591
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR4
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01588-4
  contributor:
    fullname: AA Arechar
– volume: 8
  year: 2013
  ident: 2892_CR86
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082320
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– ident: 2892_CR77
  doi: 10.21105/joss.03041
– volume: 190
  start-page: 267
  year: 2018
  ident: 2892_CR63
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.08.012
  contributor:
    fullname: CE Lim
– volume: 18
  start-page: 643
  year: 1935
  ident: 2892_CR107
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/h0054651
  contributor:
    fullname: J Stroop
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1033
  year: 1998
  ident: 2892_CR96
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03201181
  contributor:
    fullname: D Sharma
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1703
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR29
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000670
  contributor:
    fullname: ER Cohen-Shikora
– volume: 46
  start-page: 369
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR7
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000720
  contributor:
    fullname: C Bejjani
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1292
  year: 2017
  ident: 2892_CR52
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1182193
  contributor:
    fullname: TG Hutcheon
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1029
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR22
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000801
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 2
  start-page: 174
  year: 1995
  ident: 2892_CR67
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03210959
  contributor:
    fullname: CH Lu
– volume: 7
  start-page: 166
  year: 1979
  ident: 2892_CR65
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03197535
  contributor:
    fullname: GD Logan
– volume-title: Reaction Times
  year: 1980
  ident: 2892_CR117
– volume: 25
  start-page: 6
  year: 2005
  ident: 2892_CR62
  publication-title: Human Brain Mapping
  doi: 10.1002/hbm.20129
  contributor:
    fullname: AR Laird
– volume: 74
  start-page: 1657
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR46
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/17470218211032548
  contributor:
    fullname: N Hasshim
– volume: 18
  year: 2023
  ident: 2892_CR112
  publication-title: Plos one
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294957
  contributor:
    fullname: A Visalli
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1322
  year: 2006
  ident: 2892_CR34
  publication-title: Neurocomputing
  doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2005.12.100
  contributor:
    fullname: N De Pisapia
– volume: 156
  start-page: 8
  year: 2015
  ident: 2892_CR115
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.01.003
  contributor:
    fullname: DH Weissman
– ident: 2892_CR61
  doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00282.x
– volume: 133
  start-page: 188
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR108
  publication-title: Cortex
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.024
  contributor:
    fullname: A Tafuro
– volume: 2
  start-page: 264
  year: 1995
  ident: 2892_CR24
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1034
  contributor:
    fullname: CS Carter
– volume: 14
  start-page: 313
  year: 2007
  ident: 2892_CR10
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194070
  contributor:
    fullname: KW Borgmann
– volume: 114
  start-page: 1076
  year: 2007
  ident: 2892_CR9
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.114.4.1076
  contributor:
    fullname: C Blais
– volume: 99
  start-page: 33
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR25
  publication-title: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.019
  contributor:
    fullname: YC Chiu
– volume: 23
  start-page: 570
  year: 1997
  ident: 2892_CR70
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.23.3.570
  contributor:
    fullname: SJ Lupker
– volume: 44
  start-page: 1905
  year: 2018
  ident: 2892_CR17
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000580
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 20
  start-page: 615
  year: 2013
  ident: 2892_CR85
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0373-0
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 109
  start-page: 163
  year: 1991
  ident: 2892_CR73
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
  contributor:
    fullname: CM MacLeod
– ident: 2892_CR2
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01683
– volume: 10
  start-page: 1786
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR5
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01786
  contributor:
    fullname: M Augustinova
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1990
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR14
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01768
  contributor:
    fullname: TS Braver
– volume: 108
  start-page: 624
  year: 2001
  ident: 2892_CR11
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
  contributor:
    fullname: MM Botvinick
– ident: 2892_CR116
– volume: 34
  start-page: 514
  year: 2008
  ident: 2892_CR91
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 39
  start-page: 433
  year: 2013
  ident: 2892_CR19
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0029145
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 19
  start-page: 235
  year: 2010
  ident: 2892_CR94
  publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.016
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 288
  start-page: 1835
  year: 2000
  ident: 2892_CR72
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
  contributor:
    fullname: AW MacDonald
– volume: 37
  start-page: 844
  year: 2011
  ident: 2892_CR20
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0019957
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1660
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR44
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001005
  contributor:
    fullname: C Gonthier
– volume: 49
  start-page: 556
  year: 2003
  ident: 2892_CR71
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00094-9
  contributor:
    fullname: SJ Lupker
– volume: 16
  start-page: 421
  year: 2007
  ident: 2892_CR93
  publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.010
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 97
  start-page: 1944
  year: 2000
  ident: 2892_CR23
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1944
  contributor:
    fullname: CS Carter
– volume: 150
  start-page: 242
  year: 2021
  ident: 2892_CR37
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000783
  contributor:
    fullname: C Draheim
– ident: 2892_CR55
– volume: 44
  start-page: 778
  year: 2016
  ident: 2892_CR43
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0591-1
  contributor:
    fullname: C Gonthier
– volume: 110
  start-page: 422
  year: 2003
  ident: 2892_CR74
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.422
  contributor:
    fullname: RD Melara
– volume: 6
  start-page: 81
  year: 1997
  ident: 2892_CR110
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0285
  contributor:
    fullname: SF Taylor
– volume: 45
  start-page: 995
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR106
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000656
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– volume: 48
  start-page: 497
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR100
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000996
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR27
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-021-02403-0
  contributor:
    fullname: BA Cochrane
– volume: 145
  start-page: 508
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR36
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: 10.1037/bul0000192
  contributor:
    fullname: C Draheim
– volume: 46
  start-page: 2007
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR104
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0000934
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– ident: 2892_CR80
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1821
  year: 2022
  ident: 2892_CR28
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-022-02104-1
  contributor:
    fullname: BA Cochrane
– ident: 2892_CR83
  doi: 10.1037/abn0000184
– volume: 1
  start-page: 194
  year: 1994
  ident: 2892_CR41
  publication-title: Human Brain Mapping
  doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010305
  contributor:
    fullname: MS George
– ident: 2892_CR101
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001144
– volume: 20
  start-page: 727
  year: 1992
  ident: 2892_CR111
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03202722
  contributor:
    fullname: J Tzelgov
– volume: 3
  start-page: 367
  year: 2012
  ident: 2892_CR16
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 10
  start-page: 638
  year: 2003
  ident: 2892_CR54
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196526
  contributor:
    fullname: LL Jacoby
– volume: 62
  start-page: 1523
  year: 2009
  ident: 2892_CR32
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470210902752096
  contributor:
    fullname: MJ Crump
– volume: 24
  start-page: 534
  year: 2002
  ident: 2892_CR81
  publication-title: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
  doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.4.534.1033
  contributor:
    fullname: B Ravnkilde
– volume: 28
  start-page: 1437
  year: 2000
  ident: 2892_CR35
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211844
  contributor:
    fullname: M Dishon-Berkovits
– volume: 24
  start-page: 167
  year: 2001
  ident: 2892_CR75
  publication-title: Annual Review of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
  contributor:
    fullname: EK Miller
– volume: 23
  start-page: 769
  year: 2019
  ident: 2892_CR12
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002
  contributor:
    fullname: S Braem
– volume: 5
  start-page: 1327
  year: 2014
  ident: 2892_CR47
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01327
  contributor:
    fullname: E Hazeltine
– volume: 45
  start-page: 11
  year: 1977
  ident: 2892_CR82
  publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills
  doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.45.1.11
  contributor:
    fullname: GM Redding
– volume: 48
  start-page: 426
  year: 2020
  ident: 2892_CR98
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-00980-y
  contributor:
    fullname: G Spinelli
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1792
  year: 2017
  ident: 2892_CR30
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1206130
  contributor:
    fullname: MJ Crump
SSID ssj0063670
Score 2.459641
Snippet Conflict-induced control refers to humans’ ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word...
Conflict-induced control refers to humans' ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the color-word...
Abstract Conflict-induced control refers to humans’ ability to regulate attention in the processing of target information (e.g., the color of a word in the...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
crossref
pubmed
springer
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 1259
SubjectTerms Adolescent
Adult
Attention
Awareness
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Cognitive Psychology
Color Perception
Conflict, Psychological
Executive Function - physiology
Feedback (Response)
Female
Humans
Learning Processes
Male
Orientation
Paradigms
Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology
Psychology
Reaction Time
Stimuli
Stroop Test
Young Adult
Title A spatial version of the Stroop task for examining proactive and reactive control independently from non-conflict processes
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13414-024-02892-9
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38691237
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3056236486
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3050177456
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC11093857
Volume 86
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LT9wwEB4VuHCBPoAupchIvYFh4zgPH1dbHi2CC6wEp8ixHYEo2RXJVgX-fMd2ErSsOHBJFHkSx_Z45rPnYYAf_VyrJC0KGqAkpFykksp-LmlSoL6LdMEibfchz87jkxH_fRVdvcRxO2f31iLpBLVdViKmPahs5jFOUaVQaxxjVCzAUhN4ujQ4vj49bAVwbHOSOWMyDymq_8DHyrzxlVl9NAcy530lXxlMnR46WoXLNprHu5_c7U_rfF89zSd3fE8TP8JKg0vJwDPSJ_hgys-w3InHxy_wPCCVdb9Gqr9-k42MC4L4kVzUCL8npJbVHUEMTMw_ee_OnSAuXssKVCJLTRCf-ofGP57cdmfw1n8eiY10IeW4pKoJVSETH8VgqjUYHR1eDk9oc3QDVajya8rwxgrOhdE2OWQf1y0q0MzE2kSJ5pJzHSRxrnggojw1nOUCJW-BaDJgUjETrsMiVmi-AgmCHDGSFKwIFUfwkUaCM4VIU3PN-qnowW47gNnEZ-jIcGVjezTzPZphj2auRzOk3mrHOGtma5WFDgXGPI17sNMV4zyzxhNZmvHU0aDwShBv9mDDs0RXXZjGAhkx6UE6wywdgc3hPVtS3t64XN424Ss2CV_da3ni5b_ebsbm-8i_wTJzbGUdNbdgsX6Ymu8Ipup8u5k827AwjId4_fnrFK8jNvgPGrQZcg
link.rule.ids 230,315,783,787,888,27936,27937,41093,41132,41535,42162,42201,42604,51588,52123,52246
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8QwEB58HPQivl2fEbxpcJumjxwXUdbnRRe8hbRJcVG7i62i-OedpA9ZxYOnUjJ9TjLzJTPzBeCgm-g0irOMemgJKRexoqqbKBpl6O8CnbFA23XI65uwP-AX98F9XRRWNNnuTUjSWWo7r0RQe1xY6jFO0adQGx1jVEzDrOVXt4z5A9Zr7G9oKclcLJn7FL2_V5XK_HGPSXf0C2P-TpX8ES91buhsERZq_Eh6lcKXYMrkyzDfmrGPFfjskcKmSaPUW7UYRkYZQZxHbkuEyWNSquKRIFYl5l09u_0hiKursoaPqFwTxJHVSZ3HTobtXrnl0wexFSkkH-U0rUtKyLiqNjDFKgzOTu9O-rTeYoGm-OtKyvDAMs6F0ZbEsYvzi9TTzITaBJHminPtRWGSck8ESWw4SwRayAxRn8dUyoy_BjP4QLMBxPMSxDJKsMxPOYKEOBCcpYgINdesG4sOHDZ_Wo4rJg2JMxCrF1npRaJepNOLROntRhmyHlWF9B1aC3kcdmC_bcbxYIMcKjejVyeDRiZCXNiB9Up37eP8OBToqaMOxBNabQUs1_ZkSz58cJzblpgVPwkvPWo6wPd7_f0Zm_8T34O5_t31lbw6v7ncgnnmOqpNrtyGmfLl1ewgACqTXdffvwCQZv1w
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1NT9wwEB1RKlVcKihtWViKkbi1FhvH-fBx1XYFtKyQYCVulhPbKirNrkioivjzjO0kdIs49BRFnsRJxp55zsw8AxyMCl1mubU0QktIucgVVaNC0cyiv0u0ZYl2_yFPp-nRjJ9cJpd_VfH7bPcuJBlqGhxLU9UcLrR1UzxGgHtYOxoyTtG_UBcpY1S8gJfoimKX1Ddj484Wp46ezMeVeUwRCUShbOaZeyy7pid482na5D-xU--SJuvwusWSZByUvwErpnoDa71Ju9uE-zGpXco0Sv0OP8bI3BLEfOS8Qci8II2qfxLErcT8Ub_8XhHE11g5I0hUpQliynDS5rSTq37f3Ob6jrjqFFLNK1q25SVkESoPTP0WZpOvF5-PaLvdAi3RTTeU4YFZzoXRjtBxhGuNMtLMpNokmeaKcx1laVHySCRFbjgrBFpLiwgwYqpkJn4Hq9ih2QISRQXiGiWYjUuOgCFPBGclokPNNRvlYgAfuy8tF4FVQ-JqxOlFBr1I1Iv0epEoPeyUIdsZVsvYI7eU5-kA9vtmnBsu4KEqM7_1MmhwMsSIA3gfdNd3F-epQK-dDSBf0mov4Hi3l1uqqx-ef9uRtOIr4aWfugHw-FzPv8b2_4nvwauzLxP5_Xj6bQfWmB-nLs9yCKvNza3ZRSzUFB_8cH8AC6IBxA
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=A+spatial+version+of+the+Stroop+task+for+examining+proactive+and+reactive+control+independently+from+non-conflict+processes&rft.jtitle=Attention%2C+perception+%26+psychophysics&rft.au=Spinelli%2C+Giacomo&rft.au=Lupker%2C+Stephen+J&rft.date=2024-05-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.issn=1943-3921&rft.eissn=1943-393X&rft.volume=86&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=1259&rft.epage=1286&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13414-024-02892-9&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1943-3921&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1943-3921&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1943-3921&client=summon