The Neuronal Correlates of Indeterminate Sentence Comprehension: An fMRI Study
Sentences such as are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject ( ) started doing with the object ( ). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by , a linguistic proces...
Saved in:
Published in | Frontiers in human neuroscience Vol. 10; p. 614 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Switzerland
Frontiers Research Foundation
20.12.2016
Frontiers Media S.A |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1662-5161 1662-5161 |
DOI | 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00614 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Sentences such as
are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (
) started doing with the object (
). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by
, a linguistic process that forces the noun
to be interpreted as an
(e.g.,
) or by a process that interpolates this activity information in the resulting enriched semantic composition. An alternative theory,
, assumes classical semantic composition, whereby meaning arises from the denotation of words and how they are combined syntactically, with enrichment obtained via pragmatic inferences beyond linguistic-semantic processes. Cognitive neuroscience studies investigating the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of indeterminate sentences have shown activations either at the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vmPFC) or at the left inferior frontal gyrus (L-IFG). These studies have supported the semantic coercion theory assuming that one of these regions is where enriched semantic composition takes place. Employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we found that indeterminate sentences activate bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the right inferior frontal gyrus (R-IFG), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), more so than control sentences (
). Activation of indeterminate sentences exceeded that of anomalous sentences (…
) and engaged more left- and right-hemisphere areas than other sentence types. We suggest that the widespread activations for indeterminate sentences represent the deployment of pragmatic-inferential processes, which seek to enrich sentence content without necessarily resorting to semantic coercion. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Sentences such as
are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (
) started doing with the object (
). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by
, a linguistic process that forces the noun
to be interpreted as an
(e.g.,
) or by a process that interpolates this activity information in the resulting enriched semantic composition. An alternative theory,
, assumes classical semantic composition, whereby meaning arises from the denotation of words and how they are combined syntactically, with enrichment obtained via pragmatic inferences beyond linguistic-semantic processes. Cognitive neuroscience studies investigating the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of indeterminate sentences have shown activations either at the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vmPFC) or at the left inferior frontal gyrus (L-IFG). These studies have supported the semantic coercion theory assuming that one of these regions is where enriched semantic composition takes place. Employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we found that indeterminate sentences activate bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the right inferior frontal gyrus (R-IFG), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), more so than control sentences (
). Activation of indeterminate sentences exceeded that of anomalous sentences (…
) and engaged more left- and right-hemisphere areas than other sentence types. We suggest that the widespread activations for indeterminate sentences represent the deployment of pragmatic-inferential processes, which seek to enrich sentence content without necessarily resorting to semantic coercion. Sentences such as The author started the book are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (the author) started doing with the object (the book). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by semanticcoercion, a linguistic process that forces the noun book to be interpreted as an activity (e.g., writing the book) or by a process that interpolates this activity information in the resulting enriched semantic composition. An alternative theory, pragmatic, assumes classical semantic composition, whereby meaning arises from the denotation of words and how they are combined syntactically, with enrichment obtained via pragmatic inferences beyond linguistic-semantic processes. Cognitive neuroscience studies investigating the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of indeterminate sentences have shown activations either at the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vmPFC) or at the left inferior frontal gyrus (L-IFG). These studies have supported the semantic coercion theory assuming that one of these regions is where enriched semantic composition takes place. Employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we found that indeterminate sentences activate bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the right inferior frontal gyrus (R-IFG), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), more so than control sentences (The author wrote the book). Activation of indeterminate sentences exceeded that of anomalous sentences (…drank the book) and engaged more left- and right-hemisphere areas than other sentence types. We suggest that the widespread activations for indeterminate sentences represent the deployment of pragmatic-inferential processes, which seek to enrich sentence content without necessarily resorting to semantic coercion.Sentences such as The author started the book are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (the author) started doing with the object (the book). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by semanticcoercion, a linguistic process that forces the noun book to be interpreted as an activity (e.g., writing the book) or by a process that interpolates this activity information in the resulting enriched semantic composition. An alternative theory, pragmatic, assumes classical semantic composition, whereby meaning arises from the denotation of words and how they are combined syntactically, with enrichment obtained via pragmatic inferences beyond linguistic-semantic processes. Cognitive neuroscience studies investigating the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of indeterminate sentences have shown activations either at the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vmPFC) or at the left inferior frontal gyrus (L-IFG). These studies have supported the semantic coercion theory assuming that one of these regions is where enriched semantic composition takes place. Employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we found that indeterminate sentences activate bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the right inferior frontal gyrus (R-IFG), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), more so than control sentences (The author wrote the book). Activation of indeterminate sentences exceeded that of anomalous sentences (…drank the book) and engaged more left- and right-hemisphere areas than other sentence types. We suggest that the widespread activations for indeterminate sentences represent the deployment of pragmatic-inferential processes, which seek to enrich sentence content without necessarily resorting to semantic coercion. Sentences such as The author started the book are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject ( the author ) started doing with the object ( the book ). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by semantic coercion , a linguistic process that forces the noun book to be interpreted as an activity (e.g., writing the book ) or by a process that interpolates this activity information in the resulting enriched semantic composition. An alternative theory, pragmatic , assumes classical semantic composition, whereby meaning arises from the denotation of words and how they are combined syntactically, with enrichment obtained via pragmatic inferences beyond linguistic-semantic processes. Cognitive neuroscience studies investigating the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of indeterminate sentences have shown activations either at the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vmPFC) or at the left inferior frontal gyrus (L-IFG). These studies have supported the semantic coercion theory assuming that one of these regions is where enriched semantic composition takes place. Employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we found that indeterminate sentences activate bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the right inferior frontal gyrus (R-IFG), and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), more so than control sentences ( The author wrote the book ). Activation of indeterminate sentences exceeded that of anomalous sentences (… drank the book ) and engaged more left- and right-hemisphere areas than other sentence types. We suggest that the widespread activations for indeterminate sentences represent the deployment of pragmatic-inferential processes, which seek to enrich sentence content without necessarily resorting to semantic coercion. Sentences such as "The author started the book" are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (the author) started doing with the object (the book). In principle, indeterminate sentences allow for an infinite number of interpretations. One theory, however, assumes that these sentences are resolved by semantic coercion, a linguistic process that forces the noun "book" to be interpreted as an activity (e.g., writing the book) or by a process that interpolates this activity information in the resulting enriched semantic composition. An alternative theory, pragmatic, assumes classical semantic composition, whereby meaning arises from the denotation of words and how they are combined syntactically, with enrichment obtained via pragmatic inferences beyond linguistic-semantic processes. Cognitive neuroscience studies investigating the neuroanatomical and functional correlates of indeterminate sentences have shown activations either at the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex or at the left inferior frontal gyrus. These studies have supported the semantic coercion theory assuming that one of these regions is where enriched semantic composition takes place. Employing fMRI, we found that indeterminate sentences activate bilaterally the superior temporal gyrus, the right inferior frontal gyrus, and the anterior cingulate cortex, more so than control sentences ("The author wrote the book"). Activation of indeterminate sentences exceeded that of anomalous sentences ("…drank the book") and engaged more left- and right-hemisphere areas than other sentence types. We suggest that the widespread activations for indeterminate sentences represent the deployment of pragmatic-inferential processes, which seek to enrich sentence content without necessarily resorting to semantic coercion. |
Author | Gillon, Brendan Jarema, Gonia Riven, Levi Dwivedi, Veena D. Manouilidou, Christina Lungu, Ovidiu de Almeida, Roberto G. |
AuthorAffiliation | 5 Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Université de Montréal Montreal, QC, Canada 3 Unité de Neuroimagerie Fonctionnelle, Institute Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Université de Montréal Montreal, QC, Canada 4 Department of Applied Linguistics, Brock University St. Catharines, ON, Canada 1 Department of Psychology, Concordia University Montreal, QC, Canada 2 Department of Comparative and General Linguistics, University of Ljubljana Ljubljana, Slovenia 6 Department of Linguistics, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 1 Department of Psychology, Concordia University Montreal, QC, Canada – name: 3 Unité de Neuroimagerie Fonctionnelle, Institute Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Université de Montréal Montreal, QC, Canada – name: 4 Department of Applied Linguistics, Brock University St. Catharines, ON, Canada – name: 5 Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Université de Montréal Montreal, QC, Canada – name: 2 Department of Comparative and General Linguistics, University of Ljubljana Ljubljana, Slovenia – name: 6 Department of Linguistics, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Roberto G. surname: de Almeida fullname: de Almeida, Roberto G. – sequence: 2 givenname: Levi surname: Riven fullname: Riven, Levi – sequence: 3 givenname: Christina surname: Manouilidou fullname: Manouilidou, Christina – sequence: 4 givenname: Ovidiu surname: Lungu fullname: Lungu, Ovidiu – sequence: 5 givenname: Veena D. surname: Dwivedi fullname: Dwivedi, Veena D. – sequence: 6 givenname: Gonia surname: Jarema fullname: Jarema, Gonia – sequence: 7 givenname: Brendan surname: Gillon fullname: Gillon, Brendan |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28066204$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp1kUtLAzEUhYMo1tfelQy4cdOaZJLMjAuhFB-FquBjHdLJjR2ZSWoyI_jvTWuVWnCVkPvdw8k5-2jbOgsIHRM8SNO8ODd21jUDiokYYCwI20J7RAja50SQ7bV7D-2H8BYRKjjZRT2a4zjCbA_dP88guYfOO6vqZOS8h1q1EBJnkrHV0IJvKhtfkiewLdgSItTMPczAhsrZi2RoE3P3OE6e2k5_HqIdo-oAR6vzAL1cXz2PbvuTh5vxaDjpl4yytj81Jis0M0qTTGjMsrzIsaK0FJpTQ7KMG5IyYsxUFSkVuRFaYaUxFyTTrID0AF1-6867aQO6jN68quXcV43yn9KpSv6d2GomX92HjGnkgokocLYS8O69g9DKpgol1LWy4LogSc4jyHmOI3q6gb65zse4gqS0wJHg2ULwZN3Rr5WfqCOAv4HSuxA8mF-EYLloUy7blIs25bLNuCI2VsqqVW2MPf6pqv9f_AKqeaUJ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2025_02_050 crossref_primary_10_1093_brain_awy270 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_741685 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00429_020_02196_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lingua_2024_103754 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13388 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_616065 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhaa222 |
Cites_doi | 10.1515/9783110226614.574 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01839-8 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1905 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.07.001 10.1075/la.194.13alm 10.1093/cercor/13.2.170 10.1093/oso/9780199252152.003.0006 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.001 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.07.015 10.1006/brln.2001.2492 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.04.003 10.1162/089892900562138 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.07.005 10.1073/pnas.0810928106 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00113-x 10.1111/j.1749-818x.2009.00143.x 10.1037/0894-4105.14.3.361 10.1162/08989290260045800 10.1016/0093-934x(86)90022-2 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00438-3 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_49 10.1353/cjl.0.0026 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.02.002 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002 10.1007/978-3-642-59794-7_7 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.002 10.1037/10693-000 10.1093/oso/9780195136975.003.0010 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.004 10.1093/cercor/bhp055 10.1002/9780470754603 10.7551/mitpress/3225.001.0001 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011 10.1006/nimg.1999.0493 10.1006/brcg.1999.1088 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.009 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.011 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.06.006 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00353.1997 10.1075/ml.7.1.03kat 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00099-x 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.048 10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.002 10.1162/08989290051137648 10.1038/nrn2113 10.1006/brln.1996.0024 10.1162/jocn.2009.21333 10.1111/j.1749-818x.2008.00073.x 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145128 10.1007/978-3-319-10112-5_1 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.08.001 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.02.053 10.1006/nimg.1997.0289 10.1093/brain/117.6.1241 10.1038/nrn2532 10.1075/slcs.152.05dol 10.33588/rn.4608.2008079 10.1162/jocn.2009.21303 10.1002/hbm.20070 10.1016/j.lingua.2004.01.013 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.10.010 10.3115/997939.997947 10.1002/hbm.20249 10.1162/jocn_a_00040 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.021 10.1002/hbm.20422 10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00021-9 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.004 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2016. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. Copyright © 2016 de Almeida, Riven, Manouilidou, Lungu, Dwivedi, Jarema and Gillon. 2016 de Almeida, Riven, Manouilidou, Lungu, Dwivedi, Jarema and Gillon |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2016. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: Copyright © 2016 de Almeida, Riven, Manouilidou, Lungu, Dwivedi, Jarema and Gillon. 2016 de Almeida, Riven, Manouilidou, Lungu, Dwivedi, Jarema and Gillon |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 3V. 7XB 88I 8FE 8FH 8FK ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BBNVY BENPR BHPHI CCPQU DWQXO GNUQQ HCIFZ LK8 M2P M7P PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PKEHL PQEST PQGLB PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS Q9U 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00614 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed ProQuest Central (Corporate) ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland ProQuest Central Essentials Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Natural Science Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central ProQuest Central Student SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest Biological Science Collection Science Database Biological Science Database ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central Basic MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Central Student ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central Korea Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Science Journals ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition Biological Science Database ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | PubMed MEDLINE - Academic Publicly Available Content Database |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1662-5161 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC5168646 28066204 10_3389_fnhum_2016_00614 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | Canada Montreal Quebec Canada |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: Canada – name: Montreal Quebec Canada |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada – fundername: Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada |
GroupedDBID | --- 29H 2WC 53G 5GY 5VS 88I 8FE 8FH 9T4 AAFWJ AAYXX ABIVO ABUWG ACGFO ACGFS ACXDI ADBBV ADRAZ AEGXH AENEX AFKRA AFPKN AIAGR ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS AZQEC BAWUL BBNVY BCNDV BENPR BHPHI BPHCQ CCPQU CITATION CS3 DIK DU5 DWQXO E3Z EMOBN F5P GNUQQ GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HCIFZ HYE KQ8 LK8 M2P M48 M7P M~E O5R O5S OK1 OVT PGMZT PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC RNS RPM TR2 C1A IAO IEA IHR IHW IPNFZ IPY ISR NPM RIG 3V. 7XB 8FK PKEHL PQEST PQGLB PQUKI PRINS Q9U 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c424t-bff79d4fad176d0478980a22c6d52f1775f1341ffba93268f6da0ad05617d49e3 |
IEDL.DBID | BENPR |
ISSN | 1662-5161 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:32:38 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 12:42:49 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 11:47:32 EDT 2025 Thu Jan 02 22:25:21 EST 2025 Thu Jul 03 08:32:55 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:12:43 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | fMRI indeterminate sentences inferior frontal gyrus compositionality pragmatics semantic coercion anterior cingulate cortex superior temporal gyrus |
Language | English |
License | This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c424t-bff79d4fad176d0478980a22c6d52f1775f1341ffba93268f6da0ad05617d49e3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Reviewed by: Frank van der Velde, University of Twente, Netherlands; Lars Kuchinke, International Psychoanalytic University Berlin, Germany Edited by: Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, USA |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.proquest.com/docview/2290803576?pq-origsite=%requestingapplication% |
PMID | 28066204 |
PQID | 2290803576 |
PQPubID | 4424408 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5168646 proquest_miscellaneous_1856865580 proquest_journals_2290803576 pubmed_primary_28066204 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2016_00614 crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_fnhum_2016_00614 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2016-12-20 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-12-20 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 12 year: 2016 text: 2016-12-20 day: 20 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Switzerland |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Switzerland – name: Lausanne |
PublicationTitle | Frontiers in human neuroscience |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Front Hum Neurosci |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Frontiers Research Foundation Frontiers Media S.A |
Publisher_xml | – name: Frontiers Research Foundation – name: Frontiers Media S.A |
References | Ni (B58) 2000; 12 Grice (B37) 1989 Stringaris (B76) 2007; 100 Brennan (B10) 2008; 106 Pustejovsky (B65) 1995 de Almeida (B20) 2012 Fiebach (B28) 2005; 24 Gernsbacher (B33) 2003; 54 Kang (B46) 1999; 10 Katsika (B47) 2012; 7 Champagne (B14) 2003; 53 Husband (B40) 2011; 23 Bohrn (B7) 2012; 50 Champagne-Lavau (B15) 2009; 22 de Swart (B22) 2011 Szabó (B78) 2012 Binder (B6) 2011; 54 Lau (B51) 2008; 9 Binder (B5) 1997; 17 Rapp (B69) 2011; 119 Seger (B73) 2000; 14 Jung-Beeman (B44) 2005; 9 Rapp (B70) 2007; 100 de Almeida (B18) 2008; 53 Stromswold (B77) 1996; 52 Dronkers (B24) 2004; 92 Kuperberg (B49) 2000; 12 Ojemann (B59) 1997; 6 Schmidt (B71) 2009; 71 Briscoe (B11) 1990 Egg (B25) 2005 Friederici (B30) 2002; 6 Damasio (B17) 2004; 92 McElree (B56) 2001; 78 Baggio (B1) 2010; 22 Traxler (B80) 2005; 53 Friederici (B31) 2011; 91 Friederici (B32) 2003; 13 Pickering (B63) 2005; 93 Kline (B48) 2004 Bookheimer (B8) 2002; 25 Bottini (B9) 1994; 117 Traxler (B81) 2002; 47 Stemmer (B74) 2016 Brownell (B12) 1986; 27 de Almeida (B21) 2004; 90 Hickock (B38) 2007; 8 Pylkkänen (B66) 2008; 2 Ferstl (B27) 2008; 29 de Almeida (B19) 2015 Margulies (B54) 2007; 37 Berwick (B3) 2013; 17 Pylkkänen (B67) 2007; 19 Stowe (B75) 2005; 115 Virtue (B83) 2006; 1084 Carston (B13) 2002 Fodor (B29) 2002 Joanette (B43) 2008; 46 Leitão (B52) 2010 Indefrey (B41) 2004 Goddard (B35) 1993 de Almeida (B220) in press McElree (B55) 2006; 30 Partee (B60) 1984 Crosson (B16) 1999; 40 Kacinik (B45) 2007; 100 Goebel (B36) 2006; 27 Meyer (B57) 2009 Fedorenko (B26) 2009; 3 Kuperberg (B50) 2010; 22 Schneider (B72) 2002 Talairach (B79) 1988 Hobbs (B39) 2004 Piñango (B64) 2001; 79 Bambini (B2) 2011; 86 Vandenberghe (B82) 2002; 14 Jackendoff (B42) 1997 Gillon (B34) 2004 Dölling (B23) 2014 Partee (B61) 1986 Binder (B4) 2009; 19 Wahl (B84) 2008; 59 Penny (B62) 2003 Makuuchi (B53) 2009; 106 Pylkkänen (B68) 2009; 108 15037130 - Cognition. 2004 May-Jun;92 (1-2):179-229 16596654 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2006 May;27(5):392-401 10413568 - Brain Cogn. 1999 Jul;40(2):414-38 12359916 - Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:91-114 21557650 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2011 Nov;23(11):3254-66 8987760 - J Neurosci. 1997 Jan 1;17(1):353-62 23313359 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Feb;17(2):89-98 22013214 - Physiol Rev. 2011 Oct;91(4):1357-92 11062325 - Cognition. 2001 Jan;78(1):B17-25 12126497 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 May 15;14 (4):550-60 15037129 - Cognition. 2004 May-Jun;92(1-2):145-77 19586700 - Brain Cogn. 2009 Dec;71(3):375-86 15866191 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2002 Feb 1;6(2):78-84 18786354 - Neuron. 2008 Sep 11;59(5):695-707 16574079 - Brain Res. 2006 Apr 21;1084(1):104-14 15455462 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2005 Feb;24(2):79-91 22824234 - Neuropsychologia. 2012 Sep;50(11):2669-83 3955344 - Brain Lang. 1986 Mar;27(2):310-21 21803125 - Brain Res Bull. 2011 Oct 10;86(3-4):203-16 15766763 - Brain Lang. 2005 Apr;93(1):1-9 19702471 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Dec;22(12):2685-701 10769310 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Jan;12(1):120-33 20884358 - Neuroimage. 2011 Jan 15;54(2):1465-75 19329570 - Cereb Cortex. 2009 Dec;19(12):2767-96 9344820 - Neuroimage. 1997 Oct;6(3):156-67 16677700 - Brain Lang. 2007 Feb;100(2):142-9 18678402 - Brain Lang. 2009 Mar;108(3):184-90 17431404 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007 May;8(5):393-402 21889196 - Brain Lang. 2011 Dec;119(3):196-205 26925175 - Ment Lex. 2012;7(1):58-76 10547332 - Neuroimage. 1999 Nov;10(5):555-61 7820563 - Brain. 1994 Dec;117 ( Pt 6):1241-53 16325253 - Brain Lang. 2007 Feb;100(2):188-207 16165201 - Brain Lang. 2007 Feb;100(2):150-62 10928739 - Neuropsychology. 2000 Jul;14(3):361-9 16214387 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Nov;9(11):512-8 19416819 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 May 19;106(20):8362-7 12052907 - Annu Rev Neurosci. 2002;25:151-88 14607137 - Brain Cogn. 2003 Nov;53(2):152-7 17604651 - Neuroimage. 2007 Aug 15;37(2):579-88 10771415 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Mar;12(2):321-41 15172543 - Brain Lang. 2004 Jul-Sep;90(1-3):249-61 8653390 - Brain Lang. 1996 Mar;52(3):452-73 11712848 - Brain Lang. 2001 Nov;79(2):297-308 17557297 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2008 May;29(5):581-93 17958491 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2007 Nov;19(11):1905-21 21702813 - Cogn Sci. 2006 Jan 2;30(1):181-92 19020511 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008 Dec;9(12 ):920-33 18428106 - Rev Neurol. 2008 Apr 16-30;46(8):481-8 12507948 - Cereb Cortex. 2003 Feb;13(2):170-7 19583469 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Sep;22(9):2131-40 18561993 - Brain Lang. 2008 Aug;106(2):132-43 |
References_xml | – start-page: 535 volume-title: Semantics: An International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning year: 2011 ident: B22 article-title: Mismatches and coercion doi: 10.1515/9783110226614.574 – volume: 6 start-page: 78 year: 2002 ident: B30 article-title: Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01839-8 – volume: 19 start-page: 1905 year: 2007 ident: B67 article-title: An MEG study of silent meaning publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1905 – volume: 92 start-page: 179 year: 2004 ident: B17 article-title: Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.07.001 – start-page: 277 volume-title: Towards a Biolinguistic Understanding of Grammar year: 2012 ident: B20 article-title: Indeterminacy and coercion effects: minimal representations with pragmatic enrichment doi: 10.1075/la.194.13alm – volume-title: The Architecture of the Language Faculty. year: 1997 ident: B42 – volume: 13 start-page: 170 year: 2003 ident: B32 article-title: The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes publication-title: Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.2.170 – start-page: 89 volume-title: The Compositionality Papers year: 2002 ident: B29 article-title: The emptiness of the lexicon: reflections on Pustejovsky doi: 10.1093/oso/9780199252152.003.0006 – volume: 71 start-page: 375 year: 2009 ident: B71 article-title: Neural correlates of metaphor processing: the roles of figurativeness, familiarity and difficulty publication-title: Brain Cogn. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.001 – volume: 86 start-page: 203 year: 2011 ident: B2 article-title: Decomposing metaphor processing at the cognitive and neural level trough functional magnatic resonance imaging publication-title: Brain Res. Bull. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.07.015 – volume: 79 start-page: 297 year: 2001 ident: B64 article-title: Semantic operations in aphasic comprehension: implications for the cortical organization of language publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2492 – volume: 106 start-page: 132 year: 2008 ident: B10 article-title: Processing events: behavioral and neuromagnetic correlates of Aspectual Coercion publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.04.003 – volume: 12 start-page: 321 year: 2000 ident: B49 article-title: Common and distinct neural substrates for pragmatic, semantic and syntactic processing of spoken sentences: an fMRI study publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/089892900562138 – volume: 93 start-page: 1 year: 2005 ident: B63 article-title: The difficulty of coercion: a response to de Almeida publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.07.005 – volume: 106 start-page: 8362 year: 2009 ident: B53 article-title: Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810928106 – volume: 78 start-page: B17 year: 2001 ident: B56 article-title: Reading time evidence for enriched composition publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00113-x – volume: 3 start-page: 839 year: 2009 ident: B26 article-title: Neuroimaging of language: why hasn’t a clearer picture emerged? publication-title: Lang. Linguist. Compass doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818x.2009.00143.x – volume: 14 start-page: 361 year: 2000 ident: B73 article-title: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for right-hemisphere involvement in processing unusual semantic relationships publication-title: Neuropsychology doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.14.3.361 – volume: 14 start-page: 550 year: 2002 ident: B82 article-title: The response of the left temporal cortex to sentences publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045800 – volume: 27 start-page: 310 year: 1986 ident: B12 article-title: Inference deficits in right brain-damaged patients publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(86)90022-2 – volume: 90 start-page: 249 year: 2004 ident: B21 article-title: The effect of context on the processing of type-shifting verbs publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00438-3 – volume: 30 start-page: 181 year: 2006 ident: B55 article-title: Deferred interpretations: why starting Dickens is taxing but reading Dickens isn’t publication-title: Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_49 – volume-title: E-Prime User’s Guide. year: 2002 ident: B72 – volume: 53 start-page: 301 year: 2008 ident: B18 article-title: Coercion without lexical decomposition: type-shifting effects revisited publication-title: Can. J. Linguist. doi: 10.1353/cjl.0.0026 – volume-title: The Oxford Handbook of Pragmatics year: 2016 ident: B74 article-title: Neuropragmatics – volume-title: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. year: 1988 ident: B79 – volume: 22 start-page: 413 year: 2009 ident: B15 article-title: Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: different patterns of deficits publication-title: J. Neurolinguistics doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.02.002 – volume: 92 start-page: 145 year: 2004 ident: B24 article-title: Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002 – volume: 37 start-page: 579 year: 2007 ident: B54 article-title: Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-59794-7_7 – volume: 17 start-page: 89 year: 2013 ident: B3 article-title: Evolution, brain and the nature of language publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.002 – volume-title: Beyond Significance Testing: Reforming Data Analysis Methods in Behavioural Research. year: 2004 ident: B48 doi: 10.1037/10693-000 – start-page: 157 volume-title: Semantics: A Reader year: 2004 ident: B34 article-title: Ambiguity, indeterminacy, deixis and vagueness: evidence and theory doi: 10.1093/oso/9780195136975.003.0010 – volume: 100 start-page: 142 year: 2007 ident: B70 article-title: Laterality in metaphor processing: lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.004 – volume: 19 start-page: 2767 year: 2009 ident: B4 article-title: Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies publication-title: Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055 – volume-title: Thoughts and Utterances. year: 2002 ident: B13 doi: 10.1002/9780470754603 – start-page: 168 volume-title: Proceedings of the 6th Conference of the European chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics year: 1993 ident: B35 article-title: Towards a proper treatment of coercion phenomena – volume-title: The Generative Lexicon. year: 1995 ident: B65 doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3225.001.0001 – volume: 91 start-page: 1357 year: 2011 ident: B31 article-title: The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function publication-title: Physiol. Rev. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011 – volume: 10 start-page: 555 year: 1999 ident: B46 article-title: An event-related fMRI study of implicit phrase-level syntactic and semantic processing publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0493 – volume: 40 start-page: 414 year: 1999 ident: B16 article-title: Subcortical mechanisms in language: lexical-semantic mechanisms and the thalamus publication-title: Brain Cogn. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1088 – start-page: 724 volume-title: The Handbook of Pragmatics year: 2004 ident: B39 article-title: Abduction in natural language understanding – volume: 9 start-page: 512 year: 2005 ident: B44 article-title: Bilateral brain processes for comprehending naural language publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.009 – volume: 59 start-page: 695 year: 2008 ident: B84 article-title: The human thalamus processes syntactic and semantic violations publication-title: Neuron doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.011 – volume: 108 start-page: 184 year: 2009 ident: B68 article-title: The anterior midline field: coercion or decision making? publication-title: Int. J. Cardiol. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.06.006 – volume: 17 start-page: 353 year: 1997 ident: B5 article-title: Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging publication-title: J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00353.1997 – volume: 7 start-page: 58 year: 2012 ident: B47 article-title: Complement coercion: distinguishing between type-shifting and pragmatic inferencing publication-title: Ment. Lex. doi: 10.1075/ml.7.1.03kat – volume: 53 start-page: 152 year: 2003 ident: B14 article-title: Impact of right hemispheric damage on a hierarchy of complexity evidenced in young normal subjects publication-title: Brain Cogn. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00099-x – volume: 54 start-page: 1465 year: 2011 ident: B6 article-title: Mapping the anterior temporal lobe language areas with fMRI: a multicenter normative study publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.048 – start-page: 843 volume-title: Human Brain Function year: 2003 ident: B62 article-title: Random-effects analysis – volume: 53 start-page: 1 year: 2005 ident: B80 article-title: Context effects in coercion: evidence from eye movements publication-title: J. Mem. Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.002 – volume: 12 start-page: 120 year: 2000 ident: B58 article-title: An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137648 – volume-title: On Concepts, Modules, and Language year: in press ident: B220 article-title: Semantics for a module – volume: 8 start-page: 393 year: 2007 ident: B38 article-title: The cortical organization of language publication-title: Nat. Rev. Neurosci. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113 – volume: 52 start-page: 452 year: 1996 ident: B77 article-title: Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0024 – start-page: 283 volume-title: Quantitative and Experimental Linguistics year: 2009 ident: B57 article-title: Neuroimaging of speech and language – volume: 22 start-page: 2685 year: 2010 ident: B50 article-title: Electrophysiological correlates of complement coercion publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21333 – start-page: 1 volume-title: Studies in the Way of Words year: 1989 ident: B37 article-title: Logic and conversation (1967/1987) – volume: 2 start-page: 712 year: 2008 ident: B66 article-title: Mismatching meanings in brain and behaviour publication-title: Lang. Linguist. Compass doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818x.2008.00073.x – volume: 54 start-page: 91 year: 2003 ident: B33 article-title: Neuroimaging studies of language production and comprehension publication-title: Annu. Rev. Psychol. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145128 – start-page: 3 volume-title: Cognitive Science Perspectives on Verb Representation and Processing year: 2015 ident: B19 article-title: The study of verbs in cognitive science doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-10112-5_1 – start-page: 203 volume-title: Compositionality in Formal Semantics year: 1986 ident: B61 article-title: Noun phrase interpretation and type-shifting principles – volume: 100 start-page: 150 year: 2007 ident: B76 article-title: Deriving meaning: distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal and non-meaningful sentences publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.08.001 – volume: 1084 start-page: 104 year: 2006 ident: B83 article-title: Neural activity of inferences during story comprehension publication-title: Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.02.053 – volume: 6 start-page: 156 year: 1997 ident: B59 article-title: Anatomic localization and quantitative analysis of gradient refocused echo-planar fMRI susceptibility artifacts publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0289 – start-page: 153 volume-title: Compositionality in Formal Semantics year: 1984 ident: B60 article-title: Compositionality – start-page: 759 volume-title: The Cognitive Neurosciences year: 2004 ident: B41 article-title: Prelexical and lexical processing in listening – volume: 117 start-page: 1241 year: 1994 ident: B9 article-title: The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language: a positron emission tomography activation study publication-title: Brain doi: 10.1093/brain/117.6.1241 – volume: 9 start-page: 920 year: 2008 ident: B51 article-title: A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400 publication-title: Nat. Rev. Neurosci. doi: 10.1038/nrn2532 – start-page: 113 volume-title: Events, Arguments and Aspects: Topics in the Semantics of Verbs year: 2014 ident: B23 article-title: Aspectual coercion and eventuality structure doi: 10.1075/slcs.152.05dol – volume: 46 start-page: 481 year: 2008 ident: B43 article-title: Impacto de las lesiones del hemisferio derecho sobre las habilidades lingüísticas: perspectivas teórica y clínica [The impact of right-hemisphere lesions on linguistic abilities: theoretical and clinical perspectives] publication-title: Rev. Neurol. doi: 10.33588/rn.4608.2008079 – start-page: 64 volume-title: The Oxford Handbook of Compositionality year: 2012 ident: B78 article-title: The case for compositionality – volume: 22 start-page: 2131 year: 2010 ident: B1 article-title: Coercion and compositionality publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21303 – volume-title: Flexible Semantics for Reinterpretation Phenomena. year: 2005 ident: B25 – volume: 24 start-page: 79 year: 2005 ident: B28 article-title: Revisiting the role of Broca’s area in sentence processing: syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory publication-title: Hum. Brain Mapp. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20070 – volume: 115 start-page: 997 year: 2005 ident: B75 article-title: Rethinking the neurological basis of language publication-title: Lingua doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2004.01.013 – volume: 25 start-page: 151 year: 2002 ident: B8 article-title: Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing publication-title: Annu. Rev. Neurosci. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946 – volume: 100 start-page: 188 year: 2007 ident: B45 article-title: Understanding metaphors: is the right hemisphere uniquely involved? publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.10.010 – start-page: 42 volume-title: Proceedings of 13th International Conference on Computational Linguistics year: 1990 ident: B11 article-title: Enjoy the paper: lexical semantics via lexicology doi: 10.3115/997939.997947 – volume: 27 start-page: 392 year: 2006 ident: B36 article-title: Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with Brainvoyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis publication-title: Hum. Brain Mapp. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20249 – start-page: 151 volume-title: Papers in Psycholinguistics: Proceedings of the First International Psycholinguistics Congress year: 2010 ident: B52 article-title: Looking for coercion effects in Brazilian Portuguese – volume: 23 start-page: 3254 year: 2011 ident: B40 article-title: Using complement coercion to understand the neural basis of semantic composition: evidence from an fMRI study publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00040 – volume: 50 start-page: 2669 year: 2012 ident: B7 article-title: Looking at the brains behind figurative language—a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on metaphor, idiom and irony processing publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.021 – volume: 29 start-page: 581 year: 2008 ident: B27 article-title: The extended language network: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension publication-title: Hum. Brain Mapp. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20422 – volume: 47 start-page: 530 year: 2002 ident: B81 article-title: Coercion in sentence processing: evidence from eye movements and self-paced reading publication-title: J. Mem. Lang. doi: 10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00021-9 – volume: 119 start-page: 196 year: 2011 ident: B69 article-title: Neural correlates of metonymy resolution publication-title: Brain Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.004 – reference: 19702471 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Dec;22(12):2685-701 – reference: 16596654 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2006 May;27(5):392-401 – reference: 10771415 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Mar;12(2):321-41 – reference: 18428106 - Rev Neurol. 2008 Apr 16-30;46(8):481-8 – reference: 19583469 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Sep;22(9):2131-40 – reference: 11712848 - Brain Lang. 2001 Nov;79(2):297-308 – reference: 10928739 - Neuropsychology. 2000 Jul;14(3):361-9 – reference: 17604651 - Neuroimage. 2007 Aug 15;37(2):579-88 – reference: 23313359 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Feb;17(2):89-98 – reference: 10413568 - Brain Cogn. 1999 Jul;40(2):414-38 – reference: 16574079 - Brain Res. 2006 Apr 21;1084(1):104-14 – reference: 21889196 - Brain Lang. 2011 Dec;119(3):196-205 – reference: 11062325 - Cognition. 2001 Jan;78(1):B17-25 – reference: 17557297 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2008 May;29(5):581-93 – reference: 12507948 - Cereb Cortex. 2003 Feb;13(2):170-7 – reference: 15766763 - Brain Lang. 2005 Apr;93(1):1-9 – reference: 15037130 - Cognition. 2004 May-Jun;92 (1-2):179-229 – reference: 15037129 - Cognition. 2004 May-Jun;92(1-2):145-77 – reference: 9344820 - Neuroimage. 1997 Oct;6(3):156-67 – reference: 21803125 - Brain Res Bull. 2011 Oct 10;86(3-4):203-16 – reference: 20884358 - Neuroimage. 2011 Jan 15;54(2):1465-75 – reference: 19329570 - Cereb Cortex. 2009 Dec;19(12):2767-96 – reference: 17431404 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007 May;8(5):393-402 – reference: 19416819 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 May 19;106(20):8362-7 – reference: 17958491 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2007 Nov;19(11):1905-21 – reference: 19586700 - Brain Cogn. 2009 Dec;71(3):375-86 – reference: 12359916 - Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:91-114 – reference: 15172543 - Brain Lang. 2004 Jul-Sep;90(1-3):249-61 – reference: 22824234 - Neuropsychologia. 2012 Sep;50(11):2669-83 – reference: 12126497 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 May 15;14 (4):550-60 – reference: 16325253 - Brain Lang. 2007 Feb;100(2):188-207 – reference: 10769310 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Jan;12(1):120-33 – reference: 21702813 - Cogn Sci. 2006 Jan 2;30(1):181-92 – reference: 18561993 - Brain Lang. 2008 Aug;106(2):132-43 – reference: 16677700 - Brain Lang. 2007 Feb;100(2):142-9 – reference: 18786354 - Neuron. 2008 Sep 11;59(5):695-707 – reference: 16165201 - Brain Lang. 2007 Feb;100(2):150-62 – reference: 26925175 - Ment Lex. 2012;7(1):58-76 – reference: 3955344 - Brain Lang. 1986 Mar;27(2):310-21 – reference: 15866191 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2002 Feb 1;6(2):78-84 – reference: 18678402 - Brain Lang. 2009 Mar;108(3):184-90 – reference: 7820563 - Brain. 1994 Dec;117 ( Pt 6):1241-53 – reference: 10547332 - Neuroimage. 1999 Nov;10(5):555-61 – reference: 8987760 - J Neurosci. 1997 Jan 1;17(1):353-62 – reference: 15455462 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2005 Feb;24(2):79-91 – reference: 19020511 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008 Dec;9(12 ):920-33 – reference: 14607137 - Brain Cogn. 2003 Nov;53(2):152-7 – reference: 12052907 - Annu Rev Neurosci. 2002;25:151-88 – reference: 8653390 - Brain Lang. 1996 Mar;52(3):452-73 – reference: 16214387 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Nov;9(11):512-8 – reference: 21557650 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2011 Nov;23(11):3254-66 – reference: 22013214 - Physiol Rev. 2011 Oct;91(4):1357-92 |
SSID | ssj0062651 |
Score | 2.1869714 |
Snippet | Sentences such as
are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (
) started doing with the object (
). In principle, indeterminate... Sentences such as "The author started the book" are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (the author) started doing with the object... Sentences such as The author started the book are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject (the author) started doing with the object... Sentences such as The author started the book are indeterminate because they do not make explicit what the subject ( the author ) started doing with the object... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 614 |
SubjectTerms | Cognitive ability Cortex (cingulate) Cortex (frontal) Cortex (temporal) Eye movements Frontal gyrus Functional anatomy Functional magnetic resonance imaging Handbooks Hemispheric laterality Linguistics Nervous system Neuroscience NMR Nuclear magnetic resonance Reading Semantics Superior temporal gyrus Temporal gyrus Theory |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access dbid: M48 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1LS8QwEA6iFy_i2_oigggeqn2kSSuILKKosB7UBW8lbTKssGZ1H-D-e2e6u9VV8dykTWeSznydmW8YO0TEgH6DUn4AZeILDaVfpGXsW5mCjpMCbEa1w817edMSd8_J81d59ESA_T-hHfWTavU6Jx_vows88OeEONHenoJrD6moPKS4gqSu1gtolxQ1cmiKOqaAnnsSjgOVf86aNUy_vM2fSZPfrND1MluauI-8Mdb3CpuzbpWtNRxC59cRP-JVQmf1p3yN3eMW4BX5Bk25pDYcHfIseRf4rTPTPJiB5Y9EzIna5_R16Nk2JbV33RlvOA7Nh1tOyYajdda6vnq6vPEn7RP8UkRi4BcAKjMCtAmVNMTCk6WBjqJSmiSCUKkEiM0NoNDkxKUgjQ60IUihjMhsvMHmXdfZLcZBI6whZniErSI2RSpFZoJCFjKyoDR47HQqu7yccItTi4tOjhiDpJ1X0s5J2nklbY8d1zPexrwa_4zdnaojn26QnFaTBjHCJY8d1JfxbFDAQzvbHfZz9EUkFd6mgcc2x9qrH0YRZeLi95ia0Ws9gHi3Z6-4l3bFv52EeFcht_9f1g5bpFeg1Jco2GXzg97Q7qEDMyj2q335CZ9A8TU priority: 102 providerName: Scholars Portal |
Title | The Neuronal Correlates of Indeterminate Sentence Comprehension: An fMRI Study |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28066204 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2290803576 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1856865580 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5168646 |
Volume | 10 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwEB5Be-GCoOURaCsjISQO0ebh2A4XtK1aWqRdoUKlvUVO7NEiFad0dw_998xkk4UtUi--xHnN2ONvPONvAN6Tx0C4Qes4waaIpcUmrk2Tx14ZtHlRoy_57PBkqs6v5NdZMes33BZ9WuVgEztD7dqG98hHzEtukpzg8eeb3zFXjeLoal9C4zHskgk2NMJ3j0-n3y4HW0xovUjXwUlyxcoRhvmKj5-nHIFQqdxejP5DmPcTJf9Zec6ewdMeMorxWsfP4ZEPe7A_DuQu_7oTH0SXxNntju_DlNQuOsINvuWES29cM5oULYqL4Ibcl6UX35mMkzQu2CLc-jknsrfhkxgHgZPLC8EJhncv4Ors9MfJedyXTIgbmcllXCPq0km0LtXKMfNOaRKbZY1yRYap1gUygxtibRm4GVTOJtaxG6GdLH3-EnZCG_xrEGjJlWGpk6sqc1cbJUuX1KpWmUdtMYLRILuq6fnEuazFdUV-BUu76qRdsbSrTtoRfNzccbPm0nig78GgjqqfVYvq7xiI4N3mMs0HDnLY4NvVoiL8ofiwrUkieLXW3uZlHEVm_v0I9JZeNx2Ya3v7Svg57zi3i5SeKtWbhz_rLTzhX-B0lyw5gJ3l7cofEmhZ1kf9yDzqnH5qv8xSaifS_AFv-PG0 |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwED-N7gFeEGN8BDYwEiDxEDUfjpMgIdSNTS1bKzQ2aW_GiW0VaThjbYX6T_E3cpePjoK0tz3bcZK7s_073_l3AK_RY0DckKZ-YMvE58qWfpGVsW9EZlWcFNbkdHd4PBHDM_75PDnfgN_dXRhKq-zWxHqh1lVJZ-R94iXPghjh8cfLnz5VjaLoaldCozGLI7P8hS7b7MPoE-r3TRQdHpzuD_22qoBf8ojP_cLaNNfcKh2mQhM5TZ4FKopKoZPIhmmaWCI5s7ZQhG0yK7QKlCaknWqemxjHvQObPEao0IPNvYPJl5Nu7UfvIAmbYCi6fnnfuumCrruHFPEQIV_f_P5DtP8mZv610x0-gPstRGWDxqa2YMO4h7A9cOie_1iyt6xOGq1P47dhgmbGaoIPemSfSn1cEHpllWUjp7tcm7lhX4n8Ey2M0Qp0ZaaUOF-592zgmB2fjBglNC4fwdmtCPMx9FzlzFNgVqHrRFpG15jHusgEz3VQiEJExqbKetDvZCfLlr-cymhcSPRjSNqylrYkacta2h68Wz1x2XB33NB3p1OHbGfxTF7bnAevVs04_yioopypFjOJeEfQ5d4s8OBJo73VyyhqTXz_HqRrel11IG7v9Rb3fVpzfCchjsrFs5s_6yXcHZ6Oj-XxaHL0HO7R71CqTRTsQG9-tTC7CJjmxYvWShl8u-2J8QeG9iu- |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9QwEB6VrYS4IKA80hYwEiBxiDYPx04qIbS0XXUpXVWFSr0FJ_ZokVqn7e4K7V_rr8OTx8KC1FvPdl4zY_ubzMw3AG-dx-Bwg5R-gGXic4WlX6Rl7BuRooqTAk1GtcNHY3Fwyr-cJWdrcNPVwlBaZbcn1hu1rkr6R94nXvI0iB087mObFnG8N_x0eeVTBymKtHbtNBoTOTSLX859m34c7Tldv4ui4f733QO_7TDglzziM79AlJnmqHQohSaimiwNVBSVQicRhlImSIRniIUinJOi0CpQmlC31DwzsbvvPViXVD7ag_XP--Pjk-4ccJ5CEjaBUecGZn20kzmVvocU_RAhXz0I_0O3_yZp_nXqDR_BwxauskFjX49hzdgnsDGwzlW_WLD3rE4grf_Mb8DYmRyryT7okl1q-3FOSJZVyEZWd3k3M8O-ERGoszZGu9G1mVASfWV32MAyPDoZMUpuXDyF0zsR5jPo2cqaF8BQOTeKNO7cZB7rIhU800EhChEZlAo96Heyy8uWy5xaapznzqchaee1tHOSdl5L24MPyysuGx6PW-Zud-rI2xU9zf_YnwdvlsNuLVKARVlTzae5wz6CCn3TwIPnjfaWD6MINnH_eyBX9LqcQDzfqyP256Tm-05Cd1cuNm9_rddw3y2I_OtofLgFD-hrKOsmCrahN7uem5cOO82KV62RMvhx1-viN1A-L_w |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Neuronal+Correlates+of+Indeterminate+Sentence+Comprehension%3A+An+fMRI+Study&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+human+neuroscience&rft.au=de+Almeida%2C+Roberto+G&rft.au=Riven%2C+Levi&rft.au=Manouilidou%2C+Christina&rft.au=Lungu%2C+Ovidiu&rft.date=2016-12-20&rft.pub=Frontiers+Research+Foundation&rft.eissn=1662-5161&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffnhum.2016.00614&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1662-5161&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1662-5161&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1662-5161&client=summon |