Planning and on-line control of catching as a function of perceptual-motor constraints
Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumin...
Saved in:
Published in | Acta psychologica Vol. 126; no. 1; pp. 59 - 78 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Amsterdam
Elsevier B.V
01.09.2007
Elsevier |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0001-6918 1873-6297 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumination circuit in the lab: upon the first movement of the catcher, all lights went out within 3
ms. The authors studied (a) how much movement time catchers prefer if no visual information is available after movement onset, and (b) how movement execution changes under such temporal constraints. It was hypothesised that, in order to accomplish successful catching behaviour, (1) movement initiation would be postponed in order to allow sufficient information uptake before the lights went out, and (2) an alternative control strategy would have to be mobilised, since on-line control becomes inappropriate when catching in the dark. In the first experiment, the adaptation process to the light–dark paradigm was investigated. In the second experiment, the conclusions from experiment 1 were challenged under varying ball speeds. In order to maintain catching performance, subjects initiated the catch approximately 280
ms before ball-hand contact. Next to changes in temporal structure of the catch and subtle kinematic adaptations, evidence for a change in the control mode emerged: while an on-line control strategy was adopted under normal illumination, catching movements seemed to be executed as planned in advance when catching in the dark. Additionally, perceptual constraints seem to determine the time of movement initiation, rather than motor constraints. These results emphasize the capability of the human perceptuo-motor system to adjust promptly to new task constraints. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumination circuit in the lab: upon the first movement of the catcher, all lights went out within 3
ms. The authors studied (a) how much movement time catchers prefer if no visual information is available after movement onset, and (b) how movement execution changes under such temporal constraints. It was hypothesised that, in order to accomplish successful catching behaviour, (1) movement initiation would be postponed in order to allow sufficient information uptake before the lights went out, and (2) an alternative control strategy would have to be mobilised, since on-line control becomes inappropriate when catching in the dark. In the first experiment, the adaptation process to the light–dark paradigm was investigated. In the second experiment, the conclusions from experiment 1 were challenged under varying ball speeds. In order to maintain catching performance, subjects initiated the catch approximately 280
ms before ball-hand contact. Next to changes in temporal structure of the catch and subtle kinematic adaptations, evidence for a change in the control mode emerged: while an on-line control strategy was adopted under normal illumination, catching movements seemed to be executed as planned in advance when catching in the dark. Additionally, perceptual constraints seem to determine the time of movement initiation, rather than motor constraints. These results emphasize the capability of the human perceptuo-motor system to adjust promptly to new task constraints. Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumination circuit in the lab: upon the first movement of the catcher, all lights went out within 3ms. The authors studied (a) how much movement time catchers prefer if no visual information is available after movement onset, and (b) how movement execution changes under such temporal constraints. It was hypothesised that, in order to accomplish successful catching behaviour, (1) movement initiation would be postponed in order to allow sufficient information uptake before the lights went out, and (2) an alternative control strategy would have to be mobilised, since on-line control becomes inappropriate when catching in the dark. In the first experiment, the adaptation process to the light-dark paradigm was investigated. In the second experiment, the conclusions from experiment 1 were challenged under varying ball speeds. In order to maintain catching performance, subjects initiated the catch approximately 280ms before ball-hand contact. Next to changes in temporal structure of the catch and subtle kinematic adaptations, evidence for a change in the control mode emerged: while an on-line control strategy was adopted under normal illumination, catching movements seemed to be executed as planned in advance when catching in the dark. Additionally, perceptual constraints seem to determine the time of movement initiation, rather than motor constraints. These results emphasize the capability of the human perceptuo-motor system to adjust promptly to new task constraints. Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumination circuit in the lab: upon the first movement of the catcher, all lights went out within 3 ms. The authors studied (a) how much movement time catchers prefer if no visual information is available after movement onset, and (b) how movement execution changes under such temporal constraints. It was hypothesised that, in order to accomplish successful catching behaviour, (1) movement initiation would be postponed in order to allow sufficient information uptake before the lights went out, and (2) an alternative control strategy would have to be mobilised, since on-line control becomes inappropriate when catching in the dark. In the first experiment, the adaptation process to the light-dark paradigm was investigated. In the second experiment, the conclusions from experiment 1 were challenged under varying ball speeds. In order to maintain catching performance, subjects initiated the catch approximately 280 ms before ball-hand contact. Next to changes in temporal structure of the catch and subtle kinematic adaptations, evidence for a change in the control mode emerged: while an on-line control strategy was adopted under normal illumination, catching movements seemed to be executed as planned in advance when catching in the dark. Additionally, perceptual constraints seem to determine the time of movement initiation, rather than motor constraints. These results emphasize the capability of the human perceptuo-motor system to adjust promptly to new task constraints.Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumination circuit in the lab: upon the first movement of the catcher, all lights went out within 3 ms. The authors studied (a) how much movement time catchers prefer if no visual information is available after movement onset, and (b) how movement execution changes under such temporal constraints. It was hypothesised that, in order to accomplish successful catching behaviour, (1) movement initiation would be postponed in order to allow sufficient information uptake before the lights went out, and (2) an alternative control strategy would have to be mobilised, since on-line control becomes inappropriate when catching in the dark. In the first experiment, the adaptation process to the light-dark paradigm was investigated. In the second experiment, the conclusions from experiment 1 were challenged under varying ball speeds. In order to maintain catching performance, subjects initiated the catch approximately 280 ms before ball-hand contact. Next to changes in temporal structure of the catch and subtle kinematic adaptations, evidence for a change in the control mode emerged: while an on-line control strategy was adopted under normal illumination, catching movements seemed to be executed as planned in advance when catching in the dark. Additionally, perceptual constraints seem to determine the time of movement initiation, rather than motor constraints. These results emphasize the capability of the human perceptuo-motor system to adjust promptly to new task constraints. Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing constraints. In a catching task, perceptual-motor constraints were internally controlled by coupling movement onset of the catch and the illumination circuit in the lab: upon the first movement of the catcher, all lights went out within 3 ms. The authors studied (a) how much movement time catchers prefer if no visual information is available after movement onset, and (b) how movement execution changes under such temporal constraints. It was hypothesised that, in order to accomplish successful catching behaviour, (1) movement initiation would be postponed in order to allow sufficient information uptake before the lights went out, and (2) an alternative control strategy would have to be mobilised, since on-line control becomes inappropriate when catching in the dark. In the first experiment, the adaptation process to the light-dark paradigm was investigated. In the second experiment, the conclusions from experiment 1 were challenged under varying ball speeds. In order to maintain catching performance, subjects initiated the catch approximately 280 ms before ball-hand contact. Next to changes in temporal structure of the catch and subtle kinematic adaptations, evidence for a change in the control mode emerged: while an on-line control strategy was adopted under normal illumination, catching movements seemed to be executed as planned in advance when catching in the dark. Additionally, perceptual constraints seem to determine the time of movement initiation, rather than motor constraints. These results emphasize the capability of the human perceptuo-motor system to adjust promptly to new task constraints. |
Author | Mazyn, Liesbeth I.N. Montagne, Gilles Lenoir, Matthieu Savelsbergh, Geert J.P. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Liesbeth I.N. surname: Mazyn fullname: Mazyn, Liesbeth I.N. organization: Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2, B-9000 Gent, Belgium – sequence: 2 givenname: Geert J.P. surname: Savelsbergh fullname: Savelsbergh, Geert J.P. organization: Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, Free University Amsterdam, The Netherlands – sequence: 3 givenname: Gilles surname: Montagne fullname: Montagne, Gilles organization: Faculté des Sciences du Sport, Université de la Méditerranée, France – sequence: 4 givenname: Matthieu surname: Lenoir fullname: Lenoir, Matthieu email: matthieu.lenoir@ugent.be organization: Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2, B-9000 Gent, Belgium |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18991191$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17239809$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkU-LFDEQxYOsuLOr30CkL3rrMf86k3gQZNFVWNCDeg3V1WnN0JO0SVrYb2_aGVfwsJ5Cqn7vQb13Qc5CDI6Qp4xuGWXq5X4LWOZ8u-WUqjraUsoekA3TO9EqbnZnZEPrqFWG6XNykfO-fiUz7BE5ZzsujKZmQ75-miAEH741EIYmhnbywTUYQ0lxauLYIBT8_nufG2jGJWDxMayb2SV0c1lgag-xxLSqckngQ8mPycMRpuyenN5L8uXd289X79ubj9cfrt7ctCg5K63sKJemN2o0A8AgVN8bADTY75QRIDrg3YhUSeC7UXTSaCl6zbWWstPKGHFJXhx95xR_LC4Xe_AZ3VSPcnHJVmkmhOq6_4KCd1Izriv47AQu_cENdk7-AOnW_omsAs9PAGSEaUwQ0Oe_nDaG1ZQr9-rIYYo5Jzda9AXW8NaMJsuoXXu0e3vs0a49rtNaWhXLf8R3_vfLXh9lrmb-07tkM3oX0A0-OSx2iP5-g1-sZ7hA |
CODEN | APSOAZ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychsport_2014_03_005 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_011_2846_1 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1080_0 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_009_2041_9 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2014_10_008 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_008_1691_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneumeth_2011_01_013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2010_02_030 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_4_9 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_008_1588_1 crossref_primary_10_1080_1750984X_2023_2280899 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0119445 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2021_02_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2011_06_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2017_08_012 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0031770 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2013_01_014 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0158606 crossref_primary_10_1167_19_12_3 crossref_primary_10_1249_MSS_0b013e31828288f1 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_009_2102_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2010_02_010 crossref_primary_10_1119_1_2937899 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_642025 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/BF00228752 10.1123/mcj.8.1.76 10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.591 10.1037/h0055392 10.1016/S0167-9457(97)00014-6 10.1080/00222895.1994.9941666 10.1080/00222899809601348 10.1007/BF00227789 10.1037/h0092992 10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.342 10.1007/s002210050939 10.1037/0033-295X.86.5.415 10.1163/15685680252875129 10.1037/0096-1523.17.2.315 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.08.067 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00004-9 10.1123/mcj.10.2.143 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00177-X 10.3758/BF03195017 10.1007/s00221-001-0980-x 10.1207/s15326969eco0404_1 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941608 10.1007/s00221-004-1926-x 10.1016/j.aap.2004.12.002 10.1080/00222895.1974.10734990 10.1007/s00221-003-1642-y 10.1080/02640410400022722 10.1080/02701367.1991.10608724 10.1080/026404197366985 10.1080/00222895.1974.10734974 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2006 Elsevier B.V. 2007 INIST-CNRS |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2006 Elsevier B.V. – notice: 2007 INIST-CNRS |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TA 8FD JG9 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Materials Business File Technology Research Database Materials Research Database MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Materials Research Database Technology Research Database Materials Business File MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Materials Research Database MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1873-6297 |
EndPage | 78 |
ExternalDocumentID | 17239809 18991191 10_1016_j_actpsy_2006_10_001 S0001691806001302 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -DZ -~X .GJ .~1 0R~ 0SF 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 23M 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFWJ AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABIVO ABLJU ABMAC ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFS ACHQT ACKIV ACNCT ACPRK ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS ADMUD AEBSH AEKER AFFNX AFKWA AFMIJ AFPKN AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA GROUPED_DOAJ HMW HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IHE J1W KOM LPU M3V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A NHB O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OK1 OKEIE OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PQQKQ Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSY SSZ T5K TN5 UAP UQL W2D WH7 WUQ XPP ZKB ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABJNI ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADNMO ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH IQODW PKN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TA 8FD EFKBS JG9 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-450249b96f9daad36bb9aac9cb7693a35a25fc064a27f3549843b828844586993 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0001-6918 |
IngestDate | Fri Sep 05 03:11:17 EDT 2025 Thu Sep 04 20:20:05 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 01:47:14 EST 2025 Thu Feb 13 05:29:45 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:06:38 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:22:42 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:32:44 EST 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | 2330 Constraints Motor control Catching Kinematics Human Constraint Perception Cognition Interception Planning Experimental study Motricity |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c421t-450249b96f9daad36bb9aac9cb7693a35a25fc064a27f3549843b828844586993 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 17239809 |
PQID | 32548128 |
PQPubID | 23500 |
PageCount | 20 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_68133655 proquest_miscellaneous_32548128 pubmed_primary_17239809 pascalfrancis_primary_18991191 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_actpsy_2006_10_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2006_10_001 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_actpsy_2006_10_001 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2007-09-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2007-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 2007 text: 2007-09-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Amsterdam |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Amsterdam – name: Netherlands |
PublicationTitle | Acta psychologica |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Acta Psychol (Amst) |
PublicationYear | 2007 |
Publisher | Elsevier B.V Elsevier |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier B.V – name: Elsevier |
References | Caljouw, van der Kamp, Savelsbergh (bib4) 2006; 392 Marteniuk, Romanow (bib15) 1983 Elliott, Helsen, Chua (bib6) 2001; 127 Woodworth, R.S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review Monograph, Supplement 3. Michaels, Oudejans (bib18) 1992; 4 Vereijken, van Emmerik, Whiting, Newell (bib34) 1992; 24 Whiting, Sharp (bib37) 1974; 6 Elliott, Zuberec, Milgram (bib7) 1994; 26 Montagne, Laurent, Durey, Bootsma (bib20) 1999; 129 Savelsbergh, Whiting, Pijpers, van Santvoord (bib25) 1993; 93 Sharp, Whiting (bib28) 1974; 6 Schenk, Mair, Zihl (bib26) 2004; 154 Lee, Young (bib12) 1985 Magill (bib14) 2004 Tresilian (bib32) 2005; 67 Brenner, De Lussanet, Smeets (bib2) 2002; 15 Sharp, Whiting (bib29) 1975; 1 Fitts (bib8) 1954; 47 Brouwer, Brenner, Smeets (bib3) 2002; 143 Lenoir, Vansteenkiste, Vermeulen, De Clercq (bib13) 2005; 23 te Velde, van der Kamp, Barela, Savelsbergh (bib31) 2005; 37 Vincent (bib35) 1995 Olivier, Weeks, Lyons, Ricker, Elliott (bib21) 1998; 30 Desmurget, Pélisson, Rossetti, Prablanc (bib5) 1998; 22 Ko, Challis, Newell (bib10) 2003; 22 Temprado, Della-Grasta, Farrell, Laurent (bib30) 1997; 16 Tyldesley, Whiting (bib33) 1975; 1 Laurent, Montagne, Savelsbergh (bib11) 1994; 101 Montagne (bib19) 2005; 36 Savelsbergh, Whiting, Bootsma (bib24) 1991; 17 Mazyn, Montagne, Savelsbergh, Lenoir (bib17) 2006; 10 Peper, Bootsma, Mestre, Bakker (bib22) 1994; 20 Bootsma, Houbiers, Whiting, van Wieringen (bib1) 1991; 62 Mazyn, Lenoir, Montagne, Savelsbergh (bib16) 2004; 157 Whiting, Alderson, Sanderson (bib36) 1973; 4 Heath, Westwood, Binsted (bib9) 2004; 8 Regan (bib23) 1997; 15 Schmidt, Zelaznik, Hawkins, Franks, Quinn (bib27) 1979; 86 Desmurget (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib5) 1998; 22 Schenk (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib26) 2004; 154 Whiting (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib36) 1973; 4 Savelsbergh (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib24) 1991; 17 Temprado (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib30) 1997; 16 Elliott (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib6) 2001; 127 Sharp (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib29) 1975; 1 Tyldesley (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib33) 1975; 1 Regan (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib23) 1997; 15 Magill (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib14) 2004 Sharp (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib28) 1974; 6 Vereijken (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib34) 1992; 24 Heath (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib9) 2004; 8 Brouwer (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib3) 2002; 143 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib38 Mazyn (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib16) 2004; 157 Whiting (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib37) 1974; 6 Tresilian (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib32) 2005; 67 Schmidt (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib27) 1979; 86 Laurent (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib11) 1994; 101 Marteniuk (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib15) 1983 Fitts (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib8) 1954; 47 Savelsbergh (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib25) 1993; 93 Elliott (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib7) 1994; 26 Peper (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib22) 1994; 20 Olivier (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib21) 1998; 30 Lenoir (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib13) 2005; 23 Caljouw (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib4) 2006; 392 Montagne (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib19) 2005; 36 Brenner (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib2) 2002; 15 Michaels (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib18) 1992; 4 Montagne (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib20) 1999; 129 Vincent (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib35) 1995 Ko (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib10) 2003; 22 Lee (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib12) 1985 te Velde (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib31) 2005; 37 Mazyn (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib17) 2006; 10 Bootsma (10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib1) 1991; 62 |
References_xml | – volume: 143 start-page: 198 year: 2002 end-page: 211 ident: bib3 article-title: Hitting moving targets: is target speed used in guiding the hand? publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 4 start-page: 199 year: 1992 end-page: 222 ident: bib18 article-title: The optics and actions of catching fly balls: zeroing out optical acceleration publication-title: Ecological Psychology – volume: 15 start-page: 533 year: 1997 end-page: 558 ident: bib23 article-title: Visual factors in hitting and catching publication-title: Journal of Sports Sciences – volume: 15 start-page: 129 year: 2002 end-page: 140 ident: bib2 article-title: Independent control of acceleration and direction of the hand when hitting moving targets publication-title: Spatial Vision – volume: 17 start-page: 315 year: 1991 end-page: 322 ident: bib24 article-title: Grasping tau publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance – volume: 10 start-page: 143 year: 2006 end-page: 159 ident: bib17 article-title: Reorganization of catching coordination under varying temporal constraints publication-title: Motor Control – volume: 4 start-page: 155 year: 1973 end-page: 164 ident: bib36 article-title: Critical time intervals for viewing and individual differences in performance of a ball-catching task publication-title: International Journal of Sport Psychology – volume: 36 start-page: 127 year: 2005 end-page: 150 ident: bib19 article-title: Prospective control in sport publication-title: International Journal of Sport Psychology – volume: 24 start-page: 133 year: 1992 end-page: 142 ident: bib34 article-title: Free(z)ing degrees of freedom in skill acquisition publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – volume: 16 start-page: 653 year: 1997 end-page: 676 ident: bib30 article-title: A novice-expert comparison of (intra-limb) coordination subserving the volleyball serve publication-title: Human Movement Science – volume: 26 start-page: 113 year: 1994 end-page: 122 ident: bib7 article-title: The effects of periodic visual occlusion on ball catching publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – volume: 154 start-page: 85 year: 2004 end-page: 96 ident: bib26 article-title: The use of visual feedback and on-line target information in catching and grasping publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 101 start-page: 314 year: 1994 end-page: 322 ident: bib11 article-title: The control and coordination of one-handed catching: The effect of temporal constraints publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 22 start-page: 47 year: 2003 end-page: 66 ident: bib10 article-title: Learning to coordinate redundant degrees of freedom in a dynamic balance task publication-title: Human Movement Science – volume: 23 start-page: 871 year: 2005 end-page: 879 ident: bib13 article-title: Effects of contrasting colour patterns of the ball in the volleyball reception publication-title: Journal of Sports Sciences – reference: Woodworth, R.S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review Monograph, Supplement 3. – volume: 37 start-page: 399 year: 2005 end-page: 406 ident: bib31 article-title: Visual timing and adaptive behavior in a road-crossing simulation study publication-title: Accident Analysis and Prevention – volume: 22 start-page: 761 year: 1998 end-page: 788 ident: bib5 article-title: From eye to hand: planning goal-directed movements publication-title: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews – volume: 47 start-page: 381 year: 1954 end-page: 391 ident: bib8 article-title: The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 86 start-page: 415 year: 1979 end-page: 451 ident: bib27 article-title: Motor-output variability – Theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 8 start-page: 76 year: 2004 end-page: 106 ident: bib9 article-title: The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space publication-title: Motor Control – volume: 67 start-page: 129 year: 2005 end-page: 149 ident: bib32 article-title: Hitting a moving target: Perception and action in the timing of rapid interceptions publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – volume: 392 start-page: 84 year: 2006 end-page: 89 ident: bib4 article-title: The impact of task-constraints on the planning and control of interceptive hitting movements publication-title: Neuroscience Letters – volume: 157 start-page: 383 year: 2004 end-page: 390 ident: bib16 article-title: The contribution of stereo vision to one-handed catching publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 127 start-page: 342 year: 2001 end-page: 357 ident: bib6 article-title: A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – start-page: 1 year: 1985 end-page: 30 ident: bib12 article-title: Visual timing of interceptive action publication-title: Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision – year: 2004 ident: bib14 article-title: Motor learning and control: concepts and applications – volume: 30 start-page: 343 year: 1998 end-page: 351 ident: bib21 article-title: Monocular and binocular vision in one-hand ball catching: Interocular integration publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – volume: 129 start-page: 87 year: 1999 end-page: 92 ident: bib20 article-title: Movement reversals in ball catching publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 20 start-page: 591 year: 1994 end-page: 612 ident: bib22 article-title: Catching balls – How to get the hand to the right place at the right time publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance – volume: 1 start-page: 172 year: 1975 end-page: 177 ident: bib33 article-title: Operational timing publication-title: Journal of Human Movement Studies – volume: 1 start-page: 124 year: 1975 end-page: 131 ident: bib29 article-title: Information-processing and eye movement behavior in a ball catching skill publication-title: Journal of Human Movement Studies – volume: 62 start-page: 276 year: 1991 end-page: 284 ident: bib1 article-title: Acquiring an attacking fore-hand drive: The effects of static and dynamic environmental conditions publication-title: Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport – volume: 6 start-page: 11 year: 1974 end-page: 16 ident: bib37 article-title: Visual occlusion factors in a discrete ball-catching task publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – volume: 93 start-page: 148 year: 1993 end-page: 156 ident: bib25 article-title: The visual guidance of catching publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 6 start-page: 139 year: 1974 end-page: 147 ident: bib28 article-title: Exposure and occluded duration effects in a ball-catching skill publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – start-page: 167 year: 1983 end-page: 197 ident: bib15 article-title: Human movement and learning as revealed by variability of movement, use of kinematic information, and Fourier analysis publication-title: Memory and Control of Action – year: 1995 ident: bib35 article-title: Statistics in kinesiology – volume: 101 start-page: 314 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib11 article-title: The control and coordination of one-handed catching: The effect of temporal constraints publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/BF00228752 – volume: 36 start-page: 127 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib19 article-title: Prospective control in sport publication-title: International Journal of Sport Psychology – volume: 8 start-page: 76 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib9 article-title: The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space publication-title: Motor Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.8.1.76 – volume: 20 start-page: 591 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib22 article-title: Catching balls – How to get the hand to the right place at the right time publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.591 – volume: 1 start-page: 172 year: 1975 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib33 article-title: Operational timing publication-title: Journal of Human Movement Studies – volume: 4 start-page: 155 year: 1973 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib36 article-title: Critical time intervals for viewing and individual differences in performance of a ball-catching task publication-title: International Journal of Sport Psychology – volume: 47 start-page: 381 year: 1954 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib8 article-title: The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1037/h0055392 – volume: 16 start-page: 653 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib30 article-title: A novice-expert comparison of (intra-limb) coordination subserving the volleyball serve publication-title: Human Movement Science doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(97)00014-6 – volume: 26 start-page: 113 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib7 article-title: The effects of periodic visual occlusion on ball catching publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222895.1994.9941666 – volume: 30 start-page: 343 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib21 article-title: Monocular and binocular vision in one-hand ball catching: Interocular integration publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222899809601348 – volume: 93 start-page: 148 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib25 article-title: The visual guidance of catching publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/BF00227789 – ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib38 doi: 10.1037/h0092992 – volume: 127 start-page: 342 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib6 article-title: A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.342 – volume: 129 start-page: 87 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib20 article-title: Movement reversals in ball catching publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s002210050939 – volume: 86 start-page: 415 year: 1979 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib27 article-title: Motor-output variability – Theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.86.5.415 – volume: 15 start-page: 129 issue: 2 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib2 article-title: Independent control of acceleration and direction of the hand when hitting moving targets publication-title: Spatial Vision doi: 10.1163/15685680252875129 – volume: 17 start-page: 315 year: 1991 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib24 article-title: Grasping tau publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.17.2.315 – volume: 392 start-page: 84 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib4 article-title: The impact of task-constraints on the planning and control of interceptive hitting movements publication-title: Neuroscience Letters doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.08.067 – volume: 22 start-page: 761 issue: 6 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib5 article-title: From eye to hand: planning goal-directed movements publication-title: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00004-9 – volume: 10 start-page: 143 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib17 article-title: Reorganization of catching coordination under varying temporal constraints publication-title: Motor Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.10.2.143 – volume: 22 start-page: 47 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib10 article-title: Learning to coordinate redundant degrees of freedom in a dynamic balance task publication-title: Human Movement Science doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00177-X – volume: 1 start-page: 124 year: 1975 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib29 article-title: Information-processing and eye movement behavior in a ball catching skill publication-title: Journal of Human Movement Studies – volume: 67 start-page: 129 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib32 article-title: Hitting a moving target: Perception and action in the timing of rapid interceptions publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03195017 – volume: 143 start-page: 198 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib3 article-title: Hitting moving targets: is target speed used in guiding the hand? publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-001-0980-x – volume: 4 start-page: 199 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib18 article-title: The optics and actions of catching fly balls: zeroing out optical acceleration publication-title: Ecological Psychology doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0404_1 – volume: 24 start-page: 133 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib34 article-title: Free(z)ing degrees of freedom in skill acquisition publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941608 – volume: 157 start-page: 383 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib16 article-title: The contribution of stereo vision to one-handed catching publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1926-x – volume: 37 start-page: 399 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib31 article-title: Visual timing and adaptive behavior in a road-crossing simulation study publication-title: Accident Analysis and Prevention doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2004.12.002 – start-page: 167 year: 1983 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib15 article-title: Human movement and learning as revealed by variability of movement, use of kinematic information, and Fourier analysis – volume: 6 start-page: 139 year: 1974 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib28 article-title: Exposure and occluded duration effects in a ball-catching skill publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222895.1974.10734990 – volume: 154 start-page: 85 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib26 article-title: The use of visual feedback and on-line target information in catching and grasping publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1642-y – volume: 23 start-page: 871 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib13 article-title: Effects of contrasting colour patterns of the ball in the volleyball reception publication-title: Journal of Sports Sciences doi: 10.1080/02640410400022722 – volume: 62 start-page: 276 year: 1991 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib1 article-title: Acquiring an attacking fore-hand drive: The effects of static and dynamic environmental conditions publication-title: Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport doi: 10.1080/02701367.1991.10608724 – year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib35 – volume: 15 start-page: 533 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib23 article-title: Visual factors in hitting and catching publication-title: Journal of Sports Sciences doi: 10.1080/026404197366985 – start-page: 1 year: 1985 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib12 article-title: Visual timing of interceptive action – year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib14 – volume: 6 start-page: 11 year: 1974 ident: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001_bib37 article-title: Visual occlusion factors in a discrete ball-catching task publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222895.1974.10734974 |
SSID | ssj0004191 |
Score | 1.9585327 |
Snippet | Two experiments were conducted in order to investigate the adaptability and associated strategies of the human perceptuo-motor system to deal with changing... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 59 |
SubjectTerms | Activity levels. Psychomotricity Adult Biological and medical sciences Biomechanical Phenomena Catching Constraints Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Hand Strength Humans Kinematics Male Motor control Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor activities Psychomotor Performance Visual Perception |
Title | Planning and on-line control of catching as a function of perceptual-motor constraints |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.10.001 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17239809 https://www.proquest.com/docview/32548128 https://www.proquest.com/docview/68133655 |
Volume | 126 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LT9wwEB4hekGqKkpfy2PrQ6-GTfyIc0Qr0EIBVVVB3CI_EhWEkhXZPXDpb-9MnLDisELiFMmP2JpxZr6J5wHww6JaKp2qOCqTikuvUu4QNfAyeIX6T0npyVC8vNKza3l-q243YDrEwpBbZS_7o0zvpHXfctRT82h-d0cxvpRJJDETHa_fKIJdZpQ___Dfys1DJkPVvITT6CF8rvPxsh5XfYpXEp2PV7JOPb2f2xaJVsVqF-vhaKeWTrfhQ48n2XHc8kfYKOsd2HoWa0-f4GaoS8RsHVhTcwKWrHdRZ03FPErjv11_yywjRUfMop559HpZ2geOHG0eaVbbFZVYtJ_h-vTkz3TG-2oK3Ms0WXCpKDugy3WVB2uD0M7l1vrcOyqHaIWyqao8IhSbZpVAs9FI4dAeM1IqoxHGfIHNuqnLb8BCyExIhShD6dE-M84r461UXk9MPgliBGIgYuH7VOO0uYdi8Cm7LyLpqQqmplYk_Qj486x5TLXxyvhs4E_x4sgUqA1emTl-wc7Vcmh9Usa7EXwf-Fvg50Z3KLYum2VbCDSoEROZ9SO0QbNfKzWCr_FgrN6eUbLFSb775o3vwVb8t0w-bvuwuXhclgcIihZu3J36Mbw7nv6--EXPs5-zq__C2Q1z |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LT9wwEB5ROBSpqsqjdNsCPnB12cSPOMcKgZYWOAHiFvmRCNAqWZHdA5f-9s7ECSsOKySusR1bM87MN_HnGYAji26pdKri6EwqLr1KuUPUwMvgFfo_JaWnQPHySk9u5J87dbcGJ8NdGKJV9rY_2vTOWvdPjntpHs8eHuiOL2USScxYx-O3D7Ahlchoa__6t-R5yGQom5dw6j7cn-tIXtbjtM_xTKIjeSWr_NOnmW1RalUsd7Eaj3Z-6ewLfO4BJfsd17wFa2W9DZsvdu15B26HwkTM1oE1NSdkyXqOOmsq5tEc33ftLbOMPB1pi1pmkfaysFOOKm2eaFTbVZWYt7twc3Z6fTLhfTkF7mWazLlUlB7Q5brKg7VBaOdya33uHdVDtELZVFUeIYpNs0pg3GikcBiQGSmV0YhjvsJ63dTlN2AhZCakQpSh9BigGeeV8VYqr8cmHwcxAjEIsfB9rnFa3LQYSGWPRRQ9lcHU9BRFPwL-MmoWc2280T8b9FO82jMFuoM3Rh68UudyOgw_KeXdCA4H_Rb4vdEhiq3LZtEWAiNqBEVmdQ9tMO7XSo1gL26M5dszyrY4zr-_e-GH8HFyfXlRXJxf_f0Bm_FHMxHefsL6_GlR7iNCmruD7gv4D3bzDXQ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Planning+and+on-line+control+of+catching+as+a+function+of+perceptual-motor+constraints&rft.jtitle=Acta+psychologica&rft.au=Mazyn%2C+Liesbeth+I+N&rft.au=Savelsbergh%2C+Geert+J+P&rft.au=Montagne%2C+Gilles&rft.au=Lenoir%2C+Matthieu&rft.date=2007-09-01&rft.issn=0001-6918&rft.volume=126&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=59&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.actpsy.2006.10.001&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0001-6918&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0001-6918&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0001-6918&client=summon |