Learning to write letters: transfer in automated movements indicates modularity of motor programs in human subjects
Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves. However, it is unclear whether their internal representation consists of monolithic blocks or subroutines. This question was investigated using...
Saved in:
Published in | Neuroscience letters Vol. 282; no. 1; pp. 33 - 36 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Shannon
Elsevier Ireland Ltd
17.03.2000
Elsevier |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0304-3940 1872-7972 |
DOI | 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X |
Cover
Abstract | Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves. However, it is unclear whether their internal representation consists of monolithic blocks or subroutines. This question was investigated using a learning paradigm of a writing task. Fifty-nine normal subjects were presented with two similar, but different new letters. Every subject practiced each letter in a series of 60 trials, with the order of letter series randomized. Every session was continuously recorded by a digitizing tablet. Using kinematic analysis, we measured the number of vertical acceleration peaks as an indication of the number of corrective movements (COM). Since COM declined as automatization was approached, we could quantitatively infer progress in motor learning under natural learning conditions. In the case of modular storage of MP, transfer in-between letters was expected due to the re-use of pre-learned motor subroutines. Statistical analysis showed that the exponential model described the data much better than the linear model (residual error:
P<0.88 and
P<0.00001, respectively), as expected for a learning paradigm. There was no difference between letters per se (
P<0.77). Motor improvement differed significantly (
P<0.02) between the first and the second series; there was a much greater reduction of COM in the second series (50.1 vs. 41.1%). This difference can be logically ascribed to transfer, indicating that automated movements are stored in motor subroutines. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves. However, it is unclear whether their internal representation consists of monolithic blocks or subroutines. This question was investigated using a learning paradigm of a writing task. Fifty-nine normal subjects were presented with two similar, but different new letters. Every subject practiced each letter in a series of 60 trials, with the order of letter series randomized. Every session was continuously recorded by a digitizing tablet. Using kinematic analysis, we measured the number of vertical acceleration peaks as an indication of the number of corrective movements (COM). Since COM declined as automatization was approached, we could quantitatively infer progress in motor learning under natural learning conditions. In the case of modular storage of MP, transfer in-between letters was expected due to the re-use of pre-learned motor subroutines. Statistical analysis showed that the exponential model described the data much better than the linear model (residual error: P<0.88 and P<0.00001, respectively), as expected for a learning paradigm. There was no difference between letters per se (P<0.77). Motor improvement differed significantly (P<0.02) between the first and the second series; there was a much greater reduction of COM in the second series (50.1 vs. 41.1%). This difference can be logically ascribed to transfer, indicating that automated movements are stored in motor subroutines.Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves. However, it is unclear whether their internal representation consists of monolithic blocks or subroutines. This question was investigated using a learning paradigm of a writing task. Fifty-nine normal subjects were presented with two similar, but different new letters. Every subject practiced each letter in a series of 60 trials, with the order of letter series randomized. Every session was continuously recorded by a digitizing tablet. Using kinematic analysis, we measured the number of vertical acceleration peaks as an indication of the number of corrective movements (COM). Since COM declined as automatization was approached, we could quantitatively infer progress in motor learning under natural learning conditions. In the case of modular storage of MP, transfer in-between letters was expected due to the re-use of pre-learned motor subroutines. Statistical analysis showed that the exponential model described the data much better than the linear model (residual error: P<0.88 and P<0.00001, respectively), as expected for a learning paradigm. There was no difference between letters per se (P<0.77). Motor improvement differed significantly (P<0.02) between the first and the second series; there was a much greater reduction of COM in the second series (50.1 vs. 41.1%). This difference can be logically ascribed to transfer, indicating that automated movements are stored in motor subroutines. Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves. However, it is unclear whether their internal representation consists of monolithic blocks or subroutines. This question was investigated using a learning paradigm of a writing task. Fifty-nine normal subjects were presented with two similar, but different new letters. Every subject practiced each letter in a series of 60 trials, with the order of letter series randomized. Every session was continuously recorded by a digitizing tablet. Using kinematic analysis, we measured the number of vertical acceleration peaks as an indication of the number of corrective movements (COM). Since COM declined as automatization was approached, we could quantitatively infer progress in motor learning under natural learning conditions. In the case of modular storage of MP, transfer in-between letters was expected due to the re-use of pre-learned motor subroutines. Statistical analysis showed that the exponential model described the data much better than the linear model (residual error: P < 0.88 and P < 0.00001, respectively), as expected for a learning paradigm. There was no difference between letters per se (P < 0.77). Motor improvement differed significantly (P < 0.02) between the first and the second series; there was a much greater reduction of COM in the second series (50.1 vs. 41.1%). This difference can be logically ascribed to transfer, indicating that automated movements are stored in motor subroutines. Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves. However, it is unclear whether their internal representation consists of monolithic blocks or subroutines. This question was investigated using a learning paradigm of a writing task. Fifty-nine normal subjects were presented with two similar, but different new letters. Every subject practiced each letter in a series of 60 trials, with the order of letter series randomized. Every session was continuously recorded by a digitizing tablet. Using kinematic analysis, we measured the number of vertical acceleration peaks as an indication of the number of corrective movements (COM). Since COM declined as automatization was approached, we could quantitatively infer progress in motor learning under natural learning conditions. In the case of modular storage of MP, transfer in-between letters was expected due to the re-use of pre-learned motor subroutines. Statistical analysis showed that the exponential model described the data much better than the linear model (residual error: P<0.88 and P<0.00001, respectively), as expected for a learning paradigm. There was no difference between letters per se ( P<0.77). Motor improvement differed significantly ( P<0.02) between the first and the second series; there was a much greater reduction of COM in the second series (50.1 vs. 41.1%). This difference can be logically ascribed to transfer, indicating that automated movements are stored in motor subroutines. |
Author | Straube, Andreas Eggert, Thomas Kharraz-Tavakol, Oliver D. Mai, Norbert |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Oliver D. surname: Kharraz-Tavakol fullname: Kharraz-Tavakol, Oliver D. – sequence: 2 givenname: Thomas surname: Eggert fullname: Eggert, Thomas – sequence: 3 givenname: Norbert surname: Mai fullname: Mai, Norbert – sequence: 4 givenname: Andreas surname: Straube fullname: Straube, Andreas email: astraube@brain.nefo.uni-muenchen.de |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1320874$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10713389$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkU9rFTEUxYNU7Gv1IyhZiOhi9GYymczoQkrxHzxwoUJ3IZO5qSkzSZtkWvrtzfieFlxYCFxy-Z3D5ZwjcuCDR0KeMnjNgLVvvgGHpuJ9Ay8BXgF0glVnD8iGdbKuZC_rA7L5ixySo5QuAEAw0Twihwwk47zrNyRtUUfv_DnNgd5El5FOmDPG9JbmqH2yGKnzVC85zDrjSOdwjTP6nMp6dKbsUtmNy6SL-pYGW345RHoZw3nU84rRn8usPU3LcIEmp8fkodVTwif7eUx-fPzw_fRztf366cvpybYyTc1yxZq26blhrQXRGw5Qt5z3smtNOwjdMStYj1qgsYzpvm3qQcsRrBwHq43kgh-TFzvfcsrVgimr2SWD06Q9hiUpCb1seXG9D2QF6zq-gs_24DLMOKrL6GYdb9WfPAvwfA_oZPRkS4LGpTuO19DJpmDvdpiJIaWIVhmXdXbBl8zdVPzUWrL6XbJaG1SwvlKyOitq8Y_67o7_697vdFhCv3YYVTIOvcHRxdKLGoO7x-EXDzy_ng |
CODEN | NELED5 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0158684 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2011_07_005 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1468_0017_2008_00344_x crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0270466 crossref_primary_10_1186_s40064_016_2554_1 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1469_8749_2009_03478_x crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_007_0851_1 crossref_primary_10_2466_10_11_23_PMS_113_2_619_630 crossref_primary_10_1080_09297049_2024_2328374 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0304_3940_02_01102_3 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00058-M 10.5014/ajot.48.9.810 10.1093/ptj/74.8.738 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.41 10.2466/pms.1988.67.2.563 10.1037/h0055392 10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1061 10.1007/s002210050733 10.1016/0001-6918(93)90006-D 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00409.1997 10.1006/nimg.1998.0393 10.1080/02701367.1994.10762205 10.1016/0165-0270(94)90053-1 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00022-4 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2000 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd 2000 INIST-CNRS |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2000 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd – notice: 2000 INIST-CNRS |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1872-7972 |
EndPage | 36 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10713389 1320874 10_1016_S0304_3940_00_00851_X S030439400000851X |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M -~X .55 .GJ .~1 0R~ 123 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29N 4.4 457 4G. 53G 5RE 5VS 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM AABNK AACTN AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABJNI ABLJU ABMAC ABTAH ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACIUM ACRLP ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS ADMUD AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AENEX AETEA AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AHPSJ AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA HMQ HVGLF HZ~ IHE J1W KOM M2V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A O-L O9- OAUVE OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SSN SSZ T5K WH7 WUQ X7M YCJ ZGI ZXP ZY4 ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADNMO AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK EFKBS 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-146493c16f059c30026339786c6b5a81f519ea5ecf11a9642ba7d0f7dbfac7353 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0304-3940 |
IngestDate | Thu Sep 04 20:49:40 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 14:55:08 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:32:49 EST 2025 Wed May 29 03:47:38 EDT 2024 Tue Jul 01 01:08:13 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:10:23 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:20:35 EST 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | Learning Transfer Writing Automatization Motor program Human Automation Hand writing Acquisition process Language Cognition Module Motricity Motor learning |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c421t-146493c16f059c30026339786c6b5a81f519ea5ecf11a9642ba7d0f7dbfac7353 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 10713389 |
PQID | 17638836 |
PQPubID | 23462 |
PageCount | 4 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_70976326 proquest_miscellaneous_17638836 pubmed_primary_10713389 pascalfrancis_primary_1320874 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_S0304_3940_00_00851_X crossref_primary_10_1016_S0304_3940_00_00851_X elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_S0304_3940_00_00851_X |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2000-03-17 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2000-03-17 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 03 year: 2000 text: 2000-03-17 day: 17 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Shannon |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Shannon – name: Ireland |
PublicationTitle | Neuroscience letters |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Neurosci Lett |
PublicationYear | 2000 |
Publisher | Elsevier Ireland Ltd Elsevier |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Ireland Ltd – name: Elsevier |
References | Bock, Schneider (BIB3) 1999; 25 Atkinson (BIB2) 1955; Vol. 1 Plamondon (BIB15) 1993; 82 Fitts (BIB5) 1954; 47 Adams (BIB1) 1987; 101 Lee, Chang, Roh (BIB8) 1999; 9 Shadmehr, Holcomb (BIB19) 1999; 126 Meaney (BIB13) 1994; 65 Jarus (BIB7) 1994; 48 Mai, Marquardt (BIB11) 1995; 37 Stadler (BIB20) 1989; 15 Lersten (BIB9) 1968; 39 Morris, Summers, Matyas, Iansek (BIB14) 1994; 74 Sadr, Matsuoka, Bizzi (BIB17) 1999; 25 Marquardt, Mai (BIB12) 1994; 52 Deubel (BIB4) 1995; 35 Rizzolatti, Luppino, Matelli (BIB16) 1998; 106 Mai, Marquardt (BIB10) 1996 Hautala (BIB6) 1988; 67 Shadmehr, Brashers (BIB18) 1997; 17 Morris (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB14) 1994; 74 Fitts (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB5) 1954; 47 Shadmehr (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB18) 1997; 17 Mai (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB11) 1995; 37 Adams (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB1) 1987; 101 Lee (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB8) 1999; 9 Shadmehr (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB19) 1999; 126 Plamondon (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB15) 1993; 82 Atkinson (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB2) 1955; Vol. 1 Jarus (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB7) 1994; 48 Bock (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB3) 1999; 25 Stadler (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB20) 1989; 15 Deubel (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB4) 1995; 35 Marquardt (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB12) 1994; 52 Hautala (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB6) 1988; 67 Sadr (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB17) 1999; 25 Lersten (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB9) 1968; 39 Meaney (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB13) 1994; 65 Rizzolatti (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB16) 1998; 106 Mai (10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB10) 1996 |
References_xml | – volume: Vol. 1 start-page: 677 year: 1955 end-page: 685 ident: BIB2 article-title: Alphabet publication-title: Encyclopedia Britannica Ltd, London – volume: 65 start-page: 31 year: 1994 end-page: 39 ident: BIB13 article-title: Developmental modeling effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a novel motor task publication-title: Res. Q. Exerc. Sport – volume: 39 start-page: 575 year: 1968 end-page: 581 ident: BIB9 article-title: Transfer of movement components in a motor learning task publication-title: Research. Quaterly – volume: 48 start-page: 810 year: 1994 end-page: 816 ident: BIB7 article-title: Motor learning and occupational therapy: the organization of practice publication-title: Am. J. Occup. Ther. – volume: 25 start-page: 2176 year: 1999 ident: BIB17 article-title: Asymmetric bilateral transfer of motor adaptation in a visually-guided reaching movement publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – start-page: 307 year: 1996 end-page: 328 ident: BIB10 article-title: Das vernachlässigte Verhalten: Kinematische Analysen der Schreibbewegungen beim Schreibkrampf publication-title: Verhaltenstherapie, Selbstregulation, Selbstmanagement: Frederick H. Kanfer zum 70 – volume: 9 start-page: 117 year: 1999 end-page: 123 ident: BIB8 article-title: Subregions within the supplementary motor area activated at different stages of movement preparation and execution publication-title: NeuroImage – volume: 126 start-page: 235 year: 1999 end-page: 251 ident: BIB19 article-title: Inhibitory control of competing motor memories publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. – volume: 25 start-page: 2179 year: 1999 ident: BIB3 article-title: Proactive interference in motor learning publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – volume: 67 start-page: 563 year: 1988 end-page: 567 ident: BIB6 article-title: Does transfer of training help children learn juggling publication-title: Percept. Mot. Skills – volume: 37 start-page: 538 year: 1995 end-page: 582 ident: BIB11 article-title: Analyse und Therapie motorischer Schreibstoerungen analysis and therapy of disturbed handwriting movements publication-title: Psychol. Beitraege – volume: 82 start-page: 89 year: 1993 end-page: 101 ident: BIB15 article-title: Looking at handwriting generation from a velocity control perspective publication-title: Acta Psychol. Amst. – volume: 106 start-page: 283 year: 1998 end-page: 296 ident: BIB16 article-title: The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroenceph publication-title: clin. Neurophysiol. – volume: 35 start-page: 3529 year: 1995 end-page: 3540 ident: BIB4 article-title: Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements publication-title: Vis. Res. – volume: 17 start-page: 409 year: 1997 end-page: 419 ident: BIB18 article-title: Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor memory publication-title: J. Neurosci. – volume: 101 start-page: 41 year: 1987 end-page: 74 ident: BIB1 article-title: Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills publication-title: Psychol. Bull. – volume: 47 start-page: 381 year: 1954 end-page: 391 ident: BIB5 article-title: The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. – volume: 15 start-page: 1061 year: 1989 end-page: 1069 ident: BIB20 article-title: On learning complex procedural knowledge publication-title: Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. – volume: 74 start-page: 738 year: 1994 end-page: 752 ident: BIB14 article-title: Current status of the motor program publication-title: Phys. Ther. – volume: 52 start-page: 39 year: 1994 end-page: 45 ident: BIB12 article-title: A computational procedure for movement analysis in handwriting publication-title: J. Neurosci. Methods – volume: 35 start-page: 3529 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB4 article-title: Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements publication-title: Vis. Res. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00058-M – volume: 48 start-page: 810 issue: 9 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB7 article-title: Motor learning and occupational therapy: the organization of practice publication-title: Am. J. Occup. Ther. doi: 10.5014/ajot.48.9.810 – volume: 74 start-page: 738 issue: 8 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB14 article-title: Current status of the motor program publication-title: Phys. Ther. doi: 10.1093/ptj/74.8.738 – volume: 25 start-page: 2176 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB17 article-title: Asymmetric bilateral transfer of motor adaptation in a visually-guided reaching movement publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – volume: 101 start-page: 41 issue: 1 year: 1987 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB1 article-title: Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills publication-title: Psychol. Bull. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.41 – volume: 67 start-page: 563 issue: 2 year: 1988 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB6 article-title: Does transfer of training help children learn juggling publication-title: Percept. Mot. Skills doi: 10.2466/pms.1988.67.2.563 – volume: 47 start-page: 381 year: 1954 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB5 article-title: The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/h0055392 – volume: 15 start-page: 1061 issue: 6 year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB20 article-title: On learning complex procedural knowledge publication-title: Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1061 – volume: 126 start-page: 235 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB19 article-title: Inhibitory control of competing motor memories publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/s002210050733 – start-page: 307 year: 1996 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB10 article-title: Das vernachlässigte Verhalten: Kinematische Analysen der Schreibbewegungen beim Schreibkrampf – volume: 37 start-page: 538 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB11 article-title: Analyse und Therapie motorischer Schreibstoerungen analysis and therapy of disturbed handwriting movements publication-title: Psychol. Beitraege – volume: 82 start-page: 89 issue: 1-3 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB15 article-title: Looking at handwriting generation from a velocity control perspective publication-title: Acta Psychol. Amst. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(93)90006-D – volume: 17 start-page: 409 issue: 1 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB18 article-title: Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor memory publication-title: J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00409.1997 – volume: 9 start-page: 117 issue: 1 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB8 article-title: Subregions within the supplementary motor area activated at different stages of movement preparation and execution publication-title: NeuroImage doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0393 – volume: 65 start-page: 31 issue: 1 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB13 article-title: Developmental modeling effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a novel motor task publication-title: Res. Q. Exerc. Sport doi: 10.1080/02701367.1994.10762205 – volume: Vol. 1 start-page: 677 year: 1955 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB2 article-title: Alphabet – volume: 52 start-page: 39 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB12 article-title: A computational procedure for movement analysis in handwriting publication-title: J. Neurosci. Methods doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(94)90053-1 – volume: 25 start-page: 2179 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB3 article-title: Proactive interference in motor learning publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – volume: 106 start-page: 283 issue: 4 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB16 article-title: The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroenceph publication-title: clin. Neurophysiol. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00022-4 – volume: 39 start-page: 575 issue: 3 year: 1968 ident: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X_BIB9 article-title: Transfer of movement components in a motor learning task publication-title: Research. Quaterly |
SSID | ssj0005154 |
Score | 1.6464772 |
Snippet | Many automatic movements are open-loop, feed-forward motor programs (MP) that are kinematically well characterized by smooth speed and acceleration curves.... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 33 |
SubjectTerms | Automatization Biological and medical sciences Biomechanical Phenomena Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Handwriting Humans Language Learning Learning - physiology Motor program Movement - physiology Production and perception of written language Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Transfer Writing |
Title | Learning to write letters: transfer in automated movements indicates modularity of motor programs in human subjects |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00851-X https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10713389 https://www.proquest.com/docview/17638836 https://www.proquest.com/docview/70976326 |
Volume | 282 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3da9swEBelhTEYY2v3ka7t9DDG9uBGjmVL7lsoK9nG-rIV_CYk2RqB1g6xw-jL_vbdSUrTPoTCwC8WkizrTqff6U53hHxwzHKrLWYNsCLhtSkSyXOdGC2ZEcZhjHH0trgsZlf8W5VXO-R8fRcG3Sqj7A8y3UvrWDKOszlezOfjn2jU83m9A26o8AY7F8jrp3_vuXmkeQghhRYAqL25xRN68IWfGPvsO0mqbfvTs4XuYdZcSHexHY_6feniBXkeASWdhjG_JDtNu08Opi0o0ze39CP1Lp7-7HyfPPkRLekHpI-BVX_ToaN_lgA86bW_2dOf0cGD2WZJ5y3VK-gH8GhNbzofWnzoKZq50ZGqh7Ia3VgBydPOwRso8DR6fGE16jMA0n5l8LSnf0WuLr78Op8lMQFDYvkkHRKQorzMbFo4AGE2Q30tA_wiC1uYXMvUAfxrdN5Yl6a6BE3GaFEzJ2rjtBVZnr0mu23XNm8JFaIUtswAfTWSg3w1gjclqDPG1mzipBwRvp52ZWN0ckySca02bmhALYXUUgwfoJaqRuT0rtkihOd4rIFc01Q94DMFW8hjTY8f8MDmg9mEScFH5P2aJxSsUTS86LbpVr1KQYhLmRXbawgGsBCQ9Ii8Ccx072_8MUJ5-P8Df0eehgACGIz2iOwOy1VzDFBqMCd-rZyQvenX77PLf7LDGIY |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3da9swED-6FLZBGVu7j2ztqocxtgcvcixbct9CWUnXNi9rwW9Ckq0RaO0QO4z99z3JyrI-hMLALxY6WdZJp9_pTncAnyw1zCjjsgYYHrFSZ5FgqYq0ElRzbV2McedtMcumN-xHkRY7cLq-C-PcKoPs72W6l9ahZBRGc7SYz0c_nVHP5_XucUPxBHZZitreAHYn5xfT2cbTI077KFLOCIAEm4s8fSO-8AulX307UbFti9pbqBYHzvYZL7ZDUr81nb2EFwFTkknf7VewU9X7cDCpUZ---0M-E-_l6Y_P9-HpVTCmH0AbYqv-Il1Dfi8Re5Jbf7mnPSGdx7PVksxrolbYDkLSktw1Prp41xJn6Xa-VC2Wlc6TFcE8aSy-oQ5PgtOXq0Z8EkDSrrQ78Glfw83Z9-vTaRRyMESGjeMuQkHK8sTEmUUcZhKnsiUIYURmMp0qEVtEgJVKK2PjWOWozGjFS2p5qa0yPEmTNzCom7p6B4TznJs8QQBWCYYiVnNW5ajRaFPSsRViCGw97NKEAOUuT8at3HiiIbek45ak7kFuyWII3_6SLfoIHY8RiDVP5YOpJnEXeYz06MEc2HwwGVPB2RCO13NC4jJ1thdVV82qlTHKcSGSbHsNThEZIpgewtt-Mv3zN_4kIX___x0_hmfT66tLeXk-u_gAz_t4Ai427SEMuuWqOkJk1emPYeXcA1_2Gz0 |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Learning+to+write+letters%3A+transfer+in+automated+movements+indicates+modularity+of+motor+programs+in+human+subjects&rft.jtitle=Neuroscience+letters&rft.au=Kharraz-Tavakol%2C+O+D&rft.au=Eggert%2C+T&rft.au=Mai%2C+N&rft.au=Straube%2C+A&rft.date=2000-03-17&rft.issn=0304-3940&rft.volume=282&rft.issue=1-2&rft.spage=33&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2FS0304-3940%2800%2900851-X&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F10713389&rft.externalDocID=10713389 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0304-3940&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0304-3940&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0304-3940&client=summon |