The precedence effect for lateralization at low sensation levels

Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for five normal hearing subjects. Stimuli were based on 2400-Hz low pass filtered 5-ms noise bursts. We used the paradigm, as described by Aoki...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inHearing research Vol. 148; no. 1; pp. 88 - 94
Main Authors Goverts, S.Theo, Houtgast, Tammo, van Beek, Hans H.M.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Amsterdam Elsevier B.V 01.10.2000
Elsevier
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0378-5955
1878-5891
DOI10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for five normal hearing subjects. Stimuli were based on 2400-Hz low pass filtered 5-ms noise bursts. We used the paradigm, as described by Aoki and Houtgast (Hear. Res., 59 (1992) 25–30) and Houtgast and Aoki (Hear. Res., 72 (1994) 29–36), in which the stimulus is divided into a leading and a lagging part with opposite lateralization cues (i.e. an interaural time delay of 0.2 ms). The occurrence of onset dominance was investigated by measuring lateral perception of the stimulus, with fixed equal duration of leading and lagging part, while decreasing absolute signal level or adding a filtered white noise with the signal level set at 65 dBA. The dominance of the leading part was quantified by measuring the perceived lateral position of the stimulus as a function of the relative duration of the leading (and thus the lagging) part. This was done at about 45 dB SL without masking noise and also at a signal-to-noise ratio resulting in a sensation level of 10 dB. The occurrence and strength of the precedence effect was found to depend on sensation level, which was decreased either by lowering the signal level or by adding noise. With the present paradigm, besides a decreased lateralization accuracy, a decrease in the precedence effect was found for sensation levels below about 30–40 dB. In daily-life conditions, with a sensation level in noise of typically 10 dB, the onset dominance was still manifest, albeit degraded to some extent.
AbstractList Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for five normal hearing subjects. Stimuli were based on 2400-Hz low pass filtered 5-ms noise bursts. We used the paradigm, as described by Aoki and Houtgast (Hear. Res., 59 (1992) 25-30) and Houtgast and Aoki (Hear. Res., 72 (1994) 29-36), in which the stimulus is divided into a leading and a lagging part with opposite lateralization cues (i.e. an interaural time delay of 0.2 ms). The occurrence of onset dominance was investigated by measuring lateral perception of the stimulus, with fixed equal duration of leading and lagging part, while decreasing absolute signal level or adding a filtered white noise with the signal level set at 65 dBA. The dominance of the leading part was quantified by measuring the perceived lateral position of the stimulus as a function of the relative duration of the leading (and thus the lagging) part. This was done at about 45 dB SL without masking noise and also at a signal-to-noise ratio resulting in a sensation level of 10 dB. The occurrence and strength of the precedence effect was found to depend on sensation level, which was decreased either by lowering the signal level or by adding noise. With the present paradigm, besides a decreased lateralization accuracy, a decrease in the precedence effect was found for sensation levels below about 30-40 dB. In daily-life conditions, with a sensation level in noise of typically 10 dB, the onset dominance was still manifest, albeit degraded to some extent.
Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for five normal hearing subjects. Stimuli were based on 2400-Hz low pass filtered 5-ms noise bursts. We used the paradigm, as described by Aoki and Houtgast (Hear. Res., 59 (1992) 25–30) and Houtgast and Aoki (Hear. Res., 72 (1994) 29–36), in which the stimulus is divided into a leading and a lagging part with opposite lateralization cues (i.e. an interaural time delay of 0.2 ms). The occurrence of onset dominance was investigated by measuring lateral perception of the stimulus, with fixed equal duration of leading and lagging part, while decreasing absolute signal level or adding a filtered white noise with the signal level set at 65 dBA. The dominance of the leading part was quantified by measuring the perceived lateral position of the stimulus as a function of the relative duration of the leading (and thus the lagging) part. This was done at about 45 dB SL without masking noise and also at a signal-to-noise ratio resulting in a sensation level of 10 dB. The occurrence and strength of the precedence effect was found to depend on sensation level, which was decreased either by lowering the signal level or by adding noise. With the present paradigm, besides a decreased lateralization accuracy, a decrease in the precedence effect was found for sensation levels below about 30–40 dB. In daily-life conditions, with a sensation level in noise of typically 10 dB, the onset dominance was still manifest, albeit degraded to some extent.
Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for five normal hearing subjects. Stimuli were based on 2400-Hz low pass filtered 5-ms noise bursts. We used the paradigm, as described by Aoki and Houtgast (Hear. Res., 59 (1992) 25-30) and Houtgast and Aoki (Hear. Res., 72 (1994) 29-36), in which the stimulus is divided into a leading and a lagging part with opposite lateralization cues (i.e. an interaural time delay of 0.2 ms). The occurrence of onset dominance was investigated by measuring lateral perception of the stimulus, with fixed equal duration of leading and lagging part, while decreasing absolute signal level or adding a filtered white noise with the signal level set at 65 dBA. The dominance of the leading part was quantified by measuring the perceived lateral position of the stimulus as a function of the relative duration of the leading (and thus the lagging) part. This was done at about 45 dB SL without masking noise and also at a signal-to-noise ratio resulting in a sensation level of 10 dB. The occurrence and strength of the precedence effect was found to depend on sensation level, which was decreased either by lowering the signal level or by adding noise. With the present paradigm, besides a decreased lateralization accuracy, a decrease in the precedence effect was found for sensation levels below about 30-40 dB. In daily-life conditions, with a sensation level in noise of typically 10 dB, the onset dominance was still manifest, albeit degraded to some extent.Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for five normal hearing subjects. Stimuli were based on 2400-Hz low pass filtered 5-ms noise bursts. We used the paradigm, as described by Aoki and Houtgast (Hear. Res., 59 (1992) 25-30) and Houtgast and Aoki (Hear. Res., 72 (1994) 29-36), in which the stimulus is divided into a leading and a lagging part with opposite lateralization cues (i.e. an interaural time delay of 0.2 ms). The occurrence of onset dominance was investigated by measuring lateral perception of the stimulus, with fixed equal duration of leading and lagging part, while decreasing absolute signal level or adding a filtered white noise with the signal level set at 65 dBA. The dominance of the leading part was quantified by measuring the perceived lateral position of the stimulus as a function of the relative duration of the leading (and thus the lagging) part. This was done at about 45 dB SL without masking noise and also at a signal-to-noise ratio resulting in a sensation level of 10 dB. The occurrence and strength of the precedence effect was found to depend on sensation level, which was decreased either by lowering the signal level or by adding noise. With the present paradigm, besides a decreased lateralization accuracy, a decrease in the precedence effect was found for sensation levels below about 30-40 dB. In daily-life conditions, with a sensation level in noise of typically 10 dB, the onset dominance was still manifest, albeit degraded to some extent.
Author Houtgast, Tammo
Goverts, S.Theo
van Beek, Hans H.M.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: S.Theo
  surname: Goverts
  fullname: Goverts, S.Theo
  email: st.goverts@azvu.nl
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Tammo
  surname: Houtgast
  fullname: Houtgast, Tammo
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Hans H.M.
  surname: van Beek
  fullname: van Beek, Hans H.M.
BackLink http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=807060$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10978827$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkV1rFDEUhoNU7Lb6E5QBQerF6EnmIwleqBS_oOCFFXoXzmROMJKdWZNsRX99M7trBS_sVeDkeV-S85ywo2meiLHHHF5w4P3LL9BIVXe6684AngPwtqmv7rEVV8tYaX7EVrfIMTtJ6XuBuqYVD9gxBy2VEnLF3lx-o2oTydJIk6WKnCObKzfHKmCmiMH_xuznqcJchflnlWhK-0GgawrpIbvvMCR6dDhP2df37y7PP9YXnz98On97UdtWQK4HcGBROA2jbuwgJbVcoLJatnLgAjqkDgc3Sj1Sue0d9r0CIQuvnLLYnLJn-95NnH9sKWWz9slSCDjRvE1Gla-1qtF3glKItun0Aj45gNthTaPZRL_G-Mv8WU4Bnh4ATBaDizhZn245BRJ6KNSrPWXjnFIkZ6zPuw3liD6UOrMIMzthZrFhAMxOmLkq6e6f9N9n_D_3ep8rCujaUzTJ-sXg6IvMbMbZ39FwA_a8rOI
CODEN HERED3
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1121_1_1558391
crossref_primary_10_1121_1_3372716
crossref_primary_10_1121_10_0023937
crossref_primary_10_1097_AUD_0b013e31818713a8
crossref_primary_10_1121_1_3598466
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0378_5955_01_00385_9
crossref_primary_10_1121_1_1578079
crossref_primary_10_1121_1_4869682
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0907_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heares_2004_01_003
crossref_primary_10_1121_1_2345913
Cites_doi 10.1121/1.405812
10.1121/1.1911178
10.1016/0378-5955(94)90202-X
10.1121/1.383974
10.1121/1.389394
10.1121/1.394282
10.1016/0378-5955(92)90098-8
10.1007/978-1-4612-4738-8_4
10.1121/1.427914
10.1121/1.388956
10.1121/1.389020
10.2307/1418275
10.7551/mitpress/6391.001.0001
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2000 Elsevier Science B.V.
2001 INIST-CNRS
Copyright_xml – notice: 2000 Elsevier Science B.V.
– notice: 2001 INIST-CNRS
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
8BM
DOI 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X
DatabaseName CrossRef
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
ComDisDome
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ComDisDome
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE

ComDisDome
ComDisDome
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1878-5891
EndPage 94
ExternalDocumentID 10978827
807060
10_1016_S0378_5955_00_00143_X
S037859550000143X
Genre Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
--Z
.GJ
.~1
0R~
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29I
4.4
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
7-5
71M
8P~
9JM
AACTN
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABTAH
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACIUM
ACRLP
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADMUD
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
C45
CS3
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
G8K
GBLVA
HMQ
HVGLF
HZ~
IHE
J1W
KOM
M2V
M41
MO0
MOBAO
N9A
NCXOZ
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OVD
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SNS
SPCBC
SSN
SSZ
T5K
TEORI
TN5
UNMZH
WUQ
ZGI
ZY4
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDPE
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADNMO
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
EFKBS
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
8BM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c420t-b0f0ca2f90d93cb77e412a8c9747b1205ae5abfd79deb776fa66802790d8f8ca3
IEDL.DBID AIKHN
ISSN 0378-5955
IngestDate Thu Jul 10 22:56:09 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 00:20:02 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:37:03 EST 2025
Mon Jul 21 09:15:42 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:12:46 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:56:38 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:30:56 EST 2024
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 1
Keywords Localization dominance
Lateralization accuracy
Precedence effect
Binaural hearing
Normal hearing
Auditory perception
Human
Acoustic stimulus
Stimulus localization
Dominance
Perception
Laterality
Stimulus position
Language English
License https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0
CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c420t-b0f0ca2f90d93cb77e412a8c9747b1205ae5abfd79deb776fa66802790d8f8ca3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ObjectType-Article-2
ObjectType-Feature-1
PMID 10978827
PQID 72243599
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 7
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_85344839
proquest_miscellaneous_72243599
pubmed_primary_10978827
pascalfrancis_primary_807060
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_S0378_5955_00_00143_X
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0378_5955_00_00143_X
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_S0378_5955_00_00143_X
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2000-10-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2000-10-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 10
  year: 2000
  text: 2000-10-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace Amsterdam
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Amsterdam
– name: Netherlands
PublicationTitle Hearing research
PublicationTitleAlternate Hear Res
PublicationYear 2000
Publisher Elsevier B.V
Elsevier
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier B.V
– name: Elsevier
References Litovski, Colburn, Yost, Guzman (BIB9) 1999; 106
Hafter, E.R., Buell, T.N., Richards, V., 1988. Onset-coding in lateralization: its form, site and function. In: Edelman, G.M., Gall, W.E., Cowan, W.M. (Eds.), Functions of the Auditory System, Wiley, New York.
Rakerd, Hartmann (BIB11) 1986; 80
Abel, Kunov (BIB1) 1983; 73
Shinn-Cunningham, Zurek, Durlach (BIB10) 1993; 93
Houtgast, Aoki (BIB8) 1994; 72
Zurek, P.M., 1987. The precedence effect. In: Directional Hearing (Yost, W.A., Gourevitch, G., Eds.), Springer, New York, pp. 85–105.
Wallach, Newman, Rosenzweig (BIB12) 1949; LXII
Blauert, J., 1997. Spatial Hearing: the Psychophics of Human Sound Localization. Revised Edition, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Hafter, Dye, Wenzel (BIB6) 1983; 73
Zurek (BIB13) 1980; 67
Hafter, Dye (BIB5) 1983; 73
Haas, H., 1951. The influence of a single echo on the audibility of speech. J. Audiol. Eng. Soc., 20, 145–159, English translation (1972).
Houtgast, Plomp (BIB15) 1968; 44
Aoki, Houtgast (BIB2) 1992; 59
10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB4
Litovski (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB9) 1999; 106
Hafter (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB5) 1983; 73
Aoki (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB2) 1992; 59
10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB7
Houtgast (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB15) 1968; 44
10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB3
Houtgast (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB8) 1994; 72
Zurek (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB13) 1980; 67
Abel (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB1) 1983; 73
Rakerd (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB11) 1986; 80
Shinn-Cunningham (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB10) 1993; 93
10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB14
Hafter (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB6) 1983; 73
Wallach (10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB12) 1949; LXII
References_xml – volume: 106
  start-page: 1633
  year: 1999
  end-page: 1654
  ident: BIB9
  article-title: The precedence effect (tut. review)
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– volume: 73
  start-page: 644
  year: 1983
  end-page: 651
  ident: BIB5
  article-title: Detection of interaural differences of time in trains of high-frequency clicks as a function of interclick interval and number
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– reference: Haas, H., 1951. The influence of a single echo on the audibility of speech. J. Audiol. Eng. Soc., 20, 145–159, English translation (1972).
– volume: 93
  start-page: 2923
  year: 1993
  end-page: 2932
  ident: BIB10
  article-title: Adjustment and discrimination measurement of the precedence effect
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– volume: 73
  start-page: 1708
  year: 1983
  end-page: 1713
  ident: BIB6
  article-title: Detection of interaural differences of intensity in trains of high-frequency clicks as a function of interclick interval and number
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– volume: 59
  start-page: 25
  year: 1992
  end-page: 30
  ident: BIB2
  article-title: A precedence effect in the perception of inter-aural cross correlation
  publication-title: Hear. Res.
– reference: Blauert, J., 1997. Spatial Hearing: the Psychophics of Human Sound Localization. Revised Edition, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
– volume: LXII
  start-page: 315
  year: 1949
  end-page: 336
  ident: BIB12
  article-title: The precedence effect in sound localization
  publication-title: Am. J. Psychol.
– volume: 67
  start-page: 952
  year: 1980
  end-page: 964
  ident: BIB13
  article-title: The precedence effect and its possible role in the avoidance of interaural ambiguities
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– reference: Hafter, E.R., Buell, T.N., Richards, V., 1988. Onset-coding in lateralization: its form, site and function. In: Edelman, G.M., Gall, W.E., Cowan, W.M. (Eds.), Functions of the Auditory System, Wiley, New York.
– volume: 44
  start-page: 807
  year: 1968
  end-page: 812
  ident: BIB15
  article-title: Lateralization threshold of a signal in noise
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– volume: 72
  start-page: 29
  year: 1994
  end-page: 36
  ident: BIB8
  article-title: Stimulus-onset dominance in the perception of binaural information
  publication-title: Hear. Res.
– volume: 73
  start-page: 955
  year: 1983
  end-page: 960
  ident: BIB1
  article-title: Lateralization based on interaural phase differences: effects of frequency, amplitude, duration, and shape of rise/decay
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– reference: Zurek, P.M., 1987. The precedence effect. In: Directional Hearing (Yost, W.A., Gourevitch, G., Eds.), Springer, New York, pp. 85–105.
– volume: 80
  start-page: 1695
  year: 1986
  end-page: 1706
  ident: BIB11
  article-title: Localization of sound in rooms, III: Onset and duration effects
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
– volume: 93
  start-page: 2923
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB10
  article-title: Adjustment and discrimination measurement of the precedence effect
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.405812
– ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB7
– volume: 44
  start-page: 807
  year: 1968
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB15
  article-title: Lateralization threshold of a signal in noise
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.1911178
– volume: 72
  start-page: 29
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB8
  article-title: Stimulus-onset dominance in the perception of binaural information
  publication-title: Hear. Res.
  doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(94)90202-X
– ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB4
– volume: 67
  start-page: 952
  year: 1980
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB13
  article-title: The precedence effect and its possible role in the avoidance of interaural ambiguities
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.383974
– volume: 73
  start-page: 1708
  year: 1983
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB6
  article-title: Detection of interaural differences of intensity in trains of high-frequency clicks as a function of interclick interval and number
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.389394
– volume: 80
  start-page: 1695
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB11
  article-title: Localization of sound in rooms, III: Onset and duration effects
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.394282
– volume: 59
  start-page: 25
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB2
  article-title: A precedence effect in the perception of inter-aural cross correlation
  publication-title: Hear. Res.
  doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(92)90098-8
– ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB14
  doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-4738-8_4
– volume: 106
  start-page: 1633
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB9
  article-title: The precedence effect (tut. review)
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.427914
– volume: 73
  start-page: 644
  year: 1983
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB5
  article-title: Detection of interaural differences of time in trains of high-frequency clicks as a function of interclick interval and number
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.388956
– volume: 73
  start-page: 955
  year: 1983
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB1
  article-title: Lateralization based on interaural phase differences: effects of frequency, amplitude, duration, and shape of rise/decay
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.389020
– volume: LXII
  start-page: 315
  year: 1949
  ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB12
  article-title: The precedence effect in sound localization
  publication-title: Am. J. Psychol.
  doi: 10.2307/1418275
– ident: 10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X_BIB3
  doi: 10.7551/mitpress/6391.001.0001
SSID ssj0015342
Score 1.654825
Snippet Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect) for lateralization at low sensation levels was investigated for...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 88
SubjectTerms Acoustic Stimulation - methods
Adult
Auditory perception
Auditory Perception - physiology
Auditory Threshold - physiology
Binaural hearing
Biological and medical sciences
Cues
Dichotic Listening Tests
Ear and associated structures. Auditory pathways and centers. Hearing. Vocal organ. Phonation. Sound production. Echolocation
Functional Laterality - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Hearing - physiology
Humans
Lateralization accuracy
Localization dominance
Noise
Normal hearing
Perceptual Masking - physiology
Precedence effect
Reference Values
Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs
Title The precedence effect for lateralization at low sensation levels
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0378-5955(00)00143-X
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10978827
https://www.proquest.com/docview/72243599
https://www.proquest.com/docview/85344839
Volume 148
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Na9swFH-0KYzBGGu7j2xtp8MY28GNIsm2dFsoLdlGe9kKvgnZlqDgOmFJKb3sb9_TR9L1EAq9ynqy9fQs_Z7eF8AnjhjWudZmlNUiQwlpM9UYlXHVyHpsRa6oj0Y-vyiml-JHlVdbcLKKhfFulWnvj3t62K1TyyhxczS_uhr9orz0ybnycEEteLUNO4yrQg5gZ_L95_RibUzIuYjGBFSYPMF9IE8cJDR-ofRrGCerNh1RL-ZmgYxzseLFZkgajqazV_AyYUoyiZ-9C1u234P9SY_69PUd-UyCl2e4Pt-DZ-fJmL4P31BEyBxnb0NdURJdOwiiWNIZH5jcpRhNYpakm92SBaq8saHzrkaL13B5dvr7ZJqlggpZIxhdZjV1tDHMKdoq3tRlacWYGdl4naIeM5obm5vataVqLT4tnCkKiXor9pdONoa_gUE_6-07IEIW0sqaCcmdYKh0WMRdFsFia5xpCzUEseKhblK2cV_0otP3bmXIeu1Zr6lPUYqs19UQjtdk85hu4zECuVog_UBuNB4Jj5EePFjQ9Qsl9SmFhvBxtb4afzlvRzG9nd0sdImwh-dKbe6BGAjVXo493kbB-G8yqLZLVr5_-nd_gOchH0DwKDyAwfLPjT1EZLSsj2D7-O_4KMn_P5-JBq8
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NT9swFH9iRWKTpmnAPrqx4QOatkNWN3YS-0aFQGXQXgApN8tJbAkpS6u1aNp_v-ePFjhUSFwdP8d-_vo9vy-AI4YY1trGJDSteIIrpElkrWXCZC2qoeGZpM4beTLNxzf8V5mVW3Cy8oVxZpXx7A9nuj-tY8kgcnMwv70dXFFWuOBcmX-g5qx8AdsuOhXvwfbo_GI8XSsTMsaDMgEFJkdw78gTGvGF3yn94dtJyk1X1Ou5XiDjbMh4sRmS-qvp7C28iZiSjEK3d2HLdHuwP-pQnv79j3wj3srTP5_vwc4kKtP34RiXCJnj6I3PK0qCaQdBFEta7RyT2-ijSfSStLO_ZIEibyhonanR4h3cnJ1en4yTmFAhqXlKl0lFLa11aiVtJKurojB8mGpRO5miGqY00ybTlW0K2Rj8mlud5wLlVqwvrKg1ew-9btaZj0C4yIURVcoFszxFocMg7jIIFhttdZPLPvAVD1Udo427pBetujcrQ9Yrx3pFXYhSZL0q-_BzTTYP4TaeIhCrCVKP1o3CK-Ep0oNHE7r-oaAupFAfDlfzq3DLOT2K7szsbqEKhD0sk3JzDcRAKPYyrPEhLIwHg0GxXaTFp-f3-xBejq8nl-ryfHrxGV752ADeuvAAess_d-YLoqRl9TXugv_UzwiX
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+precedence+effect+for+lateralization+at+low+sensation+levels&rft.jtitle=Hearing+research&rft.au=GOVERTS%2C+S.+Theo&rft.au=HOUTGAST%2C+Tammo&rft.au=VAN+BEEK%2C+Hans+H.+M&rft.date=2000-10-01&rft.pub=Elsevier&rft.issn=0378-5955&rft.volume=148&rft.issue=1-2&rft.spage=88&rft.epage=94&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2FS0378-5955%2800%2900143-X&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=807060
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0378-5955&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0378-5955&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0378-5955&client=summon