Experience-dependent integration of texture and motion cues to depth
Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers’ acquire these estimated re...
Saved in:
Published in | Vision research (Oxford) Vol. 39; no. 24; pp. 4062 - 4075 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Oxford
Elsevier Ltd
01.12.1999
Elsevier Science |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0042-6989 1878-5646 |
DOI | 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0 |
Cover
Abstract | Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers’ acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers’ abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers’ cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Previous investigators have shown that observers' visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers' acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers' abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers' cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others.Previous investigators have shown that observers' visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers' acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers' abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers' cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others. Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers’ acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers’ abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers’ cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others. |
Author | Fine, I Jacobs, Robert A |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Robert A surname: Jacobs fullname: Jacobs, Robert A email: robbie@bcs.rochester.edu organization: Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627, USA – sequence: 2 givenname: I surname: Fine fullname: Fine, I organization: Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1949704$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748939$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkU2P0zAYhC20iH7ATwDlgBB7CLxOnA9rDwiVLiBV4gCcLcd-A0aJXWwHdf_9ummhEgc4WbaeGY1nVuTKOouEPKXwigKtX38GYEVe85a_5PwagBaQwwOypG3T5lXN6iuy_IMsyCqEHwDQVAV_RBYUGtbyki_Ju-1hj96gVZhr3KPVaGNmbMRvXkbjbOb6LOIhTh4zaXU2uvlVTRiy6LKkid8fk4e9HAI-OZ9r8vV2-2XzId99ev9x83aXK0Z5zFUHyHQKyLHUNRbQUeCyghL6dNFKalqgZEARUWEr-x4YbbHsaKf6OnFr8uLku_fuZwoQxWiCwmGQFt0URM1Z1ZZ1lcBnZ3DqRtRi780o_Z34_e8EPD8DMig59F5aZcKF44w3wBJ2c8KUdyF47IUyca4lemmG5CeOY4h5DHFsWnAu5jHEMW71l_qS49-6Nycdpi5_GfQiqHkhbTyqKLQz_3G4B24ioU8 |
CODEN | VISRAM |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2014_08_023 crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193516 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2009_10_043 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2010_06_013 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_0506728103 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0021575 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2018_12_018 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_00_00254_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_00_00323_0 crossref_primary_10_1080_1750984X_2020_1855667 crossref_primary_10_1167_18_5_16 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnbeh_2016_00187 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10827_020_00764_3 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1713316115 crossref_primary_10_1109_TCSVT_2023_3295375 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2004_02_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107530 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10162_017_0636_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2006_07_009 crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_00002559 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_11341_7 crossref_primary_10_1068_p5066 crossref_primary_10_1016_S1364_6613_02_01948_4 crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03194782 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_03_00458_9 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3212_18_2019 crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_111_4_1072 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00088_7 crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_54172 crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_112_2_329 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_91129_2008 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4986_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2004_01_029 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797620966785 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2018_09_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neucom_2007_04_006 crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_bja10089 crossref_primary_10_1068_p5798 crossref_primary_10_1068_p3314 crossref_primary_10_1068_p5253 crossref_primary_10_1038_nrn914 crossref_primary_10_1068_p5012 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11434_008_0309_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_preteyeres_2021_101014 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00004-X 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00176-M 10.1093/cercor/5.3.247 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90201-7 10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.198 10.1080/14640748908402390 10.1364/JOSAA.5.001749 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90149-Q 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90154-9 10.3758/BF03206861 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90037-W 10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.161 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90228-O 10.3758/BF03211904 10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00415-X 10.3758/BF03213099 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90200-G 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90106-6 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd 1999 INIST-CNRS |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd – notice: 1999 INIST-CNRS |
DBID | 6I. AAFTH AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0 |
DatabaseName | ScienceDirect Open Access Titles Elsevier:ScienceDirect:Open Access CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1878-5646 |
EndPage | 4075 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10748939 1949704 10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0 S0042698999001200 |
Genre | Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS grantid: R29-MH54770 |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M -~X .55 .GJ .~1 0R~ 0SF 123 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29Q 4.4 457 4G. 53G 5RE 5VS 6I. 6PF 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM AABNK AACTN AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFTH AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAWTL AAXLA AAXUO ABBQC ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABLVK ABMAC ABMZM ABVKL ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACIUM ACNCT ACRLP ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS ADMUD AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AENEX AETEA AEXQZ AFDAS AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AHPSJ AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AJRQY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ANZVX ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC BNPGV C45 CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q G8K GBLVA HEA HMK HMO HMQ HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IHE IXB J1W K-O KOM L7B LCYCR LZ2 M29 M2V M41 MO0 MOBAO N9A NCXOZ O-L O9- OAUVE OK1 OVD OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. Q38 R2- RIG RNS ROL RPZ SAE SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SSH SSN SSZ T5K TEORI TN5 WUQ X7M XOL XPP ZA5 ZGI ZKB ZMT ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABDPE ABWVN ACIEU ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADNMO ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PKN 7X8 EFKBS |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c419t-cb0e4d1879e3d6e20b109a5030fe20dcad12ea401eeece8aff0418e3b1bcf6503 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0042-6989 |
IngestDate | Fri Sep 05 12:20:45 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:36:01 EST 2025 Thu Nov 28 05:44:48 EST 2024 Tue Jul 01 03:59:38 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:00:21 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:29:30 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 24 |
Keywords | Perception Cue combination Motion and texture cues Human Visual stimulus Acquisition process Stimulus texture Stimulus movement Vision Space perception Perceptive learning Experimental study |
Language | English |
License | http://www.elsevier.com/open-access/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c419t-cb0e4d1879e3d6e20b109a5030fe20dcad12ea401eeece8aff0418e3b1bcf6503 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698999001200 |
PMID | 10748939 |
PQID | 69458365 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 14 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_69458365 pubmed_primary_10748939 pascalfrancis_primary_1949704 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 1900 |
PublicationDate | 1999-12-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 1999-12-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 12 year: 1999 text: 1999-12-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 1990 |
PublicationPlace | Oxford |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Oxford – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Vision research (Oxford) |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Vision Res |
PublicationYear | 1999 |
Publisher | Elsevier Ltd Elsevier Science |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Ltd – name: Elsevier Science |
References | Biederman (BIB1) 1987; 94 McCullagh, Nelder (BIB17) 1989 Cumming, Johnston, Parker (BIB6) 1993; 33 Johnston, Cumming, Parker (BIB13) 1993; 33 Turner, Braunstein, Anderson (BIB23) 1997; 59 Cutting, Vishton (BIB8) 1995 Rogers, Collett (BIB21) 1989; 41 Knill (BIB14) 1998; 38 Blake, Bülthoff, Sheinberg (BIB2) 1993; 33 Maloney, Landy (BIB16) 1989; 1199 Nawrot, Blake (BIB18) 1993; 33 Perotti, Todd, Lappin, Phillips (BIB19) 1998; 60 Landy, Maloney, Johnston, Young (BIB15) 1995; 35 Cutting, Millard (BIB7) 1984; 113 Young, Landy, Maloney (BIB25) 1993; 33 Bülthoff, Mallot (BIB5) 1988; 5 Vetter, Poggio, Bülthoff (BIB24) 1994; 4 Dosher, Sperling, Wurst (BIB10) 1986; 26 Bruno, Cutting (BIB3) 1988; 117 Tittle, Norman, Perotti, Phillips (BIB22) 1997; 26 Johnston, Cumming, Landy (BIB12) 1994; 34 Hearn, Baker (BIB11) 1997 Dobson (BIB9) 1990 Bülthoff, Edelman, Tarr (BIB4) 1995; 5 Perotti, Todd, Norman (BIB20) 1996; 58 Bülthoff (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB5) 1988; 5 McCullagh (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB17) 1989 Vetter (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB24) 1994; 4 Hearn (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB11) 1997 Bruno (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB3) 1988; 117 Turner (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB23) 1997; 59 Johnston (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB13) 1993; 33 Cutting (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB7) 1984; 113 Dosher (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB10) 1986; 26 Maloney (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB16) 1989; 1199 Young (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB25) 1993; 33 Blake (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB2) 1993; 33 Perotti (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB20) 1996; 58 Nawrot (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB18) 1993; 33 Cumming (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB6) 1993; 33 Cutting (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB8) 1995 Johnston (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB12) 1994; 34 Perotti (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB19) 1998; 60 Knill (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB14) 1998; 38 Tittle (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB22) 1997; 26 Dobson (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB9) 1990 Biederman (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB1) 1987; 94 Landy (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB15) 1995; 35 Rogers (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB21) 1989; 41 Bülthoff (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB4) 1995; 5 |
References_xml | – volume: 60 start-page: 377 year: 1998 end-page: 388 ident: BIB19 article-title: The perception of surface curvature from optical motion publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – volume: 4 start-page: 18 year: 1994 end-page: 23 ident: BIB24 article-title: The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 58 start-page: 666 year: 1996 end-page: 679 ident: BIB20 article-title: The visual perception of rigid motion from constant flow fields publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – year: 1989 ident: BIB17 publication-title: Generalized linear models – volume: 5 start-page: 1749 year: 1988 end-page: 1758 ident: BIB5 article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading publication-title: Journal of the Optical Society of America – volume: 38 start-page: 2635 year: 1998 end-page: 2656 ident: BIB14 article-title: Ideal observer perturbation analysis reveals human strategies for inferring surface orientation from texture publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 26 start-page: 147 year: 1997 end-page: 166 ident: BIB22 article-title: The perception of scale-dependent and scale-independent surface structure from binocular disparity, texture and shading publication-title: Perception – volume: 26 start-page: 973 year: 1986 end-page: 990 ident: BIB10 article-title: Tradeoffs between stereopsis and proximity luminance covariance as determinants of perceived 3D structure publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 59 start-page: 370 year: 1997 end-page: 380 ident: BIB23 article-title: The relationship between binocular disparity and motion parallax in surface detection publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – year: 1995 ident: BIB8 article-title: Perceiving layout and knowing distances: the integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth publication-title: Perception of space and motion – volume: 33 start-page: 813 year: 1993 end-page: 826 ident: BIB13 article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereopsis and texture publication-title: Vision Research – year: 1997 ident: BIB11 publication-title: Computer graphics (C Version) – volume: 33 start-page: 2685 year: 1993 end-page: 2696 ident: BIB25 article-title: A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 117 start-page: 161 year: 1988 end-page: 170 ident: BIB3 article-title: Minimodularity and the perception of layout publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 33 start-page: 1723 year: 1993 end-page: 1737 ident: BIB2 article-title: Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics publication-title: Vision Research – year: 1990 ident: BIB9 publication-title: An introduction to generalized linear models – volume: 1199 start-page: 1154 year: 1989 end-page: 1163 ident: BIB16 publication-title: A statistical framework for robust fusion of depth information. Visual Communications and Image Processing IV: Proceedings of the SPIE – volume: 41 start-page: 697 year: 1989 end-page: 717 ident: BIB21 article-title: The appearance of surfaces specified by motion parallax and binocular disparity publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 113 start-page: 198 year: 1984 end-page: 216 ident: BIB7 article-title: Three gradients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 5 start-page: 247 year: 1995 end-page: 260 ident: BIB4 article-title: How are three-dimensional objects represented in the brain? publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 94 start-page: 115 year: 1987 end-page: 147 ident: BIB1 article-title: Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 34 start-page: 2259 year: 1994 end-page: 2275 ident: BIB12 article-title: Integration of motion and stereopsis cues publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 33 start-page: 1561 year: 1993 end-page: 1571 ident: BIB18 article-title: On the perceptual identity of dynamic stereopsis and kinetic depth publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 33 start-page: 827 year: 1993 end-page: 838 ident: BIB6 article-title: Effects of different texture cues on curved surfaces viewed stereoscopically publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 35 start-page: 389 year: 1995 end-page: 412 ident: BIB15 article-title: Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion publication-title: Vision Research – volume: 4 start-page: 18 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB24 article-title: The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00004-X – volume: 35 start-page: 389 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB15 article-title: Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00176-M – year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB8 article-title: Perceiving layout and knowing distances: the integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth – year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB17 – volume: 5 start-page: 247 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB4 article-title: How are three-dimensional objects represented in the brain? publication-title: Cerebral Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.3.247 – year: 1990 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB9 – volume: 33 start-page: 827 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB6 article-title: Effects of different texture cues on curved surfaces viewed stereoscopically publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90201-7 – volume: 113 start-page: 198 year: 1984 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB7 article-title: Three gradients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.198 – volume: 41 start-page: 697 year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB21 article-title: The appearance of surfaces specified by motion parallax and binocular disparity publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/14640748908402390 – volume: 5 start-page: 1749 year: 1988 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB5 article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading publication-title: Journal of the Optical Society of America doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.5.001749 – volume: 33 start-page: 1561 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB18 article-title: On the perceptual identity of dynamic stereopsis and kinetic depth publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90149-Q – volume: 26 start-page: 973 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB10 article-title: Tradeoffs between stereopsis and proximity luminance covariance as determinants of perceived 3D structure publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90154-9 – volume: 26 start-page: 147 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB22 article-title: The perception of scale-dependent and scale-independent surface structure from binocular disparity, texture and shading publication-title: Perception – volume: 60 start-page: 377 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB19 article-title: The perception of surface curvature from optical motion publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03206861 – volume: 33 start-page: 1723 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB2 article-title: Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90037-W – volume: 117 start-page: 161 year: 1988 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB3 article-title: Minimodularity and the perception of layout publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.161 – volume: 33 start-page: 2685 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB25 article-title: A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90228-O – volume: 59 start-page: 370 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB23 article-title: The relationship between binocular disparity and motion parallax in surface detection publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211904 – volume: 94 start-page: 115 year: 1987 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB1 article-title: Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115 – volume: 38 start-page: 2635 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB14 article-title: Ideal observer perturbation analysis reveals human strategies for inferring surface orientation from texture publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00415-X – volume: 58 start-page: 666 year: 1996 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB20 article-title: The visual perception of rigid motion from constant flow fields publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03213099 – volume: 33 start-page: 813 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB13 article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereopsis and texture publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90200-G – volume: 1199 start-page: 1154 year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB16 publication-title: A statistical framework for robust fusion of depth information. Visual Communications and Image Processing IV: Proceedings of the SPIE – year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB11 – volume: 34 start-page: 2259 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB12 article-title: Integration of motion and stereopsis cues publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90106-6 |
SSID | ssj0007529 |
Score | 1.8814203 |
Snippet | Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the... Previous investigators have shown that observers' visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 4062 |
SubjectTerms | Biological and medical sciences Cue combination Cues Depth Perception - physiology Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Learning - physiology Models, Psychological Motion and texture cues Motion Perception - physiology Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology Perception Photic Stimulation - methods Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Vision |
Title | Experience-dependent integration of texture and motion cues to depth |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748939 https://www.proquest.com/docview/69458365 |
Volume | 39 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnR1LS_QwcBAFEUR8fbo-c_j48DtU0276yHF9sSp6UthbyBMF7S7aPXjxtztJW6sHEbwUEjrpdGaSzCTzAPhrZN_lxtHIGi0jpiSLpDISH4VNUYIKpnw08vVNNrxjl6N0NAMnbSyMd6ts1v56TQ-rddNz1FDzaPLw4GN8fRgm2gs8RIB6u52x3Mv64Vvn5pGnCW_DUPzbXRRPPULoPOD8fxgkot_tT4sT-YJUc3W5i-_10bAvnS_DUqNQkkGN8wrM2HIV1gYlGtNPr-QfCS6e4ex8Feavm5v0NTjtchxHbSHcirTJI5BZZOyI9wqZPlsiS0Pqcj9EIzKkGhOEqe7X4e787PZkGDUVFSLNYl5FWlHLjC8wbvsmswlVMeUyxYnusIHcMnFiJZpc1lptC-kcZXFh-ypW2qEu1_8Ds-W4tJtAUFUwVGeJMSpmuYyV0mj5MZO4jGVFznrAWjoK3aQb91UvHkXnV4bkF578gnMRyC9oDw4_wCZ1vo2fAIqWSeKL4AjcE34C3f3C1O6DnPGc4i_st0wWOOn8TYos7Xj6ggP56-Ys7cFGzftPuIZ0Pnzr92htw0LIDRF8ZnZgtnqe2l3UfCq1F0R7D-YGF1fDG2xdjI7fAdDw_l0 |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnR3LTuMwcIRYCZBWK950l4cPCMEh1Emdh488VR7lBFJvlp8CCdKKpof9-x07SbscEBKXSLEyzmTGHs9kXgCHRvZcbhyNrNEyYkqySCoj8VLYFFdQwZTPRh48ZP0ndjtMhwtw0ebC-LDKRvbXMj1I62ak21CzO3558Tm-Pg0T7QUeMkDRbv-B2kDmC-jfDM9n4jhPE97mofjH52k89RRh8JjzkzBLRD87oH6O5QTJ5up-F58rpOFgul6FX41GSc5qpNdgwZbrsHFWojX99pcckRDjGX6er8PSoHGlb8DlvMhx1HbCrUhbPQK5RUaO-LCQ6bslsjSk7vdDNCJDqhFBmOp5E56urx4v-lHTUiHSLOZVpBW1zPgO47ZnMptQFVMuU9zpDm-QXSZOrESby1qrbSGdoywubE_FSjtU5npbsFiOSrsDBHUFQ3WWGKNilstYKY2mHzOJy1hW5KwDrKWj0E29cd_24lXMA8uQ_MKTX3AuAvkF7cDpDGxcF9z4CqBomSQ-rByBh8JXoHsfmDp_IWc8p_gJBy2TBe4670qRpR1NJziR9zdnaQe2a97_h2uo58N_fx-tA1juPw7uxf3Nw90fWAmFIkIAzS4sVu9Tu4dqUKX2wzL_B8NX_vk |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Experience-dependent+integration+of+texture+and+motion+cues+to+depth&rft.jtitle=Vision+research+%28Oxford%29&rft.au=Jacobs%2C+Robert+A&rft.au=Fine%2C+I&rft.date=1999-12-01&rft.pub=Elsevier+Ltd&rft.issn=0042-6989&rft.eissn=1878-5646&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=24&rft.spage=4062&rft.epage=4075&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2FS0042-6989%2899%2900120-0&rft.externalDocID=S0042698999001200 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0042-6989&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0042-6989&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0042-6989&client=summon |