Experience-dependent integration of texture and motion cues to depth

Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers’ acquire these estimated re...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inVision research (Oxford) Vol. 39; no. 24; pp. 4062 - 4075
Main Authors Jacobs, Robert A, Fine, I
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Oxford Elsevier Ltd 01.12.1999
Elsevier Science
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0042-6989
1878-5646
DOI10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0

Cover

Abstract Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers’ acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers’ abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers’ cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others.
AbstractList Previous investigators have shown that observers' visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers' acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers' abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers' cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others.Previous investigators have shown that observers' visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers' acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers' abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers' cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others.
Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the basis of the estimated reliabilities of the visual cues. However, these researchers have not addressed how observers’ acquire these estimated reliabilities. This article studies observers’ abilities to learn cue combination strategies. Subjects made depth judgments about simulated cylinders whose shapes were indicated by motion and texture cues. Because the two cues could indicate different shapes, it was possible to design tasks in which one cue provided useful information for making depth judgments, whereas the other cue was irrelevant. The results of experiment 1 suggest that observers’ cue combination strategies are adaptable as a function of training; subjects adjusted their cue combination rules to use a cue more heavily when the cue was informative on a task versus when the cue was irrelevant. Experiment 2 demonstrated that experience-dependent adaptation of cue combination rules is context-sensitive. On trials with presentations of short cylinders, one cue was informative, whereas on trials with presentations of tall cylinders, the other cue was informative. The results suggest that observers can learn multiple cue combination rules, and can learn to apply each rule in the appropriate context. Experiment 3 demonstrated a possible limitation on the context-sensitivity of adaptation of cue combination rules. One cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a left oblique orientation, whereas the other cue was informative on trials with presentations of cylinders at a right oblique orientation. The results indicate that observers did not learn to use different cue combination rules in different contexts under these circumstances. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that observers' visual systems are biased to learn to perceive in the same way views of bilaterally symmetric objects that differ solely by a symmetry transformation. Taken in conjunction with the results of Experiment 2, this means that the visual learning mechanism underlying cue combination adaptation is biased such that some sets of statistics are more easily learned than others.
Author Fine, I
Jacobs, Robert A
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Robert A
  surname: Jacobs
  fullname: Jacobs, Robert A
  email: robbie@bcs.rochester.edu
  organization: Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627, USA
– sequence: 2
  givenname: I
  surname: Fine
  fullname: Fine, I
  organization: Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
BackLink http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1949704$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748939$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkU2P0zAYhC20iH7ATwDlgBB7CLxOnA9rDwiVLiBV4gCcLcd-A0aJXWwHdf_9ummhEgc4WbaeGY1nVuTKOouEPKXwigKtX38GYEVe85a_5PwagBaQwwOypG3T5lXN6iuy_IMsyCqEHwDQVAV_RBYUGtbyki_Ju-1hj96gVZhr3KPVaGNmbMRvXkbjbOb6LOIhTh4zaXU2uvlVTRiy6LKkid8fk4e9HAI-OZ9r8vV2-2XzId99ev9x83aXK0Z5zFUHyHQKyLHUNRbQUeCyghL6dNFKalqgZEARUWEr-x4YbbHsaKf6OnFr8uLku_fuZwoQxWiCwmGQFt0URM1Z1ZZ1lcBnZ3DqRtRi780o_Z34_e8EPD8DMig59F5aZcKF44w3wBJ2c8KUdyF47IUyca4lemmG5CeOY4h5DHFsWnAu5jHEMW71l_qS49-6Nycdpi5_GfQiqHkhbTyqKLQz_3G4B24ioU8
CODEN VISRAM
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2014_08_023
crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193516
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2009_10_043
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2010_06_013
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_0506728103
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0021575
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2018_12_018
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_00_00254_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_00_00323_0
crossref_primary_10_1080_1750984X_2020_1855667
crossref_primary_10_1167_18_5_16
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnbeh_2016_00187
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10827_020_00764_3
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1713316115
crossref_primary_10_1109_TCSVT_2023_3295375
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2004_02_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107530
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10162_017_0636_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2006_07_009
crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_00002559
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_11341_7
crossref_primary_10_1068_p5066
crossref_primary_10_1016_S1364_6613_02_01948_4
crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03194782
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_03_00458_9
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3212_18_2019
crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_111_4_1072
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00088_7
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_54172
crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_112_2_329
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_91129_2008
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4986_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2004_01_029
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797620966785
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2018_09_001
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neucom_2007_04_006
crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_bja10089
crossref_primary_10_1068_p5798
crossref_primary_10_1068_p3314
crossref_primary_10_1068_p5253
crossref_primary_10_1038_nrn914
crossref_primary_10_1068_p5012
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11434_008_0309_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_preteyeres_2021_101014
Cites_doi 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00004-X
10.1016/0042-6989(94)00176-M
10.1093/cercor/5.3.247
10.1016/0042-6989(93)90201-7
10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.198
10.1080/14640748908402390
10.1364/JOSAA.5.001749
10.1016/0042-6989(93)90149-Q
10.1016/0042-6989(86)90154-9
10.3758/BF03206861
10.1016/0042-6989(93)90037-W
10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.161
10.1016/0042-6989(93)90228-O
10.3758/BF03211904
10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115
10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00415-X
10.3758/BF03213099
10.1016/0042-6989(93)90200-G
10.1016/0042-6989(94)90106-6
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd
1999 INIST-CNRS
Copyright_xml – notice: 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd
– notice: 1999 INIST-CNRS
DBID 6I.
AAFTH
AAYXX
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
DOI 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0
DatabaseName ScienceDirect Open Access Titles
Elsevier:ScienceDirect:Open Access
CrossRef
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic

MEDLINE
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1878-5646
EndPage 4075
ExternalDocumentID 10748939
1949704
10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0
S0042698999001200
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS
  grantid: R29-MH54770
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
-~X
.55
.GJ
.~1
0R~
0SF
123
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29Q
4.4
457
4G.
53G
5RE
5VS
6I.
6PF
7-5
71M
8P~
9JM
AABNK
AACTN
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFTH
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAWTL
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABBQC
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABLVK
ABMAC
ABMZM
ABVKL
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACIUM
ACNCT
ACRLP
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADIYS
ADMUD
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AETEA
AEXQZ
AFDAS
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AHPSJ
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
AJRQY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
ANZVX
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
BNPGV
C45
CS3
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
G8K
GBLVA
HEA
HMK
HMO
HMQ
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IHE
IXB
J1W
K-O
KOM
L7B
LCYCR
LZ2
M29
M2V
M41
MO0
MOBAO
N9A
NCXOZ
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OK1
OVD
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
Q38
R2-
RIG
RNS
ROL
RPZ
SAE
SCC
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SNS
SPCBC
SSH
SSN
SSZ
T5K
TEORI
TN5
WUQ
X7M
XOL
XPP
ZA5
ZGI
ZKB
ZMT
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDPE
ABWVN
ACIEU
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADNMO
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PKN
7X8
EFKBS
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c419t-cb0e4d1879e3d6e20b109a5030fe20dcad12ea401eeece8aff0418e3b1bcf6503
IEDL.DBID .~1
ISSN 0042-6989
IngestDate Fri Sep 05 12:20:45 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:36:01 EST 2025
Thu Nov 28 05:44:48 EST 2024
Tue Jul 01 03:59:38 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:00:21 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:29:30 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 24
Keywords Perception
Cue combination
Motion and texture cues
Human
Visual stimulus
Acquisition process
Stimulus texture
Stimulus movement
Vision
Space perception
Perceptive learning
Experimental study
Language English
License http://www.elsevier.com/open-access/userlicense/1.0
CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c419t-cb0e4d1879e3d6e20b109a5030fe20dcad12ea401eeece8aff0418e3b1bcf6503
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0042698999001200
PMID 10748939
PQID 69458365
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 14
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_69458365
pubmed_primary_10748939
pascalfrancis_primary_1949704
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_S0042_6989_99_00120_0
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 1900
PublicationDate 1999-12-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 1999-12-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 12
  year: 1999
  text: 1999-12-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 1990
PublicationPlace Oxford
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Oxford
– name: England
PublicationTitle Vision research (Oxford)
PublicationTitleAlternate Vision Res
PublicationYear 1999
Publisher Elsevier Ltd
Elsevier Science
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Ltd
– name: Elsevier Science
References Biederman (BIB1) 1987; 94
McCullagh, Nelder (BIB17) 1989
Cumming, Johnston, Parker (BIB6) 1993; 33
Johnston, Cumming, Parker (BIB13) 1993; 33
Turner, Braunstein, Anderson (BIB23) 1997; 59
Cutting, Vishton (BIB8) 1995
Rogers, Collett (BIB21) 1989; 41
Knill (BIB14) 1998; 38
Blake, Bülthoff, Sheinberg (BIB2) 1993; 33
Maloney, Landy (BIB16) 1989; 1199
Nawrot, Blake (BIB18) 1993; 33
Perotti, Todd, Lappin, Phillips (BIB19) 1998; 60
Landy, Maloney, Johnston, Young (BIB15) 1995; 35
Cutting, Millard (BIB7) 1984; 113
Young, Landy, Maloney (BIB25) 1993; 33
Bülthoff, Mallot (BIB5) 1988; 5
Vetter, Poggio, Bülthoff (BIB24) 1994; 4
Dosher, Sperling, Wurst (BIB10) 1986; 26
Bruno, Cutting (BIB3) 1988; 117
Tittle, Norman, Perotti, Phillips (BIB22) 1997; 26
Johnston, Cumming, Landy (BIB12) 1994; 34
Hearn, Baker (BIB11) 1997
Dobson (BIB9) 1990
Bülthoff, Edelman, Tarr (BIB4) 1995; 5
Perotti, Todd, Norman (BIB20) 1996; 58
Bülthoff (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB5) 1988; 5
McCullagh (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB17) 1989
Vetter (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB24) 1994; 4
Hearn (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB11) 1997
Bruno (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB3) 1988; 117
Turner (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB23) 1997; 59
Johnston (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB13) 1993; 33
Cutting (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB7) 1984; 113
Dosher (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB10) 1986; 26
Maloney (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB16) 1989; 1199
Young (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB25) 1993; 33
Blake (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB2) 1993; 33
Perotti (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB20) 1996; 58
Nawrot (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB18) 1993; 33
Cumming (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB6) 1993; 33
Cutting (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB8) 1995
Johnston (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB12) 1994; 34
Perotti (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB19) 1998; 60
Knill (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB14) 1998; 38
Tittle (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB22) 1997; 26
Dobson (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB9) 1990
Biederman (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB1) 1987; 94
Landy (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB15) 1995; 35
Rogers (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB21) 1989; 41
Bülthoff (10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB4) 1995; 5
References_xml – volume: 60
  start-page: 377
  year: 1998
  end-page: 388
  ident: BIB19
  article-title: The perception of surface curvature from optical motion
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
– volume: 4
  start-page: 18
  year: 1994
  end-page: 23
  ident: BIB24
  article-title: The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition
  publication-title: Current Biology
– volume: 58
  start-page: 666
  year: 1996
  end-page: 679
  ident: BIB20
  article-title: The visual perception of rigid motion from constant flow fields
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
– year: 1989
  ident: BIB17
  publication-title: Generalized linear models
– volume: 5
  start-page: 1749
  year: 1988
  end-page: 1758
  ident: BIB5
  article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading
  publication-title: Journal of the Optical Society of America
– volume: 38
  start-page: 2635
  year: 1998
  end-page: 2656
  ident: BIB14
  article-title: Ideal observer perturbation analysis reveals human strategies for inferring surface orientation from texture
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 26
  start-page: 147
  year: 1997
  end-page: 166
  ident: BIB22
  article-title: The perception of scale-dependent and scale-independent surface structure from binocular disparity, texture and shading
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 26
  start-page: 973
  year: 1986
  end-page: 990
  ident: BIB10
  article-title: Tradeoffs between stereopsis and proximity luminance covariance as determinants of perceived 3D structure
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 59
  start-page: 370
  year: 1997
  end-page: 380
  ident: BIB23
  article-title: The relationship between binocular disparity and motion parallax in surface detection
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
– year: 1995
  ident: BIB8
  article-title: Perceiving layout and knowing distances: the integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth
  publication-title: Perception of space and motion
– volume: 33
  start-page: 813
  year: 1993
  end-page: 826
  ident: BIB13
  article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereopsis and texture
  publication-title: Vision Research
– year: 1997
  ident: BIB11
  publication-title: Computer graphics (C Version)
– volume: 33
  start-page: 2685
  year: 1993
  end-page: 2696
  ident: BIB25
  article-title: A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 117
  start-page: 161
  year: 1988
  end-page: 170
  ident: BIB3
  article-title: Minimodularity and the perception of layout
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1723
  year: 1993
  end-page: 1737
  ident: BIB2
  article-title: Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics
  publication-title: Vision Research
– year: 1990
  ident: BIB9
  publication-title: An introduction to generalized linear models
– volume: 1199
  start-page: 1154
  year: 1989
  end-page: 1163
  ident: BIB16
  publication-title: A statistical framework for robust fusion of depth information. Visual Communications and Image Processing IV: Proceedings of the SPIE
– volume: 41
  start-page: 697
  year: 1989
  end-page: 717
  ident: BIB21
  article-title: The appearance of surfaces specified by motion parallax and binocular disparity
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 113
  start-page: 198
  year: 1984
  end-page: 216
  ident: BIB7
  article-title: Three gradients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 5
  start-page: 247
  year: 1995
  end-page: 260
  ident: BIB4
  article-title: How are three-dimensional objects represented in the brain?
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
– volume: 94
  start-page: 115
  year: 1987
  end-page: 147
  ident: BIB1
  article-title: Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 34
  start-page: 2259
  year: 1994
  end-page: 2275
  ident: BIB12
  article-title: Integration of motion and stereopsis cues
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1561
  year: 1993
  end-page: 1571
  ident: BIB18
  article-title: On the perceptual identity of dynamic stereopsis and kinetic depth
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 33
  start-page: 827
  year: 1993
  end-page: 838
  ident: BIB6
  article-title: Effects of different texture cues on curved surfaces viewed stereoscopically
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 35
  start-page: 389
  year: 1995
  end-page: 412
  ident: BIB15
  article-title: Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 4
  start-page: 18
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB24
  article-title: The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00004-X
– volume: 35
  start-page: 389
  year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB15
  article-title: Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00176-M
– year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB8
  article-title: Perceiving layout and knowing distances: the integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth
– year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB17
– volume: 5
  start-page: 247
  year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB4
  article-title: How are three-dimensional objects represented in the brain?
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.3.247
– year: 1990
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB9
– volume: 33
  start-page: 827
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB6
  article-title: Effects of different texture cues on curved surfaces viewed stereoscopically
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90201-7
– volume: 113
  start-page: 198
  year: 1984
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB7
  article-title: Three gradients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.198
– volume: 41
  start-page: 697
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB21
  article-title: The appearance of surfaces specified by motion parallax and binocular disparity
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/14640748908402390
– volume: 5
  start-page: 1749
  year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB5
  article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading
  publication-title: Journal of the Optical Society of America
  doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.5.001749
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1561
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB18
  article-title: On the perceptual identity of dynamic stereopsis and kinetic depth
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90149-Q
– volume: 26
  start-page: 973
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB10
  article-title: Tradeoffs between stereopsis and proximity luminance covariance as determinants of perceived 3D structure
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90154-9
– volume: 26
  start-page: 147
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB22
  article-title: The perception of scale-dependent and scale-independent surface structure from binocular disparity, texture and shading
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 60
  start-page: 377
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB19
  article-title: The perception of surface curvature from optical motion
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206861
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1723
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB2
  article-title: Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90037-W
– volume: 117
  start-page: 161
  year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB3
  article-title: Minimodularity and the perception of layout
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.161
– volume: 33
  start-page: 2685
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB25
  article-title: A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90228-O
– volume: 59
  start-page: 370
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB23
  article-title: The relationship between binocular disparity and motion parallax in surface detection
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211904
– volume: 94
  start-page: 115
  year: 1987
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB1
  article-title: Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115
– volume: 38
  start-page: 2635
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB14
  article-title: Ideal observer perturbation analysis reveals human strategies for inferring surface orientation from texture
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00415-X
– volume: 58
  start-page: 666
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB20
  article-title: The visual perception of rigid motion from constant flow fields
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03213099
– volume: 33
  start-page: 813
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB13
  article-title: Integration of depth modules: stereopsis and texture
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90200-G
– volume: 1199
  start-page: 1154
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB16
  publication-title: A statistical framework for robust fusion of depth information. Visual Communications and Image Processing IV: Proceedings of the SPIE
– year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB11
– volume: 34
  start-page: 2259
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0_BIB12
  article-title: Integration of motion and stereopsis cues
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90106-6
SSID ssj0007529
Score 1.8814203
Snippet Previous investigators have shown that observers’ visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the...
Previous investigators have shown that observers' visual cue combination strategies are remarkably flexible in the sense that these strategies adapt on the...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 4062
SubjectTerms Biological and medical sciences
Cue combination
Cues
Depth Perception - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Learning - physiology
Models, Psychological
Motion and texture cues
Motion Perception - physiology
Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology
Perception
Photic Stimulation - methods
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Vision
Title Experience-dependent integration of texture and motion cues to depth
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00120-0
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748939
https://www.proquest.com/docview/69458365
Volume 39
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnR1LS_QwcBAFEUR8fbo-c_j48DtU0276yHF9sSp6UthbyBMF7S7aPXjxtztJW6sHEbwUEjrpdGaSzCTzAPhrZN_lxtHIGi0jpiSLpDISH4VNUYIKpnw08vVNNrxjl6N0NAMnbSyMd6ts1v56TQ-rddNz1FDzaPLw4GN8fRgm2gs8RIB6u52x3Mv64Vvn5pGnCW_DUPzbXRRPPULoPOD8fxgkot_tT4sT-YJUc3W5i-_10bAvnS_DUqNQkkGN8wrM2HIV1gYlGtNPr-QfCS6e4ex8Feavm5v0NTjtchxHbSHcirTJI5BZZOyI9wqZPlsiS0Pqcj9EIzKkGhOEqe7X4e787PZkGDUVFSLNYl5FWlHLjC8wbvsmswlVMeUyxYnusIHcMnFiJZpc1lptC-kcZXFh-ypW2qEu1_8Ds-W4tJtAUFUwVGeJMSpmuYyV0mj5MZO4jGVFznrAWjoK3aQb91UvHkXnV4bkF578gnMRyC9oDw4_wCZ1vo2fAIqWSeKL4AjcE34C3f3C1O6DnPGc4i_st0wWOOn8TYos7Xj6ggP56-Ys7cFGzftPuIZ0Pnzr92htw0LIDRF8ZnZgtnqe2l3UfCq1F0R7D-YGF1fDG2xdjI7fAdDw_l0
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnR3LTuMwcIRYCZBWK950l4cPCMEh1Emdh488VR7lBFJvlp8CCdKKpof9-x07SbscEBKXSLEyzmTGHs9kXgCHRvZcbhyNrNEyYkqySCoj8VLYFFdQwZTPRh48ZP0ndjtMhwtw0ebC-LDKRvbXMj1I62ak21CzO3558Tm-Pg0T7QUeMkDRbv-B2kDmC-jfDM9n4jhPE97mofjH52k89RRh8JjzkzBLRD87oH6O5QTJ5up-F58rpOFgul6FX41GSc5qpNdgwZbrsHFWojX99pcckRDjGX6er8PSoHGlb8DlvMhx1HbCrUhbPQK5RUaO-LCQ6bslsjSk7vdDNCJDqhFBmOp5E56urx4v-lHTUiHSLOZVpBW1zPgO47ZnMptQFVMuU9zpDm-QXSZOrESby1qrbSGdoywubE_FSjtU5npbsFiOSrsDBHUFQ3WWGKNilstYKY2mHzOJy1hW5KwDrKWj0E29cd_24lXMA8uQ_MKTX3AuAvkF7cDpDGxcF9z4CqBomSQ-rByBh8JXoHsfmDp_IWc8p_gJBy2TBe4670qRpR1NJziR9zdnaQe2a97_h2uo58N_fx-tA1juPw7uxf3Nw90fWAmFIkIAzS4sVu9Tu4dqUKX2wzL_B8NX_vk
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Experience-dependent+integration+of+texture+and+motion+cues+to+depth&rft.jtitle=Vision+research+%28Oxford%29&rft.au=Jacobs%2C+Robert+A&rft.au=Fine%2C+I&rft.date=1999-12-01&rft.pub=Elsevier+Ltd&rft.issn=0042-6989&rft.eissn=1878-5646&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=24&rft.spage=4062&rft.epage=4075&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2FS0042-6989%2899%2900120-0&rft.externalDocID=S0042698999001200
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0042-6989&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0042-6989&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0042-6989&client=summon