The influence of stress on distractor-response bindings

Stimuli and responses that occur in close temporal contiguity are bound to each other and stored in short-term episodic traces or event files. A repetition of any of the features within an event file results in the retrieval of the entire event file and can influence responding. Along with task-rele...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inStress (Amsterdam, Netherlands) Vol. 24; no. 6; pp. 795 - 804
Main Authors Singh, Tarini, Domes, Gregor, Frings, Christian
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Taylor & Francis 02.11.2021
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1025-3890
1607-8888
1607-8888
DOI10.1080/10253890.2021.1920919

Cover

Abstract Stimuli and responses that occur in close temporal contiguity are bound to each other and stored in short-term episodic traces or event files. A repetition of any of the features within an event file results in the retrieval of the entire event file and can influence responding. Along with task-relevant features, event files also contain task-irrelevant features, which are also bound to responses - distractor-response binding. In the present study, the distractor-response binding effect was examined under stress. Stress was induced via a Cold Pressor Test (CPT) and was manipulated between subjects. Distractor-response binding effects were measures at pre- and post-intervention. The CPT produced reliable effects on cortisol measurements and subjective ratings, however, no difference in the distractor-response binding effects between the groups was observed. Results are discussed against the background of the inconsistent results in the literature with respect to stimulus-response binding and stress.
AbstractList Stimuli and responses that occur in close temporal contiguity are bound to each other and stored in short-term episodic traces or event files. A repetition of any of the features within an event file results in the retrieval of the entire event file and can influence responding. Along with task-relevant features, event files also contain task-irrelevant features, which are also bound to responses - distractor-response binding. In the present study, the distractor-response binding effect was examined under stress. Stress was induced via a Cold Pressor Test (CPT) and was manipulated between subjects. Distractor-response binding effects were measures at pre- and post-intervention. The CPT produced reliable effects on cortisol measurements and subjective ratings, however, no difference in the distractor-response binding effects between the groups was observed. Results are discussed against the background of the inconsistent results in the literature with respect to stimulus-response binding and stress.
Stimuli and responses that occur in close temporal contiguity are bound to each other and stored in short-term episodic traces or event files. A repetition of any of the features within an event file results in the retrieval of the entire event file and can influence responding. Along with task-relevant features, event files also contain task-irrelevant features, which are also bound to responses - distractor-response binding. In the present study, the distractor-response binding effect was examined under stress. Stress was induced via a Cold Pressor Test (CPT) and was manipulated between subjects. Distractor-response binding effects were measures at pre- and post-intervention. The CPT produced reliable effects on cortisol measurements and subjective ratings, however, no difference in the distractor-response binding effects between the groups was observed. Results are discussed against the background of the inconsistent results in the literature with respect to stimulus-response binding and stress.Stimuli and responses that occur in close temporal contiguity are bound to each other and stored in short-term episodic traces or event files. A repetition of any of the features within an event file results in the retrieval of the entire event file and can influence responding. Along with task-relevant features, event files also contain task-irrelevant features, which are also bound to responses - distractor-response binding. In the present study, the distractor-response binding effect was examined under stress. Stress was induced via a Cold Pressor Test (CPT) and was manipulated between subjects. Distractor-response binding effects were measures at pre- and post-intervention. The CPT produced reliable effects on cortisol measurements and subjective ratings, however, no difference in the distractor-response binding effects between the groups was observed. Results are discussed against the background of the inconsistent results in the literature with respect to stimulus-response binding and stress.
Author Singh, Tarini
Domes, Gregor
Frings, Christian
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Tarini
  surname: Singh
  fullname: Singh, Tarini
  organization: Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Gregor
  orcidid: 0000-0001-5908-4374
  surname: Domes
  fullname: Domes, Gregor
  organization: Institute of Psychobiology, University of Trier
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Christian
  surname: Frings
  fullname: Frings, Christian
  organization: Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34060422$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kEtPwzAMgCM0xB7wE0A9culw0vSRG2jiJU3iMs5R0jpQ1CYjaYX278m0jSO-2LI-W_Y3JxPrLBJyTWFJoYI7CizPKgFLBowuqWAgqDgjM1pAmVYxJrGOTLqHpmQewhcAFDnwCzLNOBTAGZuRcvOJSWtNN6KtMXEmCYPHEBJnk6aNtaoH59PY2jobMNGtbVr7ES7JuVFdwKtjXpD3p8fN6iVdvz2_rh7Wac1pPqRKKyE0B62b3AClGUXBSoO6UIozFS9i2BgUlLGijG8Vmtc8B6FEVRcZrbIFuT3s3Xr3PWIYZN-GGrtOWXRjkFFCXjFBKxHRmyM66h4bufVtr_xOnp6NQH4Aau9C8Gj-EApyL1WepMq9VHmUGufuD3PRk_O9-nG-a-Sgdp3zxitbt0Fm_6_4BSfje7I
Cites_doi 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011
10.1177/0956797614567341
10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572
10.3109/10253890.2015.1053452
10.3389/neuro.08.005.2008
10.1038/nrn2651
10.1037/xhp0000341
10.1162/jocn_a_00024
10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.001
10.1080/13506285.2015.1041437
10.1007/s00221-011-2814-9
10.3758/s13414-018-1498-7
10.1097/PSY.0000000000000002
10.1027/1618-3169/a000151
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.01.013
10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.004
10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.004
10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
10.1038/nn.4087
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.032
10.1007/s002210000406
10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.001
10.1038/nrn1406
10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.015
10.1006/cogp.1995.1017
10.1016/S0002-8703(36)90370-8
10.3758/s13414-017-1393-7
10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00353
10.1001/archpsyc.55.4.362
10.1038/nature02400
10.1038/nrn2648
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.006
10.1515/CCLM.1999.154
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.002
10.1017/CBO9781139087759
10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01438-1
10.1016/0960-0760(92)90294-S
10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.1067
10.1002/smi.2533
10.1038/s41574-019-0228-0
10.1038/nature03287
10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.231
10.1080/17470210600955645
10.1017/S0140525X01000103
10.3758/s13414-014-0648-9
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2021 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group 2021
Copyright_xml – notice: 2021 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group 2021
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
DOI 10.1080/10253890.2021.1920919
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1607-8888
EndPage 804
ExternalDocumentID 34060422
10_1080_10253890_2021_1920919
1920919
Genre Research Article
Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
00X
03L
0YH
123
29Q
36B
4.4
53G
AAFWJ
AALUX
ABDBF
ABEIZ
ABIVO
ABLKL
ABUPF
ACENM
ACGEJ
ACGFS
ACUHS
ADCVX
ADRBQ
ADXPE
AECIN
AEOZL
AFKVX
AFPKN
AGYJP
AIJEM
AJWEG
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALQZU
BABNJ
BALJE
BLEHA
BOHLJ
CCCUG
CS3
DKSSO
EAP
EBD
EBS
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPL
ESX
F5P
GROUPED_DOAJ
H13
HZ~
KRBQP
KSSTO
KWAYT
KYCEM
LJTGL
M4Z
O9-
OK1
P2P
SV3
TDBHL
TFDNU
TFL
TFW
TUS
TWFNG
V1S
~1N
AAYXX
CITATION
5VS
AAFWK
AAGKR
AALIY
AAOTW
AAPXX
ABKIK
ADXRV
AEQML
AWYRJ
AXVGA
BEYMU
BSQBA
CAG
CGR
COF
CUY
CVF
CXMEQ
ECM
EIF
EJD
HMXZC
M44
NPM
NUSFT
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c415t-aba99b40bbd5f01131e927feb6aa42a0002edfe9122670806b4c4509a98c63183
ISSN 1025-3890
1607-8888
IngestDate Fri Jul 11 05:48:20 EDT 2025
Thu Jan 02 22:55:25 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:47:07 EDT 2025
Wed Dec 25 09:06:39 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 6
Keywords Cold Pressor Test
cortisol
stress
Stimulus-response binding
distractor-processing
action control
Language English
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c415t-aba99b40bbd5f01131e927feb6aa42a0002edfe9122670806b4c4509a98c63183
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0001-5908-4374
OpenAccessLink https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/10253890.2021.1920919?needAccess=true
PMID 34060422
PQID 2535829189
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 10
ParticipantIDs pubmed_primary_34060422
crossref_primary_10_1080_10253890_2021_1920919
informaworld_taylorfrancis_310_1080_10253890_2021_1920919
proquest_miscellaneous_2535829189
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2021-11-02
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2021-11-02
PublicationDate_xml – month: 11
  year: 2021
  text: 2021-11-02
  day: 02
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace England
PublicationPlace_xml – name: England
PublicationTitle Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)
PublicationTitleAlternate Stress
PublicationYear 2021
Publisher Taylor & Francis
Publisher_xml – name: Taylor & Francis
References CIT0030
CIT0010
CIT0032
CIT0031
CIT1002
CIT0012
CIT0034
CIT1001
CIT0033
CIT0014
CIT1004
CIT0036
CIT1003
CIT0013
CIT0035
CIT1006
CIT0038
CIT0015
CIT1005
CIT0037
CIT0018
CIT1008
CIT0017
CIT1007
CIT0019
CIT1009
CIT0041
CIT0040
CIT0021
CIT1011
CIT0020
CIT1010
CIT0023
CIT0022
Bear M. F. (CIT0005) 2016
CIT0003
CIT0025
CIT0002
CIT0024
CIT0027
CIT0026
CIT0007
CIT0029
CIT0006
CIT0028
CIT0009
CIT0008
References_xml – ident: CIT0038
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011
– ident: CIT0040
  doi: 10.1177/0956797614567341
– ident: CIT0020
  doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572
– ident: CIT0021
  doi: 10.3109/10253890.2015.1053452
– ident: CIT0024
  doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.005.2008
– ident: CIT0033
  doi: 10.1038/nrn2651
– ident: CIT1007
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000341
– ident: CIT1009
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00024
– ident: CIT0023
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
– ident: CIT0037
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.001
– ident: CIT0027
  doi: 10.1080/13506285.2015.1041437
– ident: CIT0025
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2814-9
– ident: CIT1011
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1498-7
– ident: CIT1006
  doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000002
– ident: CIT1008
  doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000151
– ident: CIT1003
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.01.013
– ident: CIT0012
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.004
– ident: CIT0015
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.004
– volume-title: Neuroscience: Exploring the brain
  year: 2016
  ident: CIT0005
– ident: CIT0032
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
– ident: CIT0003
  doi: 10.1038/nn.4087
– ident: CIT0008
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.032
– ident: CIT0029
  doi: 10.1007/s002210000406
– ident: CIT1010
  doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.001
– ident: CIT0041
  doi: 10.1038/nrn1406
– ident: CIT0014
  doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.015
– ident: CIT0019
  doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1017
– ident: CIT1004
  doi: 10.1016/S0002-8703(36)90370-8
– ident: CIT0028
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1393-7
– ident: CIT0035
  doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00353
– ident: CIT1001
  doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.4.362
– ident: CIT0010
  doi: 10.1038/nature02400
– ident: CIT0002
  doi: 10.1038/nrn2648
– ident: CIT0007
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.006
– ident: CIT1005
  doi: 10.1515/CCLM.1999.154
– ident: CIT0036
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.002
– ident: CIT0022
  doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139087759
– ident: CIT0009
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01438-1
– ident: CIT1002
  doi: 10.1016/0960-0760(92)90294-S
– ident: CIT0017
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.1067
– ident: CIT0030
  doi: 10.1002/smi.2533
– ident: CIT0034
  doi: 10.1038/s41574-019-0228-0
– ident: CIT0031
  doi: 10.1038/nature03287
– ident: CIT0006
  doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.231
– ident: CIT0013
  doi: 10.1080/17470210600955645
– ident: CIT0018
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103
– ident: CIT0026
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0648-9
SSID ssj0006504
Score 2.2965584
Snippet Stimuli and responses that occur in close temporal contiguity are bound to each other and stored in short-term episodic traces or event files. A repetition of...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
informaworld
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 795
SubjectTerms action control
Attention - physiology
Cold Pressor Test
cortisol
distractor-processing
Humans
Reaction Time - physiology
Stimulus-response binding
stress
Stress, Psychological
Title The influence of stress on distractor-response bindings
URI https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/10253890.2021.1920919
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34060422
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2535829189
Volume 24
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LT9wwELbKIiEuqEB59SFXQr1UWZKs17GPSylCleDCInGLbMeWOGyygnCAX8_4lc2qiwq9RKvETlYzX8ZfbH8zCB2T1FQS3qFEpYolZCxIwiojknQEZF6LnApj9c6XV_Tihvy5Hd8uCl06dUkrh-p5pa7kf7wK58CvViX7Ds92N4UT8Bv8C0fwMBzf7OO7WGXE0r6g_ACPVi4hrp2RT-79Nlj9U945CctDn5Be-x7AMyczmzOhCgBZ6IB7cwXX0NtNw0yFXfjpSHATVA1e8NJBwk4Z9jIYRCCGOYY8c2I7HyS1j4s0hcGM-Qp8MXB68XMASD8KFr5u5l_R2W9nBEoDUZanQ_ukITBMoCy83x6MPJ85lwFgqE1Sthisui2E8dIaWs-Lwq7Qr09Oz07Pu2EYqCeJki2Wnqx86ibaiPdZ4iVLWWtf__ZwHGT6EW2Fjwc88UjYRh90vYN2J7Vom9kT_oHddl63TrKDNi7DroldVABOcIcT3BjscYKbGq_ACY44-YRuzn9Pf10koWBGooCHtYmQgnNJUimrsYHAPco0zwujJRWC5MKOfroymmfAuQswCpVEEWCMgjNFbXDfQ4O6qfUBwqnIKkIZ4bYaTaEqQalQXBElmTTwAh-iYTRXOfd5UcospJuNpi6tqctg6kPE-0YtWzchZXz1mHL0j77fowdKiH52SUvUunl8KKHxmOU8Y9Bm37um-zvRs0evXvmMNhdo_4IG7f2j_gocs5XfAppeAMnXdHo
linkProvider EBSCOhost
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+influence+of+stress+on+distractor-response+bindings&rft.jtitle=Stress+%28Amsterdam%2C+Netherlands%29&rft.au=Singh%2C+Tarini&rft.au=Domes%2C+Gregor&rft.au=Frings%2C+Christian&rft.date=2021-11-02&rft.eissn=1607-8888&rft.volume=24&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=795&rft_id=info:doi/10.1080%2F10253890.2021.1920919&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F34060422&rft.externalDocID=34060422
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1025-3890&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1025-3890&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1025-3890&client=summon