Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional

Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selectio...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inJournal of cognition Vol. 1; no. 1; p. 29
Main Author Theeuwes, Jan
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Ubiquity Press 14.05.2018
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort.
AbstractList Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort.
Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection ( Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012 ). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort.
Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort.Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort.
Author Theeuwes, Jan
AuthorAffiliation 1 Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), NL
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: 1 Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), NL
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Jan
  surname: Theeuwes
  fullname: Theeuwes, Jan
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31517202$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNpdkUtP3DAUhS1EBXRKfwLKCrHo0OtX4gEJCSEoSEhdUNhaN35Qj5wY4oSKf99kBhCwsnXv8XeOdb6SzTa1jpDvFA4l5-LnMplDyjfIDpNUzIVisPnuvk12c14CAKOSQcm3yDanklYM2A65vAt5wFjcuOhMH1J7VNxOg_hcXGDuC2xtcTr0qcE-mONJZlNT3MT0b7W6SzFMrzB-I188xux2X84Zub04_3N2Ob_-_evq7PR6bgTlfO59xTzYmmMpjZWMG1pRaYF5b6EuQSnrrSu9Yw5ErRDAIFX1ovbSCAsLPiNXa65NuNQPXWiwe9YJg14NUnevsRuzRqcrpjy1UlWWG8HFAoV1aoJag7Uo7cg6WbMehrpx1ri27zB-gH7ctOGvvk9Puiy5KMfvzMjBC6BLj4PLvW5CNi5GbF0asmZsAYqrik-59957vZm8VjEK9tcC06WcO-ffJBT0VLMea9Yr0x-fhCb0OLUwZgzxs_w_JYWplA
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1007_s00429_021_02314_8
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00375
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2328379
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02404_z
crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2023_1205618
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01609
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2021_107879
crossref_primary_10_3390_ijgi10110753
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_01970_y
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11097_024_10028_5
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_023_40405_8
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02329_5
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_32
crossref_primary_10_29029_busbed_1165560
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_021_01485_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2020_07_006
crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_636
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_02025_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2023_103627
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_29
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01701_y
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02545_9
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_576430
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02740_2
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01708_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsycho_2025_108997
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_26
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_25
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_24
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_23
crossref_primary_10_15198_seeci_2024_57_e893
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01672_0
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_688157
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_vision_091718_015048
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0226284
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2021_1883031
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_018_01649_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2022_06_001
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2328902
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_019_01672_z
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01677
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_02243
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_8_16
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02818_x
crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1600
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_1_7
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_021_00281_6
crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_772
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2021_105787
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02156_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_12_024
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14002
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_541369
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02940_4
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2873_18_2019
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_023_04318_5
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01858_6
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01958_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_09_004
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_018_1493_z
crossref_primary_10_1177_23727322231218190
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_020_01722_x
crossref_primary_10_12677_ap_2024_1411818
crossref_primary_10_1111_mila_12467
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01997_8
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02066
crossref_primary_10_12677_ap_2024_144243
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_610347
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02206_9
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02667_8
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_024_02605_1
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_011624_025340
crossref_primary_10_1177_1747021819897590
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02608_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_displa_2023_102629
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2020_01_004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2020_08_002
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhab341
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_018_1094_4
crossref_primary_10_1017_jdm_2024_3
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2019_1582571
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_54
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2020_104312
crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0258_22_2023
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02489_0
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13770
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_32069_7
crossref_primary_10_7210_jrsj_38_28
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01893_1
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16037
crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2023_1203435
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2019_02_004
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02341_x
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_43604_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2024_108366
crossref_primary_10_1556_2006_2020_00100
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_019_01692_9
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02207_8
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01936_9
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2347605
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_11_004
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2023_2221046
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_251
crossref_primary_10_1007_s42113_024_00197_6
Cites_doi 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462
10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
10.3758/APP.72.1.5
10.1007/s00426-008-0215-x
10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001
10.3758/BF03209251
10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.041
10.3758/BF03211845
10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.749
10.1152/jn.00330.2004
10.1177/1073858413514136
10.1080/02699931.2016.1169998
10.1073/pnas.1104047108
10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.005
10.1037/0096-1523.28.5.1039
10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012
10.3758/BF03205306
10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1
10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.604
10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.629
10.3758/APP.72.2.326
10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269
10.1080/17470210500416367
10.1177/0956797612459767
10.1126/science.1138071
10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8
10.3758/s13414-011-0176-9
10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1
10.3758/BF03207609
10.1038/nrn755
10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799
10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02360.x
10.1038/72999
10.3758/BF03211949
10.1016/0001-6918(90)90036-F
10.1080/135062800407202
10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160
10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.006
10.3758/BF03196609
10.1007/s00221-009-2094-9
10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.022
10.1068/p230429
10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1335
10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.271
10.3758/BF03193636
10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1595
10.1037/a0015875
10.1167/14.5.6
10.1016/0042-6989(89)90144-2
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01689.x
10.1037/a0031076
10.1111/1467-9280.00071
10.1038/35058500
10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.001
10.3758/BF03211656
10.3758/s13415-014-0278-7
10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494
10.3758/BF03193640
10.1016/0001-6918(89)90043-7
10.1152/jn.00441.2015
10.1109/34.730558
10.3758/BF03206924
10.3758/BF03212219
10.1037/xge0000037
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0569-04.2004
10.3758/BF03206462
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04675.2002
10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030
10.1177/0956797612460407
10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006
10.1152/jn.00068.2009
10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010
10.3758/BF03193557
10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00003-3
10.1111/nyas.12957
10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_17
10.1080/13506285.2014.956851
10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003
10.3758/s13415-015-0391-2
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright: © 2018 The Author(s) 2018
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright: © 2018 The Author(s) 2018
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
NPM
7X8
5PM
DOA
DOI 10.5334/joc.13
DatabaseName CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList

MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 2514-4820
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d
PMC6634613
31517202
10_5334_joc_13
Genre Journal Article
GroupedDBID .0O
AAFWJ
AAPRH
AAYXX
ACCQO
ADBBV
AFPKN
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
BCNDV
CITATION
GROUPED_DOAJ
H13
HYE
IAO
M~E
OK1
PGMZT
RPM
NPM
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c4133-ff72f0db3a65cd523c1715d02ffd0b6088dfde6fe2e04b8a00ca18b9bf5c4d093
IEDL.DBID DOA
ISSN 2514-4820
IngestDate Wed Aug 27 01:31:18 EDT 2025
Thu Aug 21 14:08:52 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 12:08:38 EDT 2025
Thu Jan 02 22:29:29 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 00:36:39 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:10:23 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 1
Keywords top-down and bottom-up control
attention
selection history
Language English
License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4133-ff72f0db3a65cd523c1715d02ffd0b6088dfde6fe2e04b8a00ca18b9bf5c4d093
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://doaj.org/article/728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d
PMID 31517202
PQID 2290838739
PQPubID 23479
ParticipantIDs doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6634613
proquest_miscellaneous_2290838739
pubmed_primary_31517202
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_13
crossref_citationtrail_10_5334_joc_13
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 20180514
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2018-05-14
PublicationDate_xml – month: 5
  year: 2018
  text: 20180514
  day: 14
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace England
PublicationPlace_xml – name: England
PublicationTitle Journal of cognition
PublicationTitleAlternate J Cogn
PublicationYear 2018
Publisher Ubiquity Press
Publisher_xml – name: Ubiquity Press
References (key20180514124426_B21) 1996; 93
(key20180514124426_B102) 2004; 11
(key20180514124426_B60) 2010; 36
(key20180514124426_B101) 1999; 25
(key20180514124426_B1) 2015; 23
(key20180514124426_B41) 2016
(key20180514124426_B55) 2010; 135
(key20180514124426_B63) 2000; 7
(key20180514124426_B56) 2010; 72
(key20180514124426_B69) 1989; 15
(key20180514124426_B71) 1989; 29
(key20180514124426_B13) 2007; 315
(key20180514124426_B70) 2003; 29
(key20180514124426_B109) 1988; 40A
(key20180514124426_B100) 1998; 9
(key20180514124426_B35) 2002; 28
(key20180514124426_B4) 2011; 108
(key20180514124426_B90) 1989; 72
(key20180514124426_B79) 2005; 67
(key20180514124426_B36) 2010; 30
(key20180514124426_B93) 1992; 51
(key20180514124426_B15) 2013; 85
(key20180514124426_B27) 2017
(key20180514124426_B62) 1994; 22
(key20180514124426_B3) 2011; 6
(key20180514124426_B34) 2008; 48
(key20180514124426_B39) 2000; 62
(key20180514124426_B45) 1998; 20
(key20180514124426_B51) 2014; 20
(key20180514124426_B103) 2006; 14
(key20180514124426_B22) 1997; 48
(key20180514124426_B58) 2013; 13
(key20180514124426_B88) 2009; 73
(key20180514124426_B80) 1978
(key20180514124426_B16) 1998; 36
(key20180514124426_B61) 2002; 28
(key20180514124426_B107) 2005; 93
(key20180514124426_B9) 2010; 72
(key20180514124426_B33) 2005; 67
(key20180514124426_B19) 2006; 17
(key20180514124426_B91) 1990; 74
(key20180514124426_B20) 2009; 20
(key20180514124426_B38) 2010; 201
(key20180514124426_B43) 2007; 33
(key20180514124426_B92) 1991; 50
(key20180514124426_B75) 2010; 50
(key20180514124426_B48) 2017
(key20180514124426_B5) 2012; 16
(key20180514124426_B68) 1995; 57
(key20180514124426_B110) 2004; 30
(key20180514124426_B40) 2013; 24
(key20180514124426_B74) 2004; 24
(key20180514124426_B49) 2013; 39
(key20180514124426_B67) 2009; 102
(key20180514124426_B76) 2003; 47
(key20180514124426_B32) 2018; 22
(key20180514124426_B114) 2003; 29
(key20180514124426_B53) 2017; 79
(key20180514124426_B72) 1984
(key20180514124426_B44) 2001; 2
(key20180514124426_B37) 2006; 18
(key20180514124426_B2) 2016; 1369
(key20180514124426_B66) 2016; 16
(key20180514124426_B10) 1998; 28
(key20180514124426_B42) 2000; 3
(key20180514124426_B95) 1994; 20
(key20180514124426_B77) 2001; 52
(key20180514124426_B89) 1977; 84
(key20180514124426_B7) 2011; 34
(key20180514124426_B18) 2002; 3
(key20180514124426_B94) 1994; 23
(key20180514124426_B104) 2007; 33
(key20180514124426_B108) 2017
(key20180514124426_B65) 2013; 13
(key20180514124426_B23) 2014; 14
(key20180514124426_B83) 2016; 16
(key20180514124426_B25) 2015; 114
(key20180514124426_B87) 1977; 84
(key20180514124426_B81) 1980; 32
(key20180514124426_B96) 2004; 11
key20180514124426_B112
key20180514124426_B113
(key20180514124426_B84) 2006; 59
(key20180514124426_B11) 2002; 22
(key20180514124426_B50) 1981
(key20180514124426_B24) 2015; 23
(key20180514124426_B17) 2004; 14
(key20180514124426_B57) 1999; 61
(key20180514124426_B64) 2005; 31
(key20180514124426_B6) 1994; 55
(key20180514124426_B47) 1890
(key20180514124426_B97) 2010; 135
(key20180514124426_B14) 2011; 51
(key20180514124426_B46) 2017
(key20180514124426_B116) 2013; 24
(key20180514124426_B8) 1999; 54
(key20180514124426_B78) 2015; 23
(key20180514124426_B111) 2018; 44
(key20180514124426_B105) 2011; 73
(key20180514124426_B73) 2016
(key20180514124426_B59) 2015; 144
(key20180514124426_B52) 1999; 61
(key20180514124426_B99) 2005; 67
(key20180514124426_B106) 2013; 13
(key20180514124426_B115) 1990; 16
(key20180514124426_B31) 2017; 79
(key20180514124426_B54) 1985; 4
(key20180514124426_B12) 2014; 14
(key20180514124426_B98) 2013; 368
key20180514124426_B28
(key20180514124426_B30) 1992; 18
(key20180514124426_B82) 1980; 109
(key20180514124426_B26) 2016; 148
key20180514124426_B29
(key20180514124426_B85) 2015; 15
(key20180514124426_B86) 2017; 31
References_xml – volume: 54
  start-page: 462
  issue: 7
  year: 1999
  ident: key20180514124426_B8
  article-title: The unbearable automaticity of being
  publication-title: American Psychologist
  doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462
– volume: 36
  start-page: 28
  issue: 1
  year: 1998
  ident: key20180514124426_B16
  article-title: Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
– volume: 72
  start-page: 5
  issue: 1
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B56
  article-title: Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/APP.72.1.5
– volume: 73
  start-page: 233
  issue: 2
  year: 2009
  ident: key20180514124426_B88
  article-title: Salience detection and attentional capture
  publication-title: Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0215-x
– volume: 22
  start-page: 79
  issue: 1
  year: 2018
  ident: key20180514124426_B32
  article-title: The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001
– volume: 22
  start-page: 657
  issue: 6
  year: 1994
  ident: key20180514124426_B62
  article-title: Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03209251
– volume: 14
  start-page: R850
  issue: 19
  year: 2004
  ident: key20180514124426_B17
  article-title: Visual attention: Bottom-up versus top-down
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.041
– volume: 57
  start-page: 1
  year: 1995
  ident: key20180514124426_B68
  article-title: Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211845
– volume: 30
  start-page: 746
  issue: 4
  year: 2004
  ident: key20180514124426_B110
  article-title: The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in saccadic visual selection
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.749
– volume: 93
  start-page: 337
  year: 2005
  ident: key20180514124426_B107
  article-title: Frontal eye field activity before visual search errors reveals the integration of bottom-up and top-down salience
  publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology
  doi: 10.1152/jn.00330.2004
– volume: 44
  start-page: 13
  issue: 1
  year: 2018
  ident: key20180514124426_B111
  article-title: Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 20
  start-page: 509
  issue: 5
  year: 2014
  ident: key20180514124426_B51
  article-title: Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems
  publication-title: Neuroscientist
  doi: 10.1177/1073858413514136
– volume: 31
  start-page: 845
  issue: 5
  year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B86
  article-title: The time course of attentional bias to cues of threat and safety
  publication-title: Cognition and Emotion
  doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1169998
– volume: 108
  start-page: 10367
  issue: 25
  year: 2011
  ident: key20180514124426_B4
  article-title: Value-driven attentional capture
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104047108
– volume: 148
  start-page: 19
  year: 2016
  ident: key20180514124426_B26
  article-title: Reward alters the perception of time
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.005
– year: 1978
  ident: key20180514124426_B80
– volume: 28
  start-page: 1039
  issue: 5
  year: 2002
  ident: key20180514124426_B35
  article-title: Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.5.1039
– volume: 51
  start-page: 1484
  year: 2011
  ident: key20180514124426_B14
  article-title: Visual Attention: The past 25 years
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012
– volume: 4
  start-page: 219
  year: 1985
  ident: key20180514124426_B54
  article-title: Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry
  publication-title: Human Neurobiology
– ident: key20180514124426_B113
  article-title: Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture independent of search strategy
– volume: 55
  start-page: 485
  year: 1994
  ident: key20180514124426_B6
  article-title: Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03205306
– volume: 23
  start-page: 41
  issue: 1–2
  year: 2015
  ident: key20180514124426_B78
  article-title: Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli
  publication-title: Visual Cognition
– volume: 79
  start-page: 45
  issue: 1
  year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B31
  article-title: Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1
– volume: 28
  start-page: 902
  year: 2002
  ident: key20180514124426_B61
  article-title: Stimulus-driven and goal-driven control over visual selection
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 18
  start-page: 604
  year: 2006
  ident: key20180514124426_B37
  article-title: Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.604
– volume: 16
  start-page: 121
  year: 1990
  ident: key20180514124426_B115
  article-title: Abrupt Visual Onsets and Selective Attention: Voluntary Versus Automatic Allocation
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– ident: key20180514124426_B28
  article-title: Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
– volume: 52
  start-page: 629
  year: 2001
  ident: key20180514124426_B77
  article-title: Attention and Performance
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.629
– volume: 32
  start-page: 3
  year: 1980
  ident: key20180514124426_B81
  article-title: Orienting of Attention, The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett Lecture
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 15
  start-page: 329
  year: 2015
  ident: key20180514124426_B85
  article-title: Attentional capture by signals of threat
  publication-title: Cognition & Emotion
– volume: 72
  start-page: 326
  issue: 2
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B9
  article-title: What is top-down about contingent capture?
  publication-title: Attention Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/APP.72.2.326
– volume: 84
  start-page: 127
  issue: 3
  year: 1977
  ident: key20180514124426_B89
  article-title: Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 48
  start-page: 269
  year: 1997
  ident: key20180514124426_B22
  article-title: Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269
– volume: 59
  start-page: 1921
  year: 2006
  ident: key20180514124426_B84
  article-title: Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470210500416367
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1021
  year: 2003
  ident: key20180514124426_B70
  article-title: Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighing
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 24
  start-page: 790
  issue: 5
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B40
  article-title: Visual Working Memory Modulates Rapid Eye Movements to Simple Onset Targets
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797612459767
– volume: 315
  start-page: 1860
  year: 2007
  ident: key20180514124426_B13
  article-title: Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1138071
– volume: 28
  start-page: 309
  issue: 3
  year: 1998
  ident: key20180514124426_B10
  article-title: What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?
  publication-title: Brain Research Reviews
  doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8
– year: 1890
  ident: key20180514124426_B47
– volume: 73
  start-page: 2092
  year: 2011
  ident: key20180514124426_B105
  article-title: On the limits of top-down control
  publication-title: Attention, Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0176-9
– volume: 29
  start-page: 483
  issue: 2
  year: 2003
  ident: key20180514124426_B114
  article-title: Changing Your Mind: On the Contributions of Top-Down and Bottum-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Feature Singeltons
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1
  year: 1977
  ident: key20180514124426_B87
  article-title: Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search, and Attention
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1
– volume: 61
  start-page: 1009
  year: 1999
  ident: key20180514124426_B52
  article-title: Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03207609
– volume: 3
  start-page: 201
  issue: 3
  year: 2002
  ident: key20180514124426_B18
  article-title: Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nrn755
– volume: 79
  start-page: 1
  year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B53
  article-title: Psychonomics in Boston: A debate about types of orienting
  publication-title: Attention, Perception & Psychophysics
– year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B108
  article-title: Implicit guidance of attention: The Priority State Space framework
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 20
  start-page: 799
  issue: 4
  year: 1994
  ident: key20180514124426_B95
  article-title: Stimulus-Driven Capture and Attentional Set – Selective Search for Color and Visual Abrupt Onsets
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799
– volume: 85
  start-page: 58
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B15
  article-title: Rewards teach visual selective attention
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005
– volume: 31
  start-page: 542
  year: 2005
  ident: key20180514124426_B64
  article-title: Response selection modulates visual search within and across dimensions
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 20
  start-page: 778
  issue: 6
  year: 2009
  ident: key20180514124426_B20
  article-title: Learning to attend and to ignore is a matter of gains and losses
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02360.x
– volume: 3
  start-page: 284
  year: 2000
  ident: key20180514124426_B42
  article-title: The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/72999
– volume: 61
  start-page: 61
  year: 1999
  ident: key20180514124426_B57
  article-title: Limitations in attending to a feature value for overriding stimulus-driven interference
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211949
– volume: 74
  start-page: 81
  issue: 1
  year: 1990
  ident: key20180514124426_B91
  article-title: Perceptual Selectivity Is Task Dependent – Evidence from Selective Search
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90036-F
– year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B27
  article-title: Don’t let it distract you: How information about the availability of reward affects attentional selection
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
– volume: 7
  start-page: 571
  year: 2000
  ident: key20180514124426_B63
  article-title: Priming of pop-out: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection
  publication-title: Visual Cognition
  doi: 10.1080/135062800407202
– volume: 109
  start-page: 160
  year: 1980
  ident: key20180514124426_B82
  article-title: Attention and the detection of signals
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1315
  issue: 11
  year: 2008
  ident: key20180514124426_B34
  article-title: Expectancies modulate attentional capture by salient color singletons
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.006
– volume: 11
  start-page: 551
  issue: 3
  year: 2004
  ident: key20180514124426_B102
  article-title: Attentional capture modulates perceptual sensitivity
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196609
– volume: 201
  start-page: 789
  issue: 4
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B38
  article-title: The time course of exogenous and endogenous control of covert attention
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2094-9
– volume: 50
  start-page: 1362
  issue: 14
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B75
  article-title: Priming resolves perceptual ambiguity in visual search: evidence from behaviour and electrophysiology
  publication-title: Vision Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.022
– volume: 23
  start-page: 429
  issue: 4
  year: 1994
  ident: key20180514124426_B94
  article-title: Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visual Selection
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p230429
– volume: 33
  start-page: 1335
  issue: 6
  year: 2007
  ident: key20180514124426_B104
  article-title: The role of spatial and nonspatial information in visual selection
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1335
– volume: 33
  start-page: 271
  issue: 2
  year: 2007
  ident: key20180514124426_B43
  article-title: The time course of attentional and oculomotor capture reveals a common cause
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.271
– volume: 67
  start-page: 1305
  issue: 8
  year: 2005
  ident: key20180514124426_B99
  article-title: Attentional capture and inhibition (of return): The effect on perceptual sensitivity
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193636
– volume: 25
  start-page: 1595
  issue: 6
  year: 1999
  ident: key20180514124426_B101
  article-title: Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1595
– volume: 36
  start-page: 1
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B60
  article-title: Attentional capture with rapidly changing attentional control settings
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0015875
– volume: 16
  issue: 8
  year: 2016
  ident: key20180514124426_B83
  article-title: Was That a Threat? Attentional biases by signals of threat
  publication-title: Emotion
– volume: 14
  start-page: 6
  issue: 5
  year: 2014
  ident: key20180514124426_B23
  article-title: Exogenous visual orienting by reward
  publication-title: Journal of Vision
  doi: 10.1167/14.5.6
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1631
  year: 1989
  ident: key20180514124426_B71
  article-title: Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention
  publication-title: Vision Res
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90144-2
– volume: 13
  start-page: 1
  issue: 3
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B106
  article-title: Priming makes a stimulus more salient
  publication-title: Journal of Vision
– year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B48
  article-title: Habitual versus goal-driven attention
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 30
  start-page: 11096
  issue: 33
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B36
  article-title: Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010
– volume: 17
  start-page: 222
  issue: 3
  year: 2006
  ident: key20180514124426_B19
  article-title: Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01689.x
– volume: 13
  start-page: 529
  issue: 3
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B65
  article-title: Conditioned fear modulates visual selection
  publication-title: Emotion
  doi: 10.1037/a0031076
– volume: 9
  start-page: 379
  issue: 5
  year: 1998
  ident: key20180514124426_B100
  article-title: Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00071
– volume: 2
  start-page: 194
  issue: 3
  year: 2001
  ident: key20180514124426_B44
  article-title: Computational modelling of visual attention
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/35058500
– volume: 135
  start-page: 114
  issue: 2
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B55
  article-title: Priming in visual search: A spanner in the works for Theeuwes’s bottom-up attention sweeps?
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.001
– start-page: 1
  year: 2017
  ident: key20180514124426_B46
  article-title: Sensitivity to value-driven attention is predicted by how we learn from value
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
– volume: 39
  start-page: 87
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B49
  article-title: Rapid acquisition but slow extinction of an attentional bias in space
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
– volume: 51
  start-page: 599
  issue: 6
  year: 1992
  ident: key20180514124426_B93
  article-title: Perceptual Selectivity for Color and Form
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211656
– volume: 14
  start-page: 635
  issue: 2
  year: 2014
  ident: key20180514124426_B12
  article-title: The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing
  publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0278-7
– volume: 93
  start-page: 13494
  year: 1996
  ident: key20180514124426_B21
  article-title: Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494
– volume: 67
  start-page: 1354
  issue: 8
  year: 2005
  ident: key20180514124426_B79
  article-title: Target uncertainty does not lead to more distraction by singletons: intertrial priming does
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193640
– volume: 14
  start-page: 466
  issue: 4–8
  year: 2006
  ident: key20180514124426_B103
  article-title: Visual search for featural singletons: No top-down modulation, only bottom-up priming
  publication-title: Visual Cognition
– volume: 40A
  start-page: 201
  year: 1988
  ident: key20180514124426_B109
  article-title: “Features and objects: the fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture.”
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 72
  start-page: 177
  issue: 2
  year: 1989
  ident: key20180514124426_B90
  article-title: Effects of Location and Form Cueing on the Allocation of Attention in the Visual-Field
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(89)90043-7
– volume: 114
  start-page: 2316
  issue: 4
  year: 2015
  ident: key20180514124426_B25
  article-title: Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal the availability of reward
  publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology
  doi: 10.1152/jn.00441.2015
– volume: 20
  start-page: 1254
  year: 1998
  ident: key20180514124426_B45
  article-title: A model of saliency-based visual-attention for rapid scene analysis
  publication-title: IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence
  doi: 10.1109/34.730558
– volume: 368
  start-page: 1
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B98
  article-title: Feature-based attention: It is all bottom-up priming
  publication-title: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
– ident: key20180514124426_B29
– volume: 62
  start-page: 800
  year: 2000
  ident: key20180514124426_B39
  article-title: Repetition effects in visual search
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206924
– volume: 50
  start-page: 184
  issue: 2
  year: 1991
  ident: key20180514124426_B92
  article-title: Cross-Dimensional Perceptual Selectivity
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03212219
– volume: 144
  start-page: 158
  issue: 1
  year: 2015
  ident: key20180514124426_B59
  article-title: When goals conflict with values: Counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000037
– year: 2016
  ident: key20180514124426_B41
  article-title: Eye Movements are Captured by a Perceptually Simple Conditioned Stimulus in the Absence of Explicit Contingency Knowledge
  publication-title: Emotion
– volume: 24
  start-page: 6371
  issue: 28
  year: 2004
  ident: key20180514124426_B74
  article-title: Target selection in area V4 during a multidimensional visual search task
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0569-04.2004
– volume: 11
  start-page: 65
  issue: 1
  year: 2004
  ident: key20180514124426_B96
  article-title: Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206462
– volume: 22
  start-page: 4675
  year: 2002
  ident: key20180514124426_B11
  article-title: Priming in macaque frontal cortex during popout visual search: Feature-based facilitation and location-based inhibition of return
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04675.2002
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1030
  issue: 4
  year: 1992
  ident: key20180514124426_B30
  article-title: Involuntary Covert Orienting Is Contingent on Attentional Control Settings
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030
– volume: 24
  start-page: 667
  issue: 5
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B116
  article-title: Attention is spontaneously biased toward regularities
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797612460407
– volume: 135
  start-page: 77
  issue: 2
  year: 2010
  ident: key20180514124426_B97
  article-title: Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006
– volume: 102
  start-page: 1451
  issue: 3
  year: 2009
  ident: key20180514124426_B67
  article-title: Early and Late Modulation of Saccade Deviations by Target Distractor Similarity
  publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology
  doi: 10.1152/jn.00068.2009
– volume: 15
  start-page: 315
  year: 1989
  ident: key20180514124426_B69
  article-title: Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 16
  start-page: 437
  issue: 8
  year: 2012
  ident: key20180514124426_B5
  article-title: Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010
– volume: 13
  year: 2013
  ident: key20180514124426_B58
  article-title: Is goal-directed attentional guidance just intertrial priming? A review
  publication-title: Journal of Vision
– volume: 23
  start-page: 82
  issue: 1–2
  year: 2015
  ident: key20180514124426_B24
  article-title: Nonspatial Attentional Capture by Previously Rewarded Scene Semantics
  publication-title: Visual Cognition
– start-page: 1
  year: 2016
  ident: key20180514124426_B73
  article-title: People look at the object they fear: Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal threat
  publication-title: Cognition and Emotion
– volume: 6
  issue: 11
  year: 2011
  ident: key20180514124426_B3
  article-title: Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture
  publication-title: PLoS One
– volume: 67
  start-page: 1252
  year: 2005
  ident: key20180514124426_B33
  article-title: Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search
  publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193557
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1
  year: 2003
  ident: key20180514124426_B76
  article-title: Visual marking inhibits singleton capture
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00003-3
– volume: 1369
  start-page: 24
  issue: 1
  year: 2016
  ident: key20180514124426_B2
  article-title: The attention habit: How reward learning shapes attentional selection
  publication-title: Annals of New York Academy Sciences
  doi: 10.1111/nyas.12957
– start-page: 255
  volume-title: Cognition and Motor Processes
  year: 1984
  ident: key20180514124426_B72
  doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_17
– ident: key20180514124426_B112
  article-title: How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression
– start-page: 187
  volume-title: Attention and performance
  year: 1981
  ident: key20180514124426_B50
– volume: 23
  start-page: 67
  year: 2015
  ident: key20180514124426_B1
  article-title: Value-driven attentional capture is modulated by spatial context
  publication-title: Visual Cognition
  doi: 10.1080/13506285.2014.956851
– volume: 34
  start-page: 210
  year: 2011
  ident: key20180514124426_B7
  article-title: Mechanisms of top-down attention
  publication-title: Trends in Neurosciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003
– volume: 16
  start-page: 315
  issue: 2
  year: 2016
  ident: key20180514124426_B66
  article-title: Distracted by danger: Temporal and spatial dynamics of visual selection in the presence of threat
  publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0391-2
SSID ssj0002152063
Score 2.4057112
Snippet Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh,...
Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection ( Awh,...
SourceID doaj
pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage 29
SubjectTerms attention
selection history
top-down and bottom-up control
Title Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31517202
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2290838739
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6634613
https://doaj.org/article/728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d
Volume 1
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3BbtQwELWgp14QBQqhUIJEj2nt2LGdciqI1QoJOLRb9RbZHlu0WpKquyvUv-9Msl3tFiQuXO2JYr1xZp6V8RvGPkTM0l54KHQiUW2nYuGB-0IY4KBKbyDQfedv3_V4or5eVBdrrb6oJmyQBx6AOzKlTQIqa0AGJVXtFESrvHUQnFcaKPpizls7TFEMpm6tmHyHZkJ02_ToqguHQm5kn16k_2_M8mGB5FrGGT1lT5ZUMT8ZlrjDHsX2GdteRazb52x8fjlboMlp38wGET7OJzQwvc1HbjbPXQv5yWLe9bKsH8kMul_56bT73U-d97VvRMVfsMnoy9nncbFsjVAEzDqySMmUiYOXTlcB8DAZhBEV8DIlRFpj6IAEUadYRk5gcR6csL72qQoKeC132VbbtfEVy1P0yhiRkq6ikrpy-KQMOghrTKp9lbGDe8iasNQNp_YV0wbPDwRtg9A2Qmbs3cruelDK-MPiEyG-miVl634A_d0s_d38y98Ze3_vrwa_BPq94drYLWYNKddbaY2sM_Zy8N_qVRKJjSl5mTGz4dmNtWzOtJc_e7VtpGQKOc_r_7H4PbaNhMtS9YFQb9jW_GYR3yKpmft99viQ_9jvd_Ed2vf7pQ
linkProvider Directory of Open Access Journals
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Visual+Selection%3A+Usually+Fast+and+Automatic%3B+Seldom+Slow+and+Volitional&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+cognition&rft.au=Theeuwes%2C+Jan&rft.date=2018-05-14&rft.eissn=2514-4820&rft.volume=1&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=29&rft_id=info:doi/10.5334%2Fjoc.13&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F31517202&rft.externalDocID=31517202
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon