Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional
Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selectio...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of cognition Vol. 1; no. 1; p. 29 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
Ubiquity Press
14.05.2018
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort. Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection ( Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012 ). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort. Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort.Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh, Belopolsky & Theeuwes, 2012). Since its publication there has been a growing controversy about the terms top-down, bottom-up and selection-history in relation to visual selection. In the current paper we define these terms, discuss some controversies about these terms and explain what kind of effects should be considered to be the result of lingering biases of selection history, i.e., priming, reward/fear, and statistical learning. We discuss the properties of top-down selection (slow, effortful, and controlled) versus the properties of lingering biases of selection history (fast, effortless, and automatic). We adhere the position that the experience with selecting a particular feature or the location of a feature, may boost and sharpen its representation within the priority selection map above and beyond its physical salience. It is as if the experience may render a feature or location subjectively more salient. Our message of the current review is that true top-down control of visual selection occurs far less often than what is typically assumed. Most of the time, selection is based on experience and history. It is fast, automatic and occurs without much, if any, effort. |
Author | Theeuwes, Jan |
AuthorAffiliation | 1 Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), NL |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 1 Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Institute Brain and Behavior Amsterdam (iBBA), NL |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Jan surname: Theeuwes fullname: Theeuwes, Jan |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31517202$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpdkUtP3DAUhS1EBXRKfwLKCrHo0OtX4gEJCSEoSEhdUNhaN35Qj5wY4oSKf99kBhCwsnXv8XeOdb6SzTa1jpDvFA4l5-LnMplDyjfIDpNUzIVisPnuvk12c14CAKOSQcm3yDanklYM2A65vAt5wFjcuOhMH1J7VNxOg_hcXGDuC2xtcTr0qcE-mONJZlNT3MT0b7W6SzFMrzB-I188xux2X84Zub04_3N2Ob_-_evq7PR6bgTlfO59xTzYmmMpjZWMG1pRaYF5b6EuQSnrrSu9Yw5ErRDAIFX1ovbSCAsLPiNXa65NuNQPXWiwe9YJg14NUnevsRuzRqcrpjy1UlWWG8HFAoV1aoJag7Uo7cg6WbMehrpx1ri27zB-gH7ctOGvvk9Puiy5KMfvzMjBC6BLj4PLvW5CNi5GbF0asmZsAYqrik-59957vZm8VjEK9tcC06WcO-ffJBT0VLMea9Yr0x-fhCb0OLUwZgzxs_w_JYWplA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s00429_021_02314_8 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00375 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2328379 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02404_z crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2023_1205618 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01609 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2021_107879 crossref_primary_10_3390_ijgi10110753 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_01970_y crossref_primary_10_1007_s11097_024_10028_5 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_023_40405_8 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02329_5 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_32 crossref_primary_10_29029_busbed_1165560 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_021_01485_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2020_07_006 crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_636 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_02025_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2023_103627 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_29 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01701_y crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02545_9 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_576430 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02740_2 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01708_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsycho_2025_108997 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_26 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_25 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_24 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_23 crossref_primary_10_15198_seeci_2024_57_e893 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01672_0 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_688157 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_vision_091718_015048 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0226284 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2021_1883031 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_018_01649_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2022_06_001 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2328902 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_019_01672_z crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01677 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_02243 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_8_16 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02818_x crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1600 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_1_7 crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_021_00281_6 crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_772 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2021_105787 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02156_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_12_024 crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14002 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_541369 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02940_4 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2873_18_2019 crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_023_04318_5 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01858_6 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01958_1 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_09_004 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_018_1493_z crossref_primary_10_1177_23727322231218190 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_020_01722_x crossref_primary_10_12677_ap_2024_1411818 crossref_primary_10_1111_mila_12467 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01997_8 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02066 crossref_primary_10_12677_ap_2024_144243 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_610347 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02206_9 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02667_8 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_024_02605_1 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_011624_025340 crossref_primary_10_1177_1747021819897590 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02608_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_displa_2023_102629 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2020_01_004 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2020_08_002 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhab341 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_018_1094_4 crossref_primary_10_1017_jdm_2024_3 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2019_1582571 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_54 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2020_104312 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0258_22_2023 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02489_0 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13770 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_32069_7 crossref_primary_10_7210_jrsj_38_28 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01893_1 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16037 crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2023_1203435 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2019_02_004 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02341_x crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_43604_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2024_108366 crossref_primary_10_1556_2006_2020_00100 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_019_01692_9 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02207_8 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01936_9 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2347605 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_11_004 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2023_2221046 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_251 crossref_primary_10_1007_s42113_024_00197_6 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 10.3758/APP.72.1.5 10.1007/s00426-008-0215-x 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001 10.3758/BF03209251 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.041 10.3758/BF03211845 10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.749 10.1152/jn.00330.2004 10.1177/1073858413514136 10.1080/02699931.2016.1169998 10.1073/pnas.1104047108 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.005 10.1037/0096-1523.28.5.1039 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012 10.3758/BF03205306 10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.604 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.629 10.3758/APP.72.2.326 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269 10.1080/17470210500416367 10.1177/0956797612459767 10.1126/science.1138071 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8 10.3758/s13414-011-0176-9 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1 10.3758/BF03207609 10.1038/nrn755 10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02360.x 10.1038/72999 10.3758/BF03211949 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90036-F 10.1080/135062800407202 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160 10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.006 10.3758/BF03196609 10.1007/s00221-009-2094-9 10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.022 10.1068/p230429 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1335 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.271 10.3758/BF03193636 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1595 10.1037/a0015875 10.1167/14.5.6 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90144-2 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01689.x 10.1037/a0031076 10.1111/1467-9280.00071 10.1038/35058500 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.001 10.3758/BF03211656 10.3758/s13415-014-0278-7 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494 10.3758/BF03193640 10.1016/0001-6918(89)90043-7 10.1152/jn.00441.2015 10.1109/34.730558 10.3758/BF03206924 10.3758/BF03212219 10.1037/xge0000037 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0569-04.2004 10.3758/BF03206462 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04675.2002 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 10.1177/0956797612460407 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006 10.1152/jn.00068.2009 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010 10.3758/BF03193557 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00003-3 10.1111/nyas.12957 10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_17 10.1080/13506285.2014.956851 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003 10.3758/s13415-015-0391-2 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright: © 2018 The Author(s) 2018 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright: © 2018 The Author(s) 2018 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.5334/joc.13 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic PubMed |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 2514-4820 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d PMC6634613 31517202 10_5334_joc_13 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | .0O AAFWJ AAPRH AAYXX ACCQO ADBBV AFPKN ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BCNDV CITATION GROUPED_DOAJ H13 HYE IAO M~E OK1 PGMZT RPM NPM 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c4133-ff72f0db3a65cd523c1715d02ffd0b6088dfde6fe2e04b8a00ca18b9bf5c4d093 |
IEDL.DBID | DOA |
ISSN | 2514-4820 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:31:18 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 14:08:52 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 12:08:38 EDT 2025 Thu Jan 02 22:29:29 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:36:39 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:10:23 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | top-down and bottom-up control attention selection history |
Language | English |
License | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4133-ff72f0db3a65cd523c1715d02ffd0b6088dfde6fe2e04b8a00ca18b9bf5c4d093 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://doaj.org/article/728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d |
PMID | 31517202 |
PQID | 2290838739 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6634613 proquest_miscellaneous_2290838739 pubmed_primary_31517202 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_13 crossref_citationtrail_10_5334_joc_13 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20180514 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2018-05-14 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 5 year: 2018 text: 20180514 day: 14 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Journal of cognition |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Cogn |
PublicationYear | 2018 |
Publisher | Ubiquity Press |
Publisher_xml | – name: Ubiquity Press |
References | (key20180514124426_B21) 1996; 93 (key20180514124426_B102) 2004; 11 (key20180514124426_B60) 2010; 36 (key20180514124426_B101) 1999; 25 (key20180514124426_B1) 2015; 23 (key20180514124426_B41) 2016 (key20180514124426_B55) 2010; 135 (key20180514124426_B63) 2000; 7 (key20180514124426_B56) 2010; 72 (key20180514124426_B69) 1989; 15 (key20180514124426_B71) 1989; 29 (key20180514124426_B13) 2007; 315 (key20180514124426_B70) 2003; 29 (key20180514124426_B109) 1988; 40A (key20180514124426_B100) 1998; 9 (key20180514124426_B35) 2002; 28 (key20180514124426_B4) 2011; 108 (key20180514124426_B90) 1989; 72 (key20180514124426_B79) 2005; 67 (key20180514124426_B36) 2010; 30 (key20180514124426_B93) 1992; 51 (key20180514124426_B15) 2013; 85 (key20180514124426_B27) 2017 (key20180514124426_B62) 1994; 22 (key20180514124426_B3) 2011; 6 (key20180514124426_B34) 2008; 48 (key20180514124426_B39) 2000; 62 (key20180514124426_B45) 1998; 20 (key20180514124426_B51) 2014; 20 (key20180514124426_B103) 2006; 14 (key20180514124426_B22) 1997; 48 (key20180514124426_B58) 2013; 13 (key20180514124426_B88) 2009; 73 (key20180514124426_B80) 1978 (key20180514124426_B16) 1998; 36 (key20180514124426_B61) 2002; 28 (key20180514124426_B107) 2005; 93 (key20180514124426_B9) 2010; 72 (key20180514124426_B33) 2005; 67 (key20180514124426_B19) 2006; 17 (key20180514124426_B91) 1990; 74 (key20180514124426_B20) 2009; 20 (key20180514124426_B38) 2010; 201 (key20180514124426_B43) 2007; 33 (key20180514124426_B92) 1991; 50 (key20180514124426_B75) 2010; 50 (key20180514124426_B48) 2017 (key20180514124426_B5) 2012; 16 (key20180514124426_B68) 1995; 57 (key20180514124426_B110) 2004; 30 (key20180514124426_B40) 2013; 24 (key20180514124426_B74) 2004; 24 (key20180514124426_B49) 2013; 39 (key20180514124426_B67) 2009; 102 (key20180514124426_B76) 2003; 47 (key20180514124426_B32) 2018; 22 (key20180514124426_B114) 2003; 29 (key20180514124426_B53) 2017; 79 (key20180514124426_B72) 1984 (key20180514124426_B44) 2001; 2 (key20180514124426_B37) 2006; 18 (key20180514124426_B2) 2016; 1369 (key20180514124426_B66) 2016; 16 (key20180514124426_B10) 1998; 28 (key20180514124426_B42) 2000; 3 (key20180514124426_B95) 1994; 20 (key20180514124426_B77) 2001; 52 (key20180514124426_B89) 1977; 84 (key20180514124426_B7) 2011; 34 (key20180514124426_B18) 2002; 3 (key20180514124426_B94) 1994; 23 (key20180514124426_B104) 2007; 33 (key20180514124426_B108) 2017 (key20180514124426_B65) 2013; 13 (key20180514124426_B23) 2014; 14 (key20180514124426_B83) 2016; 16 (key20180514124426_B25) 2015; 114 (key20180514124426_B87) 1977; 84 (key20180514124426_B81) 1980; 32 (key20180514124426_B96) 2004; 11 key20180514124426_B112 key20180514124426_B113 (key20180514124426_B84) 2006; 59 (key20180514124426_B11) 2002; 22 (key20180514124426_B50) 1981 (key20180514124426_B24) 2015; 23 (key20180514124426_B17) 2004; 14 (key20180514124426_B57) 1999; 61 (key20180514124426_B64) 2005; 31 (key20180514124426_B6) 1994; 55 (key20180514124426_B47) 1890 (key20180514124426_B97) 2010; 135 (key20180514124426_B14) 2011; 51 (key20180514124426_B46) 2017 (key20180514124426_B116) 2013; 24 (key20180514124426_B8) 1999; 54 (key20180514124426_B78) 2015; 23 (key20180514124426_B111) 2018; 44 (key20180514124426_B105) 2011; 73 (key20180514124426_B73) 2016 (key20180514124426_B59) 2015; 144 (key20180514124426_B52) 1999; 61 (key20180514124426_B99) 2005; 67 (key20180514124426_B106) 2013; 13 (key20180514124426_B115) 1990; 16 (key20180514124426_B31) 2017; 79 (key20180514124426_B54) 1985; 4 (key20180514124426_B12) 2014; 14 (key20180514124426_B98) 2013; 368 key20180514124426_B28 (key20180514124426_B30) 1992; 18 (key20180514124426_B82) 1980; 109 (key20180514124426_B26) 2016; 148 key20180514124426_B29 (key20180514124426_B85) 2015; 15 (key20180514124426_B86) 2017; 31 |
References_xml | – volume: 54 start-page: 462 issue: 7 year: 1999 ident: key20180514124426_B8 article-title: The unbearable automaticity of being publication-title: American Psychologist doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462 – volume: 36 start-page: 28 issue: 1 year: 1998 ident: key20180514124426_B16 article-title: Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 – volume: 72 start-page: 5 issue: 1 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B56 article-title: Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/APP.72.1.5 – volume: 73 start-page: 233 issue: 2 year: 2009 ident: key20180514124426_B88 article-title: Salience detection and attentional capture publication-title: Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0215-x – volume: 22 start-page: 79 issue: 1 year: 2018 ident: key20180514124426_B32 article-title: The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001 – volume: 22 start-page: 657 issue: 6 year: 1994 ident: key20180514124426_B62 article-title: Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features publication-title: Memory & Cognition doi: 10.3758/BF03209251 – volume: 14 start-page: R850 issue: 19 year: 2004 ident: key20180514124426_B17 article-title: Visual attention: Bottom-up versus top-down publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.041 – volume: 57 start-page: 1 year: 1995 ident: key20180514124426_B68 article-title: Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211845 – volume: 30 start-page: 746 issue: 4 year: 2004 ident: key20180514124426_B110 article-title: The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in saccadic visual selection publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.749 – volume: 93 start-page: 337 year: 2005 ident: key20180514124426_B107 article-title: Frontal eye field activity before visual search errors reveals the integration of bottom-up and top-down salience publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.00330.2004 – volume: 44 start-page: 13 issue: 1 year: 2018 ident: key20180514124426_B111 article-title: Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 20 start-page: 509 issue: 5 year: 2014 ident: key20180514124426_B51 article-title: Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems publication-title: Neuroscientist doi: 10.1177/1073858413514136 – volume: 31 start-page: 845 issue: 5 year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B86 article-title: The time course of attentional bias to cues of threat and safety publication-title: Cognition and Emotion doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1169998 – volume: 108 start-page: 10367 issue: 25 year: 2011 ident: key20180514124426_B4 article-title: Value-driven attentional capture publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104047108 – volume: 148 start-page: 19 year: 2016 ident: key20180514124426_B26 article-title: Reward alters the perception of time publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.005 – year: 1978 ident: key20180514124426_B80 – volume: 28 start-page: 1039 issue: 5 year: 2002 ident: key20180514124426_B35 article-title: Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.5.1039 – volume: 51 start-page: 1484 year: 2011 ident: key20180514124426_B14 article-title: Visual Attention: The past 25 years publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012 – volume: 4 start-page: 219 year: 1985 ident: key20180514124426_B54 article-title: Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry publication-title: Human Neurobiology – ident: key20180514124426_B113 article-title: Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture independent of search strategy – volume: 55 start-page: 485 year: 1994 ident: key20180514124426_B6 article-title: Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03205306 – volume: 23 start-page: 41 issue: 1–2 year: 2015 ident: key20180514124426_B78 article-title: Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli publication-title: Visual Cognition – volume: 79 start-page: 45 issue: 1 year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B31 article-title: Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1 – volume: 28 start-page: 902 year: 2002 ident: key20180514124426_B61 article-title: Stimulus-driven and goal-driven control over visual selection publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 18 start-page: 604 year: 2006 ident: key20180514124426_B37 article-title: Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.604 – volume: 16 start-page: 121 year: 1990 ident: key20180514124426_B115 article-title: Abrupt Visual Onsets and Selective Attention: Voluntary Versus Automatic Allocation publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – ident: key20180514124426_B28 article-title: Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 52 start-page: 629 year: 2001 ident: key20180514124426_B77 article-title: Attention and Performance publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.629 – volume: 32 start-page: 3 year: 1980 ident: key20180514124426_B81 article-title: Orienting of Attention, The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett Lecture publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 15 start-page: 329 year: 2015 ident: key20180514124426_B85 article-title: Attentional capture by signals of threat publication-title: Cognition & Emotion – volume: 72 start-page: 326 issue: 2 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B9 article-title: What is top-down about contingent capture? publication-title: Attention Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/APP.72.2.326 – volume: 84 start-page: 127 issue: 3 year: 1977 ident: key20180514124426_B89 article-title: Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 48 start-page: 269 year: 1997 ident: key20180514124426_B22 article-title: Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269 – volume: 59 start-page: 1921 year: 2006 ident: key20180514124426_B84 article-title: Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470210500416367 – volume: 29 start-page: 1021 year: 2003 ident: key20180514124426_B70 article-title: Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighing publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 24 start-page: 790 issue: 5 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B40 article-title: Visual Working Memory Modulates Rapid Eye Movements to Simple Onset Targets publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797612459767 – volume: 315 start-page: 1860 year: 2007 ident: key20180514124426_B13 article-title: Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.1138071 – volume: 28 start-page: 309 issue: 3 year: 1998 ident: key20180514124426_B10 article-title: What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? publication-title: Brain Research Reviews doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8 – year: 1890 ident: key20180514124426_B47 – volume: 73 start-page: 2092 year: 2011 ident: key20180514124426_B105 article-title: On the limits of top-down control publication-title: Attention, Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0176-9 – volume: 29 start-page: 483 issue: 2 year: 2003 ident: key20180514124426_B114 article-title: Changing Your Mind: On the Contributions of Top-Down and Bottum-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Feature Singeltons publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 84 start-page: 1 year: 1977 ident: key20180514124426_B87 article-title: Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search, and Attention publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1 – volume: 61 start-page: 1009 year: 1999 ident: key20180514124426_B52 article-title: Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03207609 – volume: 3 start-page: 201 issue: 3 year: 2002 ident: key20180514124426_B18 article-title: Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/nrn755 – volume: 79 start-page: 1 year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B53 article-title: Psychonomics in Boston: A debate about types of orienting publication-title: Attention, Perception & Psychophysics – year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B108 article-title: Implicit guidance of attention: The Priority State Space framework publication-title: Cortex – volume: 20 start-page: 799 issue: 4 year: 1994 ident: key20180514124426_B95 article-title: Stimulus-Driven Capture and Attentional Set – Selective Search for Color and Visual Abrupt Onsets publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799 – volume: 85 start-page: 58 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B15 article-title: Rewards teach visual selective attention publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005 – volume: 31 start-page: 542 year: 2005 ident: key20180514124426_B64 article-title: Response selection modulates visual search within and across dimensions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 20 start-page: 778 issue: 6 year: 2009 ident: key20180514124426_B20 article-title: Learning to attend and to ignore is a matter of gains and losses publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02360.x – volume: 3 start-page: 284 year: 2000 ident: key20180514124426_B42 article-title: The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control publication-title: Nature Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/72999 – volume: 61 start-page: 61 year: 1999 ident: key20180514124426_B57 article-title: Limitations in attending to a feature value for overriding stimulus-driven interference publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211949 – volume: 74 start-page: 81 issue: 1 year: 1990 ident: key20180514124426_B91 article-title: Perceptual Selectivity Is Task Dependent – Evidence from Selective Search publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90036-F – year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B27 article-title: Don’t let it distract you: How information about the availability of reward affects attentional selection publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics – volume: 7 start-page: 571 year: 2000 ident: key20180514124426_B63 article-title: Priming of pop-out: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/135062800407202 – volume: 109 start-page: 160 year: 1980 ident: key20180514124426_B82 article-title: Attention and the detection of signals publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160 – volume: 48 start-page: 1315 issue: 11 year: 2008 ident: key20180514124426_B34 article-title: Expectancies modulate attentional capture by salient color singletons publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.006 – volume: 11 start-page: 551 issue: 3 year: 2004 ident: key20180514124426_B102 article-title: Attentional capture modulates perceptual sensitivity publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03196609 – volume: 201 start-page: 789 issue: 4 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B38 article-title: The time course of exogenous and endogenous control of covert attention publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2094-9 – volume: 50 start-page: 1362 issue: 14 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B75 article-title: Priming resolves perceptual ambiguity in visual search: evidence from behaviour and electrophysiology publication-title: Vision Research doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.022 – volume: 23 start-page: 429 issue: 4 year: 1994 ident: key20180514124426_B94 article-title: Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visual Selection publication-title: Perception doi: 10.1068/p230429 – volume: 33 start-page: 1335 issue: 6 year: 2007 ident: key20180514124426_B104 article-title: The role of spatial and nonspatial information in visual selection publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1335 – volume: 33 start-page: 271 issue: 2 year: 2007 ident: key20180514124426_B43 article-title: The time course of attentional and oculomotor capture reveals a common cause publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.271 – volume: 67 start-page: 1305 issue: 8 year: 2005 ident: key20180514124426_B99 article-title: Attentional capture and inhibition (of return): The effect on perceptual sensitivity publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03193636 – volume: 25 start-page: 1595 issue: 6 year: 1999 ident: key20180514124426_B101 article-title: Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1595 – volume: 36 start-page: 1 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B60 article-title: Attentional capture with rapidly changing attentional control settings publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0015875 – volume: 16 issue: 8 year: 2016 ident: key20180514124426_B83 article-title: Was That a Threat? Attentional biases by signals of threat publication-title: Emotion – volume: 14 start-page: 6 issue: 5 year: 2014 ident: key20180514124426_B23 article-title: Exogenous visual orienting by reward publication-title: Journal of Vision doi: 10.1167/14.5.6 – volume: 29 start-page: 1631 year: 1989 ident: key20180514124426_B71 article-title: Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention publication-title: Vision Res doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90144-2 – volume: 13 start-page: 1 issue: 3 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B106 article-title: Priming makes a stimulus more salient publication-title: Journal of Vision – year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B48 article-title: Habitual versus goal-driven attention publication-title: Cortex – volume: 30 start-page: 11096 issue: 33 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B36 article-title: Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010 – volume: 17 start-page: 222 issue: 3 year: 2006 ident: key20180514124426_B19 article-title: Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01689.x – volume: 13 start-page: 529 issue: 3 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B65 article-title: Conditioned fear modulates visual selection publication-title: Emotion doi: 10.1037/a0031076 – volume: 9 start-page: 379 issue: 5 year: 1998 ident: key20180514124426_B100 article-title: Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00071 – volume: 2 start-page: 194 issue: 3 year: 2001 ident: key20180514124426_B44 article-title: Computational modelling of visual attention publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/35058500 – volume: 135 start-page: 114 issue: 2 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B55 article-title: Priming in visual search: A spanner in the works for Theeuwes’s bottom-up attention sweeps? publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.001 – start-page: 1 year: 2017 ident: key20180514124426_B46 article-title: Sensitivity to value-driven attention is predicted by how we learn from value publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 39 start-page: 87 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B49 article-title: Rapid acquisition but slow extinction of an attentional bias in space publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance – volume: 51 start-page: 599 issue: 6 year: 1992 ident: key20180514124426_B93 article-title: Perceptual Selectivity for Color and Form publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211656 – volume: 14 start-page: 635 issue: 2 year: 2014 ident: key20180514124426_B12 article-title: The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0278-7 – volume: 93 start-page: 13494 year: 1996 ident: key20180514124426_B21 article-title: Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494 – volume: 67 start-page: 1354 issue: 8 year: 2005 ident: key20180514124426_B79 article-title: Target uncertainty does not lead to more distraction by singletons: intertrial priming does publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03193640 – volume: 14 start-page: 466 issue: 4–8 year: 2006 ident: key20180514124426_B103 article-title: Visual search for featural singletons: No top-down modulation, only bottom-up priming publication-title: Visual Cognition – volume: 40A start-page: 201 year: 1988 ident: key20180514124426_B109 article-title: “Features and objects: the fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture.” publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 72 start-page: 177 issue: 2 year: 1989 ident: key20180514124426_B90 article-title: Effects of Location and Form Cueing on the Allocation of Attention in the Visual-Field publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(89)90043-7 – volume: 114 start-page: 2316 issue: 4 year: 2015 ident: key20180514124426_B25 article-title: Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal the availability of reward publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.00441.2015 – volume: 20 start-page: 1254 year: 1998 ident: key20180514124426_B45 article-title: A model of saliency-based visual-attention for rapid scene analysis publication-title: IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence doi: 10.1109/34.730558 – volume: 368 start-page: 1 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B98 article-title: Feature-based attention: It is all bottom-up priming publication-title: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences – ident: key20180514124426_B29 – volume: 62 start-page: 800 year: 2000 ident: key20180514124426_B39 article-title: Repetition effects in visual search publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03206924 – volume: 50 start-page: 184 issue: 2 year: 1991 ident: key20180514124426_B92 article-title: Cross-Dimensional Perceptual Selectivity publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03212219 – volume: 144 start-page: 158 issue: 1 year: 2015 ident: key20180514124426_B59 article-title: When goals conflict with values: Counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/xge0000037 – year: 2016 ident: key20180514124426_B41 article-title: Eye Movements are Captured by a Perceptually Simple Conditioned Stimulus in the Absence of Explicit Contingency Knowledge publication-title: Emotion – volume: 24 start-page: 6371 issue: 28 year: 2004 ident: key20180514124426_B74 article-title: Target selection in area V4 during a multidimensional visual search task publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0569-04.2004 – volume: 11 start-page: 65 issue: 1 year: 2004 ident: key20180514124426_B96 article-title: Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03206462 – volume: 22 start-page: 4675 year: 2002 ident: key20180514124426_B11 article-title: Priming in macaque frontal cortex during popout visual search: Feature-based facilitation and location-based inhibition of return publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04675.2002 – volume: 18 start-page: 1030 issue: 4 year: 1992 ident: key20180514124426_B30 article-title: Involuntary Covert Orienting Is Contingent on Attentional Control Settings publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 – volume: 24 start-page: 667 issue: 5 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B116 article-title: Attention is spontaneously biased toward regularities publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797612460407 – volume: 135 start-page: 77 issue: 2 year: 2010 ident: key20180514124426_B97 article-title: Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006 – volume: 102 start-page: 1451 issue: 3 year: 2009 ident: key20180514124426_B67 article-title: Early and Late Modulation of Saccade Deviations by Target Distractor Similarity publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.00068.2009 – volume: 15 start-page: 315 year: 1989 ident: key20180514124426_B69 article-title: Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 16 start-page: 437 issue: 8 year: 2012 ident: key20180514124426_B5 article-title: Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010 – volume: 13 year: 2013 ident: key20180514124426_B58 article-title: Is goal-directed attentional guidance just intertrial priming? A review publication-title: Journal of Vision – volume: 23 start-page: 82 issue: 1–2 year: 2015 ident: key20180514124426_B24 article-title: Nonspatial Attentional Capture by Previously Rewarded Scene Semantics publication-title: Visual Cognition – start-page: 1 year: 2016 ident: key20180514124426_B73 article-title: People look at the object they fear: Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal threat publication-title: Cognition and Emotion – volume: 6 issue: 11 year: 2011 ident: key20180514124426_B3 article-title: Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture publication-title: PLoS One – volume: 67 start-page: 1252 year: 2005 ident: key20180514124426_B33 article-title: Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03193557 – volume: 47 start-page: 1 year: 2003 ident: key20180514124426_B76 article-title: Visual marking inhibits singleton capture publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00003-3 – volume: 1369 start-page: 24 issue: 1 year: 2016 ident: key20180514124426_B2 article-title: The attention habit: How reward learning shapes attentional selection publication-title: Annals of New York Academy Sciences doi: 10.1111/nyas.12957 – start-page: 255 volume-title: Cognition and Motor Processes year: 1984 ident: key20180514124426_B72 doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_17 – ident: key20180514124426_B112 article-title: How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression – start-page: 187 volume-title: Attention and performance year: 1981 ident: key20180514124426_B50 – volume: 23 start-page: 67 year: 2015 ident: key20180514124426_B1 article-title: Value-driven attentional capture is modulated by spatial context publication-title: Visual Cognition doi: 10.1080/13506285.2014.956851 – volume: 34 start-page: 210 year: 2011 ident: key20180514124426_B7 article-title: Mechanisms of top-down attention publication-title: Trends in Neurosciences doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003 – volume: 16 start-page: 315 issue: 2 year: 2016 ident: key20180514124426_B66 article-title: Distracted by danger: Temporal and spatial dynamics of visual selection in the presence of threat publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0391-2 |
SSID | ssj0002152063 |
Score | 2.4057112 |
Snippet | Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection (Awh,... Recently it was argued that in addition to top-down and bottom-up processes, lingering biases of selection history play a major role in visual selection ( Awh,... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 29 |
SubjectTerms | attention selection history top-down and bottom-up control |
Title | Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31517202 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2290838739 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6634613 https://doaj.org/article/728f1d587d3c4349a4de84b8adcab46d |
Volume | 1 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3BbtQwELWgp14QBQqhUIJEj2nt2LGdciqI1QoJOLRb9RbZHlu0WpKquyvUv-9Msl3tFiQuXO2JYr1xZp6V8RvGPkTM0l54KHQiUW2nYuGB-0IY4KBKbyDQfedv3_V4or5eVBdrrb6oJmyQBx6AOzKlTQIqa0AGJVXtFESrvHUQnFcaKPpizls7TFEMpm6tmHyHZkJ02_ToqguHQm5kn16k_2_M8mGB5FrGGT1lT5ZUMT8ZlrjDHsX2GdteRazb52x8fjlboMlp38wGET7OJzQwvc1HbjbPXQv5yWLe9bKsH8kMul_56bT73U-d97VvRMVfsMnoy9nncbFsjVAEzDqySMmUiYOXTlcB8DAZhBEV8DIlRFpj6IAEUadYRk5gcR6csL72qQoKeC132VbbtfEVy1P0yhiRkq6ikrpy-KQMOghrTKp9lbGDe8iasNQNp_YV0wbPDwRtg9A2Qmbs3cruelDK-MPiEyG-miVl634A_d0s_d38y98Ze3_vrwa_BPq94drYLWYNKddbaY2sM_Zy8N_qVRKJjSl5mTGz4dmNtWzOtJc_e7VtpGQKOc_r_7H4PbaNhMtS9YFQb9jW_GYR3yKpmft99viQ_9jvd_Ed2vf7pQ |
linkProvider | Directory of Open Access Journals |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Visual+Selection%3A+Usually+Fast+and+Automatic%3B+Seldom+Slow+and+Volitional&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+cognition&rft.au=Theeuwes%2C+Jan&rft.date=2018-05-14&rft.eissn=2514-4820&rft.volume=1&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=29&rft_id=info:doi/10.5334%2Fjoc.13&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F31517202&rft.externalDocID=31517202 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon |