Leader–follower dynamics during early social interactions matter for infant word learning
We know little about the mechanisms through which leader–follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants’ language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants’ decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these p...
Saved in:
Published in | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 121; no. 38; p. e2321008121 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
National Academy of Sciences
17.09.2024
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | We know little about the mechanisms through which leader–follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants’ language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants’ decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these proactive behaviors by naming the object it boosts infants’ word learning. To examine this, we invited caregivers and their 14-mo-old infants to play with novel objects, before testing infants’ retention of the novel object-label mappings. Meanwhile, their electroencephalograms were recorded. Results showed that infants’ proactive looks toward an object during play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during these episodes, infants showed greater word learning, but only when caregivers also joined their focus of attention. Our findings support the idea that infants’ proactive visual explorations guide their acquisition of a lexicon. |
---|---|
AbstractList | We know little about the mechanisms through which leader–follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants’ language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants’ decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these proactive behaviors by naming the object it boosts infants’ word learning. To examine this, we invited caregivers and their 14-mo-old infants to play with novel objects, before testing infants’ retention of the novel object-label mappings. Meanwhile, their electroencephalograms were recorded. Results showed that infants’ proactive looks toward an object during play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during these episodes, infants showed greater word learning, but only when caregivers also joined their focus of attention. Our findings support the idea that infants’ proactive visual explorations guide their acquisition of a lexicon. We know little about the mechanisms through which leader-follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants' language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants' decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these proactive behaviors by naming the object it boosts infants' word learning. To examine this, we invited caregivers and their 14-mo-old infants to play with novel objects, before testing infants' retention of the novel object-label mappings. Meanwhile, their electroencephalograms were recorded. Results showed that infants' proactive looks toward an object during play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during these episodes, infants showed greater word learning, but only when caregivers also joined their focus of attention. Our findings support the idea that infants' proactive visual explorations guide their acquisition of a lexicon.We know little about the mechanisms through which leader-follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants' language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants' decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these proactive behaviors by naming the object it boosts infants' word learning. To examine this, we invited caregivers and their 14-mo-old infants to play with novel objects, before testing infants' retention of the novel object-label mappings. Meanwhile, their electroencephalograms were recorded. Results showed that infants' proactive looks toward an object during play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during these episodes, infants showed greater word learning, but only when caregivers also joined their focus of attention. Our findings support the idea that infants' proactive visual explorations guide their acquisition of a lexicon. We still know little about how attentional states instantiated in the brain of infants and speakers, and leader–follower dynamics during early social interactions, affect the linguistic transmission process. Here, we address these issues by recording infants’ and caregivers’ brains while they played in a context that afforded word learning for the infant. Infants’ proactive looks toward an object during free-flowing play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during such infant-led episodes while joining their infants’ focus of attention, infants showed greater word learning. Thus, leader–follower attentional dynamics during caregiver–infant interactions matter for infant word learning. We know little about the mechanisms through which leader–follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants’ language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants’ decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these proactive behaviors by naming the object it boosts infants’ word learning. To examine this, we invited caregivers and their 14-mo-old infants to play with novel objects, before testing infants’ retention of the novel object-label mappings. Meanwhile, their electroencephalograms were recorded. Results showed that infants’ proactive looks toward an object during play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during these episodes, infants showed greater word learning, but only when caregivers also joined their focus of attention. Our findings support the idea that infants’ proactive visual explorations guide their acquisition of a lexicon. |
Author | Dautriche, Isabelle Wass, Sam Marriott-Haresign, Ira Henry, Zion Goupil, Louise Denman, Katherine |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Louise orcidid: 0000-0003-4342-9408 surname: Goupil fullname: Goupil, Louise organization: CNRS, Université Grenoble Alpes, Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, Grenoble 38400, France – sequence: 2 givenname: Isabelle surname: Dautriche fullname: Dautriche, Isabelle organization: Centre de Recherche en Psychologie et Neurosciences, Aix-Marseille University, CNRS, Marseille 13331, France – sequence: 3 givenname: Katherine surname: Denman fullname: Denman, Katherine organization: Department of Psychology, University of East London, London E15 4LZ, United Kingdom – sequence: 4 givenname: Zion surname: Henry fullname: Henry, Zion organization: Department of Psychology, University of East London, London E15 4LZ, United Kingdom – sequence: 5 givenname: Ira surname: Marriott-Haresign fullname: Marriott-Haresign, Ira organization: Department of Psychology, University of East London, London E15 4LZ, United Kingdom – sequence: 6 givenname: Sam orcidid: 0000-0002-7421-3493 surname: Wass fullname: Wass, Sam organization: Department of Psychology, University of East London, London E15 4LZ, United Kingdom |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/39254996$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed https://hal.science/hal-04694291$$DView record in HAL |
BookMark | eNp1kr9uFDEQxi0URC6Bmg6tREOKTWZs778KRREhSCfRQEVhDbY3ceSzD3s30XW8A2_Ik-DThQAnUVke_75vbH9zxA5CDJaxlwinCJ04WwfKp1xwBOiR4xO2QBiwbuUAB2wBwLu6l1wesqOcbwFgaHp4xg7FwBs5DO2CfVlaMjb9_P5jjN7He5sqswm0cjpXZk4uXFeWkt9UOWpHvnJhson05GLI1YqmsqvGmEp9pDBV9zGZyhdFKMrn7OlIPtsXD-sx-3z57tPFVb38-P7Dxfmy1hI6WXetADQWCCwRgCSOeuhbMYimQwJusLc4NoSIogFr2lH0ptc9jK2Rre7EMXu7813PX1fWaBumRF6tk1tR2qhITv17EtyNuo53ClFywEYWh5Odw82e7up8qbY1kO0g-YB3WNg3D91S_DbbPKmVy9p6T8HGOSuBwGUnoYWCvt5Db-OcQvmLQmGxa0pYhXr19_Uf-_8OqQBnO0CnmHOy4yOCoLZjoLZjoP6MQVE0ewrtJtqGVt7v_H91vwAL3rdb |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_devpsych_010923_110459 |
Cites_doi | 10.1177/0963721414522813 10.1080/15475441.2014.979387 10.1111/cdev.13620 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100810 10.1177/1745691617718355 10.1037/dev0000867 10.1111/infa.12590 10.1037/rev0000320 10.1073/pnas.1113380109 10.1111/lang.12512 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.026 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00152-1 10.1177/0963721409360003 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00044.x 10.1111/cdev.13072 10.1177/01650250544000062 10.1016/j.dr.2021.100961 10.1073/pnas.2122481120 10.1111/cdev.12707 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101093 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116602 10.1177/0956797612438734 10.1111/desc.12915 10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101331 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104191 10.1111/cdev.13274 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100680 10.1002/icd.1824 10.7554/eLife.88775.1 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006328 10.1037/dev0001285 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00135.x 10.1075/lald.44.06fer 10.1111/infa.12365 10.1007/978-3-319-77182-3_3 10.3758/s13423-013-0466-4 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.08.003 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00020.x 10.1111/desc.12562 10.2307/1130423 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright National Academy of Sciences Sep 17, 2024 Attribution Copyright © 2024 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2024 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Sep 17, 2024 – notice: Attribution – notice: Copyright © 2024 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2024 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7X8 1XC VOOES 5PM |
DOI | 10.1073/pnas.2321008121 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Immunology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Engineering Research Database AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access) PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Virology and AIDS Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Technology Research Database Nucleic Acids Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Entomology Abstracts Genetics Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Immunology Abstracts Engineering Research Database Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE Virology and AIDS Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) |
EISSN | 1091-6490 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC11420154 oai_HAL_hal_04694291v1 39254996 10_1073_pnas_2321008121 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: EC | H2020 | PRIORITY 'Excellent science' | H2020 Marie Skłodowska-Curie Actions (MSCA) grantid: 845859 – fundername: Leverhulme Trust grantid: RPG-2018-281 – fundername: ; grantid: 845859 – fundername: ; grantid: RPG-2018-281 |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 0R~ 123 29P 2FS 2WC 4.4 53G 5RE 5VS 85S AACGO AAFWJ AANCE AAYXX ABOCM ABPLY ABPPZ ABTLG ABZEH ACGOD ACIWK ACNCT ACPRK AENEX AFFNX AFOSN AFRAH ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BKOMP CITATION CS3 D0L DIK DU5 E3Z EBS F5P FRP GX1 H13 HH5 HYE JLS JSG KQ8 L7B LU7 N9A N~3 O9- OK1 PNE PQQKQ R.V RHI RNA RNS RPM RXW SJN TAE TN5 UKR W8F WH7 WOQ WOW X7M XSW Y6R YBH YKV YSK ZCA ~02 ~KM CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7X8 1XC UMC VOOES 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c4074-76301de0a0eaa004a21c986393571a02d18e1f5a111350ed6f38d8c80f6d46c73 |
ISSN | 0027-8424 1091-6490 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:34:47 EDT 2025 Fri May 09 12:16:32 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 23:47:15 EDT 2025 Mon Jun 30 09:59:01 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 06:54:58 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:35 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:37:17 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 38 |
Keywords | active learning leader–follower language acquisition social interactions |
Language | English |
License | Attribution: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by This article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND). |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4074-76301de0a0eaa004a21c986393571a02d18e1f5a111350ed6f38d8c80f6d46c73 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Edited by Daniel Swingley, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA; received December 8, 2023; accepted July 24, 2024 by Editorial Board Member Renée Baillargeon |
ORCID | 0000-0002-7421-3493 0000-0003-4342-9408 0000-0002-2297-985X |
OpenAccessLink | https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC11420154 |
PMID | 39254996 |
PQID | 3112915091 |
PQPubID | 42026 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11420154 hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_04694291v1 proquest_miscellaneous_3102474060 proquest_journals_3112915091 pubmed_primary_39254996 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2321008121 crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_2321008121 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2024-09-17 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2024-09-17 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 2024 text: 2024-09-17 day: 17 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: Washington |
PublicationTitle | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A |
PublicationYear | 2024 |
Publisher | National Academy of Sciences |
Publisher_xml | – name: National Academy of Sciences |
References | e_1_3_4_3_2 e_1_3_4_2_2 e_1_3_4_1_2 e_1_3_4_9_2 e_1_3_4_8_2 e_1_3_4_7_2 e_1_3_4_6_2 e_1_3_4_40_2 e_1_3_4_5_2 e_1_3_4_4_2 e_1_3_4_22_2 e_1_3_4_45_2 e_1_3_4_23_2 e_1_3_4_44_2 e_1_3_4_20_2 e_1_3_4_43_2 e_1_3_4_21_2 e_1_3_4_42_2 e_1_3_4_26_2 e_1_3_4_27_2 e_1_3_4_48_2 e_1_3_4_24_2 e_1_3_4_47_2 e_1_3_4_25_2 e_1_3_4_28_2 e_1_3_4_29_2 Schafer G. (e_1_3_4_41_2) 1998; 69 e_1_3_4_30_2 Michael C. (e_1_3_4_46_2) 2014 e_1_3_4_11_2 e_1_3_4_34_2 e_1_3_4_12_2 e_1_3_4_33_2 e_1_3_4_32_2 e_1_3_4_10_2 e_1_3_4_31_2 e_1_3_4_15_2 e_1_3_4_38_2 e_1_3_4_16_2 e_1_3_4_37_2 e_1_3_4_13_2 e_1_3_4_36_2 e_1_3_4_14_2 e_1_3_4_19_2 e_1_3_4_17_2 e_1_3_4_18_2 Wass S. (e_1_3_4_35_2) 2022; 16 e_1_3_4_39_2 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_3_4_5_2 doi: 10.1177/0963721414522813 – ident: e_1_3_4_37_2 doi: 10.1080/15475441.2014.979387 – ident: e_1_3_4_1_2 doi: 10.1111/cdev.13620 – ident: e_1_3_4_10_2 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100810 – ident: e_1_3_4_32_2 doi: 10.1177/1745691617718355 – ident: e_1_3_4_6_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_44_2 doi: 10.1037/dev0000867 – ident: e_1_3_4_7_2 doi: 10.1111/infa.12590 – ident: e_1_3_4_2_2 doi: 10.1037/rev0000320 – ident: e_1_3_4_14_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_39_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113380109 – ident: e_1_3_4_16_2 doi: 10.1111/lang.12512 – ident: e_1_3_4_45_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.026 – ident: e_1_3_4_19_2 doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00152-1 – ident: e_1_3_4_21_2 doi: 10.1177/0963721409360003 – ident: e_1_3_4_30_2 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X – ident: e_1_3_4_31_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00044.x – ident: e_1_3_4_34_2 doi: 10.1111/cdev.13072 – ident: e_1_3_4_33_2 doi: 10.1177/01650250544000062 – ident: e_1_3_4_48_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_13_2 doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2021.100961 – ident: e_1_3_4_25_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2122481120 – ident: e_1_3_4_24_2 doi: 10.1111/cdev.12707 – volume: 16 start-page: 896919 year: 2022 ident: e_1_3_4_35_2 article-title: Studying the developing brain in real-world contexts: Moving from castles in the air to castles on the ground publication-title: Front. Neurosci. – volume: 69 start-page: 309 year: 1998 ident: e_1_3_4_41_2 article-title: Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions publication-title: Child Dev. – ident: e_1_3_4_43_2 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101093 – ident: e_1_3_4_18_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116602 – ident: e_1_3_4_36_2 doi: 10.1177/0956797612438734 – ident: e_1_3_4_9_2 doi: 10.1111/desc.12915 – ident: e_1_3_4_15_2 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101331 – ident: e_1_3_4_38_2 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104191 – ident: e_1_3_4_28_2 doi: 10.1111/cdev.13274 – ident: e_1_3_4_20_2 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100680 – ident: e_1_3_4_40_2 doi: 10.1002/icd.1824 – ident: e_1_3_4_23_2 doi: 10.7554/eLife.88775.1 – ident: e_1_3_4_47_2 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006328 – ident: e_1_3_4_29_2 doi: 10.1037/dev0001285 – ident: e_1_3_4_26_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00135.x – ident: e_1_3_4_17_2 doi: 10.1075/lald.44.06fer – ident: e_1_3_4_42_2 doi: 10.1111/infa.12365 – ident: e_1_3_4_3_2 doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-77182-3_3 – volume-title: Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice year: 2014 ident: e_1_3_4_46_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_4_2 doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0466-4 – ident: e_1_3_4_12_2 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.08.003 – ident: e_1_3_4_8_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00020.x – ident: e_1_3_4_22_2 doi: 10.1111/desc.12562 – ident: e_1_3_4_11_2 doi: 10.2307/1130423 – ident: e_1_3_4_27_2 |
SSID | ssj0009580 |
Score | 2.4602592 |
Snippet | We know little about the mechanisms through which leader–follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants’ language acquisition. We hypothesized that... We know little about the mechanisms through which leader-follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants' language acquisition. We hypothesized that... We know little about the mechanisms through which leader–follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants' language acquisition. We hypothesized that... We still know little about how attentional states instantiated in the brain of infants and speakers, and leader–follower dynamics during early social... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral hal proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | e2321008121 |
SubjectTerms | Attention Attention - physiology Biological Sciences Cognitive science EEG Electroencephalography Female Humans Infant Infants Language Development Learning Learning - physiology Male Social behavior Social discrimination learning Social Interaction Social interactions Verbal Learning - physiology Visual discrimination learning Visual perception Words (language) |
Title | Leader–follower dynamics during early social interactions matter for infant word learning |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/39254996 https://www.proquest.com/docview/3112915091 https://www.proquest.com/docview/3102474060 https://hal.science/hal-04694291 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC11420154 |
Volume | 121 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEF6l5cIFUZ6GghbEoShy8GPtdY4RtEQQQg-JFLhYa3tNK4FT1TEHzvxwZvZhx2mRChcrWm_s1c63s7PjmW8IeSUikQsuQQKg-1w29qUrgoC7kZClL8dJyRXZ86d5PF2yD6toNRj83opaajbZKP91bV7J_0gV2kCumCX7D5JtHwoN8BvkC1eQMFxvJGNdH9MtQZZY7GxY6PLytU0-lIq92LjFkRjiUqcx1MMfilZTxRjCQGB2sWZMYWtIfNs2WU_bLa62AQVz60GcdPkoRknUQ3d4Ou-qG79fNxfazTxbN-fdl6B3AssAAGKUkqoFfv_obsrKOGY_2gTFzmdrkiq-WkAZl0XAML5CZ2gaLQtGihszXSd0JK9ps6pZZ08bDGoaGKNpZYDZR2DQ6C5XdgJQXVi-uBL1qN-zz7k9_5yeLGezdHG8WuyRWwEcNlR46HSbujnRnBZmgJYgiodvdh7fs232zjCy9uqxZTf6dsucWdwld8w5hE40qA7IQFb3yIEVIj0ydOSv75MvOyijFmVUo4wqlFGNMrqNMqpRRgFlVKOMIsqoRdkDsjw5XryduqYgh5szFbcbw3ZQSE94UghYhyLw83ESY3Y394UXFH4i_TISsH-GkSeLuAyTIskTr4wLFuc8fEj2q3UlHxOaBXHExThiMuYsClgWskD6PJOhV3ImQ4eM7FSmuWGrx6Ip31MVNcHDFOc-7ebeIUftHy40Ucvfu74E2bS9kGB9Opml2IbeIrDQ_J_Q6dCKLjVLvk5DPJ34aGM75EV7GxQyfmUTlVw32AcQz8FO9hzySEu6fRUcRtAfEzsk6WGgN5b-ner8TJG-Y847nnee3ODFT8ntbtkdkv3NZSOfge28yZ4raP8BurbF0g |
linkProvider | ABC ChemistRy |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Leader-follower+dynamics+during+early+social+interactions+matter+for+infant+word+learning&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Goupil%2C+Louise&rft.au=Dautriche%2C+Isabelle&rft.au=Denman%2C+Katherine&rft.au=Henry%2C+Zion&rft.date=2024-09-17&rft.issn=1091-6490&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft.volume=121&rft.issue=38&rft.spage=e2321008121&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.2321008121&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |