Children’s overtensing errors: Phonological and lexical effects on syntax
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗ didn’t br o ke for target didn’t br ea k) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more co...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of memory and language Vol. 57; no. 1; pp. 49 - 64 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
San Diego, CA
Elsevier Inc
01.07.2007
Elsevier Elsevier BV |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0749-596X 1096-0821 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗
didn’t br
o
ke for target
didn’t br
ea
k) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *didn't broke for target didn't break) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT] Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *didn't broke for target didn't break) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. [Copyright 2007 Elsevier Inc.] Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗ didn’t br o ke for target didn’t br ea k) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. |
Author | Stemberger, Joseph Paul |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Joseph Paul surname: Stemberger fullname: Stemberger, Joseph Paul email: stemberg@interchange.ubc.ca organization: Department of Linguistics, University of British Columbia, E270-1866 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z1 |
BackLink | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ763653$$DView record in ERIC http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18782140$$DView record in Pascal Francis |
BookMark | eNp9kcFqFTEUhoNU8Lb6AIKLQdDdjCeZSTKjq3KpVi3oQsFdyGRO2gy5SU3mlnbna_h6PklT70Whi67Ogf_7f07yH5KDEAMS8pxCQ4GKN3Mzb3zDAGQDrAFoH5EVhUHU0DN6QFYgu6Hmg_jxhBzmPANQyiVbkc_rC-enhOHPr9-5ileYFgzZhfMKU4opv62-XsQQfTx3RvtKh6nyeP13R2vRLMUUqnwTFn39lDy22md8tp9H5Pv7k2_r0_rsy4eP6-Oz2nQglnpqB8EAR9pNg-5kL0dmLTcTcE3RoqSaj50YObNysi1jg-wN73ActaScdbo9Iq93uZcp_txiXtTGZYPe64Bxm1XPhRC97Av48h44x20K5TbFKIeWt6Ir0Ks9pHN5lk06GJfVZXIbnW4ULUGMdlC4FzsOkzP_5JNPUrSCt0WmO9mkmHNC-z8B1F1FalalInVXkQKmSkXFI-95jFv04mJYknb-Qee7_THln68cJpWNw2BwcqmUoqboHnDfAkyrrQg |
CODEN | JMLAE6 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1075_ml_9_1_05ste crossref_primary_10_1075_ml_8_3_03ste crossref_primary_10_2989_16073614_2022_2063146 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11525_024_09434_x crossref_primary_10_1080_01690965_2013_849812 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1505 crossref_primary_10_1044_2017_JSLHR_L_16_0402 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90032-7 10.1017/S0305000900007510 10.1080/01690960244000045 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90018-8 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01990-3 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90002-2 10.1207/s15516709cog2606_2 10.1037/h0036026 10.1017/S0305000900007297 10.1080/01690969308406948 10.3758/BF03209225 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90042-9 10.1017/S0305000900006875 10.1017/S0305000900007558 10.1207/s15516709cog2103_2 10.1017/S0140525X99002228 10.1017/S030500090000845X 10.2307/415792 10.2307/414099 10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.001 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283 10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661 10.1073/pnas.96.13.7592 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90003-4 10.1017/S0305000900001148 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01993-9 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90017-7 10.2307/1128013 10.1017/S0305000900001999 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90046-7 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90046-0 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80021-2 10.1007/BF00133692 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.001 10.2307/1130007 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2007 Elsevier Inc. 2007 INIST-CNRS Copyright © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2007 Elsevier Inc. – notice: 2007 INIST-CNRS – notice: Copyright © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN IQODW 7T9 8BM |
DOI | 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ERIC ERIC (Ovid) ERIC ERIC ERIC (Legacy Platform) ERIC( SilverPlatter ) ERIC ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform) Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) ERIC Pascal-Francis Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) ComDisDome |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ERIC Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) ComDisDome |
DatabaseTitleList | ERIC Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: ERI name: ERIC url: https://eric.ed.gov/ sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Languages & Literatures Education Psychology |
EISSN | 1096-0821 |
ERIC | EJ763653 |
EndPage | 64 |
ExternalDocumentID | 1274396681 18782140 EJ763653 10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003 S0749596X07000277 |
Genre | Feature |
GroupedDBID | --K --M --Z -DZ -~X .GJ .~1 07C 0R3 0R~ 186 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29L 3EH 4.4 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 7-5 71M 85S 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACLR AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFJI AAGJA AAGJQ AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AATGF AAWTL AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABGZZ ABIVO ABJNI ABMAC ABMMH ABOYX ABPPZ ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACFII ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACIOK ACMRT ACNCT ACREJ ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADFGL ADIYS ADMUD ADPVW ADSFD AEBSH AEBYS AEFWE AEKER AENEX AETEA AFFDN AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJGPC AJOXV AKYCK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ AOMHK ASPBG AVARZ AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BKOMP BLXMC CAG COF CS3 DM4 EBS ECVKH EFBJH EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q G8K GBLVA HMW HMY HVGLF HYQOX HZ~ IHE J1W K-O KC5 KOM LG5 LPU M3U M3X M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A O-F O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PQQKQ PRBVW Q38 Q5E R2- RIG ROL RPZ SDF SDG SES SEW SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSO SSS SSY SSZ T5K TN5 UNMZH WH7 WUQ XJT XOL XPP XSW XZL YK3 YQT YYQ ZA5 ZCA ZCG ZMT ZU3 ZXP ~G- ~OB ~OC ~OG ~PJ ~PM ~PN AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABDPE ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADNMO ADVLN ADXHL AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION PMKZF PVKVW SSH 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO EFKBS ERI PET PMFND REK WWN 08R ABPIF ABPTK AKALU IQODW 7T9 8BM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c406t-d39620eb14d9a4787b2ff5cd05a1efe71a5b46b52f7df322978c54ebba71524a3 |
IEDL.DBID | AIKHN |
ISSN | 0749-596X |
IngestDate | Mon Jul 21 09:40:25 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 04:42:53 EDT 2025 Sun Oct 22 16:09:28 EDT 2023 Tue Sep 02 19:19:25 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:01:50 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:10:18 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:32:36 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | false |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | Phonology–morphology interactions Connectionist models First language acquisition Dual route models Morphology Symbolic models Language production Overtensing Regularity Frequency effects English past-tense Lexicon Grammatical tense Cognition Verbal production Connectionism Language Phonology-morphology interactions Child Human Grammar English Language development Frequency Phonology Models Syntax |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c406t-d39620eb14d9a4787b2ff5cd05a1efe71a5b46b52f7df322978c54ebba71524a3 |
Notes | SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 14 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
PQID | 215035364 |
PQPubID | 29955 |
PageCount | 16 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_85666878 proquest_journals_215035364 pascalfrancis_primary_18782140 eric_primary_EJ763653 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2007-07-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2007-07-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 07 year: 2007 text: 2007-07-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | San Diego, CA |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: San Diego, CA – name: New York |
PublicationTitle | Journal of memory and language |
PublicationYear | 2007 |
Publisher | Elsevier Inc Elsevier Elsevier BV |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Inc – name: Elsevier – name: Elsevier BV |
References | MacKay (bib29) 1976; 15 Jaeger (bib24) 2004 Kuczaj (bib27) 1976; 3 McCarthy (bib35) 2005 Stemberger (bib49) 2002; 26 Valian (bib57) 1991; 40 Bates, Bretherton, Snyder (bib2) 1988 Rumelhart, McClelland (bib43) 1986; Vol. 1 Pinker (bib38) 1984 Marcus, Pinker, Ullman, Hollander, Rosen, Xu (bib33) 1992; 57 Hurford (bib23) 1975; 2 Clark (bib11) 1978 McCarthy, J.J., & Wolf, M. (2005). Less than zero: Correspondence and the null output. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-772 Bloom, Hood, Lightbown (bib6) 1974; 6 Pinker, Ullman (bib40) 2002; 6 Stemberger, Middleton (bib54) 2003; 18 Stemberger, MacWhinney (bib52) 1986; 18 Bybee, Slobin (bib9) 1982; 58 Wells (bib59) 1981 . Gathercole (bib17) 1986; 13 Kuczaj (bib28) 1977; 16 Brown (bib7) 1973 Bernhardt, Stemberger (bib5) 1998 Suppes (bib55) 1974; 29 Joanisse, Seidenberg (bib25) 1999; 96 Stemberger (bib46) 1991; 30 MacWhinney (bib30) 2000 Marchman (bib32) 1997; 21 Stemberger (bib45) 1989; 28 Kiparsky (bib26) 1982 Berko (bib4) 1958; 14 Stemberger (bib47) 1992; 68 Stemberger (bib48) 1993 Fay (bib14) 1978; 5 Gleason (bib18) 1980 Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (in press). The dislike of regular plurals in compounds: Phonological or morphological. Gordon (bib19) 1985; 21 Stemberger, J.P. (2004). Output-oriented similarity effects in the production of English past tense. Paper presented at the Forty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, Nov. 2004. Cruttenden (bib12) 1978; 5 Warren-Leubecker, Bohannon (bib58) 1984; 55 Maratsos, Kuczaj (bib31) 1978; 5 Dell (bib13) 1986; 93 Hammond (bib20) 1999 Theakston, Lieven, Pine, Rowland (bib56) 2000 Stemberger, J.P., & Bernhardt, B.H. (2001). U-shaped learning in language development, and restrictions on error correction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, April 2001. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-472 Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Kansas City, Nov. 2002. Andrews (bib1) 1990; 8 Francis, Kučera (bib15) 1982 Stemberger (bib44) 1982 Pinker, Prince (bib39) 1988; 28 Prasada, Pinker (bib41) 1993; 8 Buck-Gengler, Menn, Healy (bib8) 2004; 90 Henry (bib21) 1995 Stemberger, MacWhinney (bib53) 1986; 14 Garvey, Hogan (bib16) 1973; 44 Hollebrandse, Roeper (bib22) 1996 McClelland, Patterson (bib37) 2002; 6 Clahsen (bib10) 1999; 22 Ramscar, M.J.A. (2002). Learning language from the input: Why innate constraints aren’t implicated in noun-noun compounding. Paper presented at the 43 Marshall, Van der Lely (bib34) 2006; 100 Bybee (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib9) 1982; 58 Gathercole (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib17) 1986; 13 Bloom (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib6) 1974; 6 Suppes (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib55) 1974; 29 Marchman (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib32) 1997; 21 Hurford (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib23) 1975; 2 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib52) 1986; 18 Kuczaj (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib28) 1977; 16 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib44) 1982 Maratsos (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib31) 1978; 5 Hammond (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib20) 1999 Pinker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib40) 2002; 6 Gleason (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib18) 1980 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib42 Wells (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib59) 1981 Henry (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib21) 1995 Prasada (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib41) 1993; 8 Hollebrandse (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib22) 1996 MacWhinney (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib30) 2000 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib46) 1991; 30 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib47) 1992; 68 Clahsen (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib10) 1999; 22 Marshall (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib34) 2006; 100 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib54) 2003; 18 Buck-Gengler (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib8) 2004; 90 Jaeger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib24) 2004 Fay (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib14) 1978; 5 Pinker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib38) 1984 Rumelhart (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib43) 1986; Vol. 1 Garvey (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib16) 1973; 44 Brown (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib7) 1973 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib50 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib51 Gordon (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib19) 1985; 21 Berko (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib4) 1958; 14 McClelland (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib37) 2002; 6 Cruttenden (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib12) 1978; 5 Andrews (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib1) 1990; 8 Pinker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib39) 1988; 28 Warren-Leubecker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib58) 1984; 55 Dell (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib13) 1986; 93 McCarthy (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib35) 2005 Kuczaj (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib27) 1976; 3 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib45) 1989; 28 Bates (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib2) 1988 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib49) 2002; 26 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib53) 1986; 14 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib3 Marcus (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib33) 1992; 57 Joanisse (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib25) 1999; 96 Clark (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib11) 1978 Bernhardt (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib5) 1998 Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib48) 1993 MacKay (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib29) 1976; 15 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib36 Valian (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib57) 1991; 40 Theakston (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib56) 2000 Francis (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib15) 1982 Kiparsky (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib26) 1982 |
References_xml | – volume: 8 start-page: 507 year: 1990 end-page: 557 ident: bib1 article-title: Unification and morphological blocking publication-title: Natural Language & Linguistic Theory – year: 1982 ident: bib15 article-title: Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar – volume: 21 start-page: 283 year: 1997 end-page: 304 ident: bib32 article-title: Children’s productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure publication-title: Cognitive Science – volume: 28 start-page: 73 year: 1988 end-page: 194 ident: bib39 article-title: On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition publication-title: Cognition – reference: McCarthy, J.J., & Wolf, M. (2005). Less than zero: Correspondence and the null output. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-772: < – reference: Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (in press). The dislike of regular plurals in compounds: Phonological or morphological. – volume: 8 start-page: 1 year: 1993 end-page: 56 ident: bib41 article-title: Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – volume: 100 start-page: 302 year: 2006 end-page: 320 ident: bib34 article-title: A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics publication-title: Cognition – volume: 3 start-page: 423 year: 1976 end-page: 427 ident: bib27 article-title: Arguments against Hurford’s ‘Aux Copying Rule’ publication-title: Journal of Child Language – volume: 6 start-page: 456 year: 2002 end-page: 463 ident: bib40 article-title: The past and future of the past tense publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 2 start-page: 299 year: 1975 end-page: 301 ident: bib23 article-title: A child and the English question formation rule publication-title: Journal of Child Language – year: 1999 ident: bib20 article-title: The phonology of English: A prosodic optimality-theoretic approach – volume: 6 start-page: 465 year: 2002 end-page: 472 ident: bib37 article-title: Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 5 start-page: 337 year: 1978 end-page: 345 ident: bib31 article-title: Against the transformational account: A simpler analysis of auxiliary overmarkings publication-title: Journal of Child Language – reference: Stemberger, J.P. (2004). Output-oriented similarity effects in the production of English past tense. Paper presented at the Forty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, Nov. 2004. – volume: 44 start-page: 562 year: 1973 end-page: 568 ident: bib16 article-title: Social speech and social interaction: Egocentrism revisited publication-title: Child Development – volume: Vol. 1 start-page: 216 year: 1986 end-page: 271 ident: bib43 article-title: On learning the past tenses of English verbs publication-title: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition – start-page: 130 year: 1982 end-page: 175 ident: bib26 article-title: From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology publication-title: The structure of phonological representations, part 1 – start-page: 170 year: 2005 end-page: 210 ident: bib35 article-title: Optimal paradigms publication-title: Paradigms in phonological theory – reference: Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Kansas City, Nov. 2002. – volume: 21 start-page: 73 year: 1985 end-page: 93 ident: bib19 article-title: Level ordering in lexical development publication-title: Cognition – volume: 13 start-page: 537 year: 1986 end-page: 560 ident: bib17 article-title: The acquisition of the present perfect: explaining differences in the speech of Scottish and American children publication-title: Journal of Child Language – volume: 15 start-page: 169 year: 1976 end-page: 182 ident: bib29 article-title: On the retrieval and lexical structure of verbs publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior – year: 2004 ident: bib24 article-title: Kids’ slips: What young children’s slips of the tongue reveal about language development – volume: 28 start-page: 164 year: 1989 end-page: 188 ident: bib45 article-title: Speech errors in early child language production publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 29 start-page: 103 year: 1974 end-page: 114 ident: bib55 article-title: The semantics of children’s language publication-title: American Psychologist – volume: 40 start-page: 21 year: 1991 end-page: 81 ident: bib57 article-title: Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children publication-title: Cognition – start-page: 261 year: 1996 end-page: 271 ident: bib22 article-title: The concept of DO-insertion and the theory of INFL publication-title: Proceedings of the Groningen Assembly on Language Acquisition – start-page: 119 year: 2000 end-page: 128 ident: bib56 article-title: The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of ’mixed’ verb-argument structure at stage 1 publication-title: New directions in language development and disorders – volume: 14 start-page: 150 year: 1958 end-page: 177 ident: bib4 article-title: The child’s learning of English morphology publication-title: Word – volume: 14 start-page: 17 year: 1986 end-page: 26 ident: bib53 article-title: Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms publication-title: Memory and Cognition – reference: . – volume: 30 start-page: 161 year: 1991 end-page: 185 ident: bib46 article-title: Apparent anti-frequency effects in language production: The addition bias and phonological underspecification publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 6 start-page: 380 year: 1974 end-page: 420 ident: bib6 article-title: Imitation in language development: If, when and why publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 22 start-page: 991 year: 1999 end-page: 1060 ident: bib10 article-title: Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection publication-title: Brain & Behavior Science – volume: 68 start-page: 492 year: 1992 end-page: 524 ident: bib47 article-title: Vocalic underspecification in English language production publication-title: Language – volume: 26 start-page: 737 year: 2002 end-page: 766 ident: bib49 article-title: Overtensing and the effect of regularity publication-title: Cognitive Science – year: 2000 ident: bib30 article-title: The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk – volume: 55 start-page: 1379 year: 1984 end-page: 1385 ident: bib58 article-title: Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother-father speech publication-title: Child Development – year: 1988 ident: bib2 article-title: From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms – reference: Stemberger, J.P., & Bernhardt, B.H. (2001). U-shaped learning in language development, and restrictions on error correction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, April 2001. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-472: < – volume: 93 start-page: 283 year: 1986 end-page: 321 ident: bib13 article-title: A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production publication-title: Psychological Review – year: 1984 ident: bib38 article-title: Language learnability and language development – year: 1982 ident: bib44 article-title: The lexicon in a model of language production. Doctoral Dissertation, UCSD – volume: 5 start-page: 143 year: 1978 end-page: 149 ident: bib14 article-title: Transformations as mental operations: A reply to Kuczaj publication-title: Journal of Child Language – year: 1995 ident: bib21 article-title: Belfast English and standard English: Dialect variation and parameter setting – volume: 90 start-page: 453 year: 2004 end-page: 464 ident: bib8 article-title: What “mice-trap” tells us about the mental lexicon publication-title: Brain & Language – volume: 57 year: 1992 ident: bib33 article-title: Overregularization in language acquisition publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development – year: 1981 ident: bib59 article-title: Learning through interaction: The study of language development – year: 1980 ident: bib18 article-title: The acquisition of social speech and politeness formulae publication-title: Language: Social psychological perspectives – volume: 58 start-page: 265 year: 1982 end-page: 289 ident: bib9 article-title: Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past-tense publication-title: Language – volume: 5 start-page: 373 year: 1978 end-page: 378 ident: bib12 article-title: Assimilation in child language and elsewhere publication-title: Journal of Child Language – volume: 18 start-page: 329 year: 1986 end-page: 354 ident: bib52 article-title: Form-oriented errors in inflectional processing publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – start-page: 503 year: 1993 end-page: 521 ident: bib48 article-title: Vowel dominance in overregularization publication-title: Journal of Child Language,20 – year: 1998 ident: bib5 article-title: Handbook of phonological development: From the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology – year: 1978 ident: bib11 article-title: Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do publication-title: The child’s conception of language – volume: 16 start-page: 589 year: 1977 end-page: 600 ident: bib28 article-title: The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior – year: 1973 ident: bib7 article-title: A first language: The early stages – reference: Ramscar, M.J.A. (2002). Learning language from the input: Why innate constraints aren’t implicated in noun-noun compounding. Paper presented at the 43 – volume: 96 start-page: 7592 year: 1999 end-page: 7597 ident: bib25 article-title: Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: A connectionist model publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA – reference: >. – volume: 18 start-page: 369 year: 2003 end-page: 404 ident: bib54 article-title: Vowel dominance and morphological processing publication-title: Language & Cognitive Processes – volume: 57 issue: whole no. 4 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib33 article-title: Overregularization in language acquisition publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development – year: 1980 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib18 article-title: The acquisition of social speech and politeness formulae – year: 1981 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib59 – volume: 28 start-page: 73 year: 1988 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib39 article-title: On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90032-7 – volume: 5 start-page: 337 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib31 article-title: Against the transformational account: A simpler analysis of auxiliary overmarkings publication-title: Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305000900007510 – start-page: 119 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib56 article-title: The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of ’mixed’ verb-argument structure at stage 1 – start-page: 170 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib35 article-title: Optimal paradigms – volume: 18 start-page: 369 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib54 article-title: Vowel dominance and morphological processing publication-title: Language & Cognitive Processes doi: 10.1080/01690960244000045 – volume: 6 start-page: 380 year: 1974 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib6 article-title: Imitation in language development: If, when and why publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90018-8 – year: 1982 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib44 – year: 1973 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib7 – volume: 6 start-page: 456 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib40 article-title: The past and future of the past tense publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01990-3 – ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib50 – year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib11 article-title: Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do – volume: 30 start-page: 161 year: 1991 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib46 article-title: Apparent anti-frequency effects in language production: The addition bias and phonological underspecification publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90002-2 – volume: 26 start-page: 737 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib49 article-title: Overtensing and the effect of regularity publication-title: Cognitive Science doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2606_2 – volume: 29 start-page: 103 year: 1974 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib55 article-title: The semantics of children’s language publication-title: American Psychologist doi: 10.1037/h0036026 – year: 1988 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib2 – volume: 3 start-page: 423 year: 1976 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib27 article-title: Arguments against Hurford’s ‘Aux Copying Rule’ publication-title: Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305000900007297 – volume: 8 start-page: 1 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib41 article-title: Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes doi: 10.1080/01690969308406948 – ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib3 – ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib42 – volume: 14 start-page: 17 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib53 article-title: Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms publication-title: Memory and Cognition doi: 10.3758/BF03209225 – volume: 28 start-page: 164 year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib45 article-title: Speech errors in early child language production publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90042-9 – volume: 13 start-page: 537 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib17 article-title: The acquisition of the present perfect: explaining differences in the speech of Scottish and American children publication-title: Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305000900006875 – volume: 5 start-page: 373 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib12 article-title: Assimilation in child language and elsewhere publication-title: Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305000900007558 – volume: 21 start-page: 283 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib32 article-title: Children’s productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure publication-title: Cognitive Science doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2103_2 – volume: 22 start-page: 991 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib10 article-title: Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection publication-title: Brain & Behavior Science doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002228 – start-page: 503 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib48 article-title: Vowel dominance in overregularization publication-title: Journal of Child Language,20 doi: 10.1017/S030500090000845X – ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib36 – volume: 68 start-page: 492 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib47 article-title: Vocalic underspecification in English language production publication-title: Language doi: 10.2307/415792 – volume: 58 start-page: 265 year: 1982 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib9 article-title: Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past-tense publication-title: Language doi: 10.2307/414099 – year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib24 – year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib21 – year: 1984 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib38 – start-page: 261 year: 1996 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib22 article-title: The concept of DO-insertion and the theory of INFL – volume: 90 start-page: 453 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib8 article-title: What “mice-trap” tells us about the mental lexicon publication-title: Brain & Language doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.001 – volume: 93 start-page: 283 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib13 article-title: A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283 – year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib30 – volume: 14 start-page: 150 year: 1958 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib4 article-title: The child’s learning of English morphology publication-title: Word doi: 10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661 – year: 1982 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib15 – volume: 96 start-page: 7592 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib25 article-title: Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: A connectionist model publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.13.7592 – volume: 18 start-page: 329 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib52 article-title: Form-oriented errors in inflectional processing publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90003-4 – start-page: 130 year: 1982 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib26 article-title: From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology – volume: 2 start-page: 299 year: 1975 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib23 article-title: A child and the English question formation rule publication-title: Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305000900001148 – volume: Vol. 1 start-page: 216 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib43 article-title: On learning the past tenses of English verbs – volume: 6 start-page: 465 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib37 article-title: Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01993-9 – volume: 15 start-page: 169 year: 1976 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib29 article-title: On the retrieval and lexical structure of verbs publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior doi: 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90017-7 – volume: 44 start-page: 562 year: 1973 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib16 article-title: Social speech and social interaction: Egocentrism revisited publication-title: Child Development doi: 10.2307/1128013 – volume: 5 start-page: 143 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib14 article-title: Transformations as mental operations: A reply to Kuczaj publication-title: Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305000900001999 – volume: 40 start-page: 21 year: 1991 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib57 article-title: Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90046-7 – volume: 21 start-page: 73 year: 1985 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib19 article-title: Level ordering in lexical development publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90046-0 – year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib20 – volume: 16 start-page: 589 year: 1977 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib28 article-title: The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80021-2 – volume: 8 start-page: 507 year: 1990 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib1 article-title: Unification and morphological blocking publication-title: Natural Language & Linguistic Theory doi: 10.1007/BF00133692 – ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib51 – volume: 100 start-page: 302 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib34 article-title: A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.001 – volume: 55 start-page: 1379 year: 1984 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib58 article-title: Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother-father speech publication-title: Child Development doi: 10.2307/1130007 – year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib5 |
SSID | ssj0011572 |
Score | 1.8775138 |
Snippet | Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗
didn’t br
o
ke for target
didn’t br
ea
k) is common in early syntax. In a... Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a... Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *didn't broke for target didn't break) is common in early syntax. In a... |
SourceID | proquest pascalfrancis eric crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 49 |
SubjectTerms | Biological and medical sciences Child Child development Child Language Children Children & youth Connectionist models Developmental psychology Dual route models English English past-tense Error Analysis Error Patterns First language acquisition Frequency effects Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Grammar Inflection Language acquisition Language Processing Language production Linguistics Memory Morphemes Morphology Negation Overtensing Phonemes Phonetics Phonological Processing Phonology Phonology–morphology interactions Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Regularity Rhyme Symbolic models Syntax Tense Word Frequency |
Title | Children’s overtensing errors: Phonological and lexical effects on syntax |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003 http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ763653 https://www.proquest.com/docview/215035364 https://www.proquest.com/docview/85666878 |
Volume | 57 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NbtQwEB7151IJIVgKLIXFB8QBKWzs2M6GW7VqtbSlQoJKe7Ps2CmtirPabCV6QbwGr8eTMM46QRWiB26RY0fOzHh-7PE3AK-cLIUxGJbYCmNVznKb6MwVSWYwDkuNy3mLtv_hVM7O-NFczDdg2t2FCWmVUfevdXqrrWPLOFJzvLi4GH9C41eIQs5RaENwlW_CNssKiaK9vf_-eHbaHyZQ0dZwCv2TMKA73GzTvC6_XkUgw4Dcmf3LPMVk6HsL3SDtqnXRi7_0d2uUDh_A_ehNkv31hB_ChvODUIg5Jm0M4MlJ3I9syGty0kMoNwPY6VXfzSM4nsY73b9-_GxIyOpsE9v9OXHLZb1s3pGPX2rf6UmivSUBSDM8x4QQUnvS3PiV_rYLZ4cHn6ezJJZZSEq05qvEItFYijqb20IHrB7DqkqUNhWausrlVAvDpRGsym2F6x_jzlJwZ4zO0fhznT2GLV979xSIsDST1tEJBtZcOzaxjNrgkklDA9D9ENKOuqqMGOShFMaV6pLNLhUyJNTGzFXKAnDpEN70QxZrAI67OvOOZeqWFCk0EHcN2w3s7b9_cIRaVwpsH93i958J4A8yDE2HsNcJgIprv1HoRKWZyCQfwsv-LS7acBKjvauvGzVBJ1riN57933z3YKfbYk7pc9haLa_dC_SNVmYEm2-_01FcAaN2N-A3gBgN_A |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Pb9MwFH8a48AkhKCMUQabD4gDUmji2E7DDVWbytZNSGxSb5YdO7Bpc6qmk9gF8TX4evske06coAmxA7fIsSPH9vtn__x7AG-tKLjWGJaYEmNVRjMTqdTmUaoxDou1zVjDtn90LKan7GDO52sw6e7CeFhl0P2tTm-0dSgZhdEcLc7ORl_R-OU8F3NctD64yh7AQ4bi66Xzw88e5-HJZGjLxZlHvnp3tNmAvM4vLwKNoeftTP9lnAIU-vFC1ThyZZvy4i_t3Zik_afwJPiS5FPb3WewZt3Ap2EOkI0BbM3CbmRN3pFZT6BcD2CjV3zXz-FwEm503_z6XROP6Wxg7e4bsctltaw_ki_fK9dpSaKcIZ5G0z8HOAipHKmv3Ur92ITT_b2TyTQKSRaiAm35KjJpLmiMGpuZXHmmHk3Lkhcm5iqxpc0SxTUTmtMyMyVKP0adBWdWa5Wh6WcqfQHrrnL2JRBuklQYm4wxrGbK0rGhifEOmdCJp7kfQtyNriwCA7lPhHEhO6jZucQJ8ZkxMxlTT1s6hPd9k0VLv3FfZdZNmbyzhiSah_uabfrp7b-_d4A6V3As37kz3386gD9IMTAdwna3AGSQ_FqiCxWnPBVsCLv9WxRZfw6jnK2uajlGF1rgN179X3934dH05GgmZ5-PD7dho9tsjpPXsL5aXtk36CWt9E4jBbdh2Q2_ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Children%27s+overtensing+errors%3A+Phonological+and+lexical+effects+on+syntax&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+memory+and+language&rft.au=Stemberger%2C+Joseph+Paul&rft.date=2007-07-01&rft.issn=0749-596X&rft.volume=57&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=49&rft.epage=64&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.jml.2007.02.003&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon |