Children’s overtensing errors: Phonological and lexical effects on syntax

Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗ didn’t br o ke for target didn’t br ea k) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more co...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inJournal of memory and language Vol. 57; no. 1; pp. 49 - 64
Main Author Stemberger, Joseph Paul
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published San Diego, CA Elsevier Inc 01.07.2007
Elsevier
Elsevier BV
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0749-596X
1096-0821
DOI10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗ didn’t br o ke for target didn’t br ea k) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic.
AbstractList Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic.
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *didn't broke for target didn't break) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *didn't broke for target didn't break) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic. [Copyright 2007 Elsevier Inc.]
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗ didn’t br o ke for target didn’t br ea k) is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when phonological processing biases favour the past-tense form relative to the base form (vowel dominance and the consonant addition bias). For regulars, errors are more common when the inflected form ends in a rime that can occur in monomorphemic forms in English (which children have had independent practice with) than when the rime is found only in inflected forms. Results demonstrate that default patterns can be subject to lexical frequency effects. Results are compatible with a particular conceptualization of competition (the integrated multiple competitor approach), whether connectionist or symbolic.
Author Stemberger, Joseph Paul
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Joseph Paul
  surname: Stemberger
  fullname: Stemberger, Joseph Paul
  email: stemberg@interchange.ubc.ca
  organization: Department of Linguistics, University of British Columbia, E270-1866 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z1
BackLink http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ763653$$DView record in ERIC
http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18782140$$DView record in Pascal Francis
BookMark eNp9kcFqFTEUhoNU8Lb6AIKLQdDdjCeZSTKjq3KpVi3oQsFdyGRO2gy5SU3mlnbna_h6PklT70Whi67Ogf_7f07yH5KDEAMS8pxCQ4GKN3Mzb3zDAGQDrAFoH5EVhUHU0DN6QFYgu6Hmg_jxhBzmPANQyiVbkc_rC-enhOHPr9-5ileYFgzZhfMKU4opv62-XsQQfTx3RvtKh6nyeP13R2vRLMUUqnwTFn39lDy22md8tp9H5Pv7k2_r0_rsy4eP6-Oz2nQglnpqB8EAR9pNg-5kL0dmLTcTcE3RoqSaj50YObNysi1jg-wN73ActaScdbo9Iq93uZcp_txiXtTGZYPe64Bxm1XPhRC97Av48h44x20K5TbFKIeWt6Ir0Ks9pHN5lk06GJfVZXIbnW4ULUGMdlC4FzsOkzP_5JNPUrSCt0WmO9mkmHNC-z8B1F1FalalInVXkQKmSkXFI-95jFv04mJYknb-Qee7_THln68cJpWNw2BwcqmUoqboHnDfAkyrrQg
CODEN JMLAE6
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1075_ml_9_1_05ste
crossref_primary_10_1075_ml_8_3_03ste
crossref_primary_10_2989_16073614_2022_2063146
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11525_024_09434_x
crossref_primary_10_1080_01690965_2013_849812
crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1505
crossref_primary_10_1044_2017_JSLHR_L_16_0402
Cites_doi 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90032-7
10.1017/S0305000900007510
10.1080/01690960244000045
10.1016/0010-0285(74)90018-8
10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01990-3
10.1016/0749-596X(91)90002-2
10.1207/s15516709cog2606_2
10.1037/h0036026
10.1017/S0305000900007297
10.1080/01690969308406948
10.3758/BF03209225
10.1016/0749-596X(89)90042-9
10.1017/S0305000900006875
10.1017/S0305000900007558
10.1207/s15516709cog2103_2
10.1017/S0140525X99002228
10.1017/S030500090000845X
10.2307/415792
10.2307/414099
10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.001
10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661
10.1073/pnas.96.13.7592
10.1016/0010-0285(86)90003-4
10.1017/S0305000900001148
10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01993-9
10.1016/0022-5371(76)90017-7
10.2307/1128013
10.1017/S0305000900001999
10.1016/0010-0277(91)90046-7
10.1016/0010-0277(85)90046-0
10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80021-2
10.1007/BF00133692
10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.001
10.2307/1130007
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2007 Elsevier Inc.
2007 INIST-CNRS
Copyright © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2007 Elsevier Inc.
– notice: 2007 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Copyright © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
IQODW
7T9
8BM
DOI 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003
DatabaseName CrossRef
ERIC
ERIC (Ovid)
ERIC
ERIC
ERIC (Legacy Platform)
ERIC( SilverPlatter )
ERIC
ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)
Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
Pascal-Francis
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
ComDisDome
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ERIC
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
ComDisDome
DatabaseTitleList ERIC
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: ERI
  name: ERIC
  url: https://eric.ed.gov/
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Languages & Literatures
Education
Psychology
EISSN 1096-0821
ERIC EJ763653
EndPage 64
ExternalDocumentID 1274396681
18782140
EJ763653
10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003
S0749596X07000277
Genre Feature
GroupedDBID --K
--M
--Z
-DZ
-~X
.GJ
.~1
07C
0R3
0R~
186
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29L
3EH
4.4
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
7-5
71M
85S
8P~
9JM
9JO
AABNK
AACLR
AACTN
AADFP
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFJI
AAGJA
AAGJQ
AAGUQ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AATGF
AAWTL
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABGZZ
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABMMH
ABOYX
ABPPZ
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACFII
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACIOK
ACMRT
ACNCT
ACREJ
ACRLP
ACXNI
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADFGL
ADIYS
ADMUD
ADPVW
ADSFD
AEBSH
AEBYS
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AETEA
AFFDN
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AFYLN
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJGPC
AJOXV
AKYCK
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
AOMHK
ASPBG
AVARZ
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BKOMP
BLXMC
CAG
COF
CS3
DM4
EBS
ECVKH
EFBJH
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
G8K
GBLVA
HMW
HMY
HVGLF
HYQOX
HZ~
IHE
J1W
K-O
KC5
KOM
LG5
LPU
M3U
M3X
M41
MO0
MOBAO
MVM
N9A
O-F
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OHT
OKEIE
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
PQQKQ
PRBVW
Q38
Q5E
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SDF
SDG
SES
SEW
SPCBC
SPS
SSB
SSN
SSO
SSS
SSY
SSZ
T5K
TN5
UNMZH
WH7
WUQ
XJT
XOL
XPP
XSW
XZL
YK3
YQT
YYQ
ZA5
ZCA
ZCG
ZMT
ZU3
ZXP
~G-
~OB
~OC
~OG
~PJ
~PM
~PN
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDPE
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADMHG
ADNMO
ADVLN
ADXHL
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
PMKZF
PVKVW
SSH
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
EFKBS
ERI
PET
PMFND
REK
WWN
08R
ABPIF
ABPTK
AKALU
IQODW
7T9
8BM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c406t-d39620eb14d9a4787b2ff5cd05a1efe71a5b46b52f7df322978c54ebba71524a3
IEDL.DBID AIKHN
ISSN 0749-596X
IngestDate Mon Jul 21 09:40:25 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 04:42:53 EDT 2025
Sun Oct 22 16:09:28 EDT 2023
Tue Sep 02 19:19:25 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:01:50 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:10:18 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:32:36 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess false
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 1
Keywords Phonology–morphology interactions
Connectionist models
First language acquisition
Dual route models
Morphology
Symbolic models
Language production
Overtensing
Regularity
Frequency effects
English past-tense
Lexicon
Grammatical tense
Cognition
Verbal production
Connectionism
Language
Phonology-morphology interactions
Child
Human
Grammar
English
Language development
Frequency
Phonology
Models
Syntax
Language English
License https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0
CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c406t-d39620eb14d9a4787b2ff5cd05a1efe71a5b46b52f7df322978c54ebba71524a3
Notes SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
PQID 215035364
PQPubID 29955
PageCount 16
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_85666878
proquest_journals_215035364
pascalfrancis_primary_18782140
eric_primary_EJ763653
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_jml_2007_02_003
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2007-07-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2007-07-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 07
  year: 2007
  text: 2007-07-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace San Diego, CA
PublicationPlace_xml – name: San Diego, CA
– name: New York
PublicationTitle Journal of memory and language
PublicationYear 2007
Publisher Elsevier Inc
Elsevier
Elsevier BV
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Inc
– name: Elsevier
– name: Elsevier BV
References MacKay (bib29) 1976; 15
Jaeger (bib24) 2004
Kuczaj (bib27) 1976; 3
McCarthy (bib35) 2005
Stemberger (bib49) 2002; 26
Valian (bib57) 1991; 40
Bates, Bretherton, Snyder (bib2) 1988
Rumelhart, McClelland (bib43) 1986; Vol. 1
Pinker (bib38) 1984
Marcus, Pinker, Ullman, Hollander, Rosen, Xu (bib33) 1992; 57
Hurford (bib23) 1975; 2
Clark (bib11) 1978
McCarthy, J.J., & Wolf, M. (2005). Less than zero: Correspondence and the null output. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-772
Bloom, Hood, Lightbown (bib6) 1974; 6
Pinker, Ullman (bib40) 2002; 6
Stemberger, Middleton (bib54) 2003; 18
Stemberger, MacWhinney (bib52) 1986; 18
Bybee, Slobin (bib9) 1982; 58
Wells (bib59) 1981
.
Gathercole (bib17) 1986; 13
Kuczaj (bib28) 1977; 16
Brown (bib7) 1973
Bernhardt, Stemberger (bib5) 1998
Suppes (bib55) 1974; 29
Joanisse, Seidenberg (bib25) 1999; 96
Stemberger (bib46) 1991; 30
MacWhinney (bib30) 2000
Marchman (bib32) 1997; 21
Stemberger (bib45) 1989; 28
Kiparsky (bib26) 1982
Berko (bib4) 1958; 14
Stemberger (bib47) 1992; 68
Stemberger (bib48) 1993
Fay (bib14) 1978; 5
Gleason (bib18) 1980
Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (in press). The dislike of regular plurals in compounds: Phonological or morphological.
Gordon (bib19) 1985; 21
Stemberger, J.P. (2004). Output-oriented similarity effects in the production of English past tense. Paper presented at the Forty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, Nov. 2004.
Cruttenden (bib12) 1978; 5
Warren-Leubecker, Bohannon (bib58) 1984; 55
Maratsos, Kuczaj (bib31) 1978; 5
Dell (bib13) 1986; 93
Hammond (bib20) 1999
Theakston, Lieven, Pine, Rowland (bib56) 2000
Stemberger, J.P., & Bernhardt, B.H. (2001). U-shaped learning in language development, and restrictions on error correction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, April 2001. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-472
Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Kansas City, Nov. 2002.
Andrews (bib1) 1990; 8
Francis, Kučera (bib15) 1982
Stemberger (bib44) 1982
Pinker, Prince (bib39) 1988; 28
Prasada, Pinker (bib41) 1993; 8
Buck-Gengler, Menn, Healy (bib8) 2004; 90
Henry (bib21) 1995
Stemberger, MacWhinney (bib53) 1986; 14
Garvey, Hogan (bib16) 1973; 44
Hollebrandse, Roeper (bib22) 1996
McClelland, Patterson (bib37) 2002; 6
Clahsen (bib10) 1999; 22
Ramscar, M.J.A. (2002). Learning language from the input: Why innate constraints aren’t implicated in noun-noun compounding. Paper presented at the 43
Marshall, Van der Lely (bib34) 2006; 100
Bybee (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib9) 1982; 58
Gathercole (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib17) 1986; 13
Bloom (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib6) 1974; 6
Suppes (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib55) 1974; 29
Marchman (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib32) 1997; 21
Hurford (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib23) 1975; 2
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib52) 1986; 18
Kuczaj (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib28) 1977; 16
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib44) 1982
Maratsos (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib31) 1978; 5
Hammond (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib20) 1999
Pinker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib40) 2002; 6
Gleason (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib18) 1980
10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib42
Wells (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib59) 1981
Henry (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib21) 1995
Prasada (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib41) 1993; 8
Hollebrandse (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib22) 1996
MacWhinney (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib30) 2000
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib46) 1991; 30
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib47) 1992; 68
Clahsen (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib10) 1999; 22
Marshall (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib34) 2006; 100
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib54) 2003; 18
Buck-Gengler (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib8) 2004; 90
Jaeger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib24) 2004
Fay (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib14) 1978; 5
Pinker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib38) 1984
Rumelhart (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib43) 1986; Vol. 1
Garvey (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib16) 1973; 44
Brown (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib7) 1973
10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib50
10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib51
Gordon (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib19) 1985; 21
Berko (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib4) 1958; 14
McClelland (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib37) 2002; 6
Cruttenden (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib12) 1978; 5
Andrews (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib1) 1990; 8
Pinker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib39) 1988; 28
Warren-Leubecker (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib58) 1984; 55
Dell (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib13) 1986; 93
McCarthy (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib35) 2005
Kuczaj (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib27) 1976; 3
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib45) 1989; 28
Bates (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib2) 1988
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib49) 2002; 26
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib53) 1986; 14
10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib3
Marcus (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib33) 1992; 57
Joanisse (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib25) 1999; 96
Clark (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib11) 1978
Bernhardt (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib5) 1998
Stemberger (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib48) 1993
MacKay (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib29) 1976; 15
10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib36
Valian (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib57) 1991; 40
Theakston (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib56) 2000
Francis (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib15) 1982
Kiparsky (10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib26) 1982
References_xml – volume: 8
  start-page: 507
  year: 1990
  end-page: 557
  ident: bib1
  article-title: Unification and morphological blocking
  publication-title: Natural Language & Linguistic Theory
– year: 1982
  ident: bib15
  article-title: Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar
– volume: 21
  start-page: 283
  year: 1997
  end-page: 304
  ident: bib32
  article-title: Children’s productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure
  publication-title: Cognitive Science
– volume: 28
  start-page: 73
  year: 1988
  end-page: 194
  ident: bib39
  article-title: On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition
  publication-title: Cognition
– reference: McCarthy, J.J., & Wolf, M. (2005). Less than zero: Correspondence and the null output. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-772: <
– reference: Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (in press). The dislike of regular plurals in compounds: Phonological or morphological.
– volume: 8
  start-page: 1
  year: 1993
  end-page: 56
  ident: bib41
  article-title: Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
– volume: 100
  start-page: 302
  year: 2006
  end-page: 320
  ident: bib34
  article-title: A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 3
  start-page: 423
  year: 1976
  end-page: 427
  ident: bib27
  article-title: Arguments against Hurford’s ‘Aux Copying Rule’
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– volume: 6
  start-page: 456
  year: 2002
  end-page: 463
  ident: bib40
  article-title: The past and future of the past tense
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 2
  start-page: 299
  year: 1975
  end-page: 301
  ident: bib23
  article-title: A child and the English question formation rule
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– year: 1999
  ident: bib20
  article-title: The phonology of English: A prosodic optimality-theoretic approach
– volume: 6
  start-page: 465
  year: 2002
  end-page: 472
  ident: bib37
  article-title: Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out?
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 5
  start-page: 337
  year: 1978
  end-page: 345
  ident: bib31
  article-title: Against the transformational account: A simpler analysis of auxiliary overmarkings
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– reference: Stemberger, J.P. (2004). Output-oriented similarity effects in the production of English past tense. Paper presented at the Forty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, Nov. 2004.
– volume: 44
  start-page: 562
  year: 1973
  end-page: 568
  ident: bib16
  article-title: Social speech and social interaction: Egocentrism revisited
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: Vol. 1
  start-page: 216
  year: 1986
  end-page: 271
  ident: bib43
  article-title: On learning the past tenses of English verbs
  publication-title: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition
– start-page: 130
  year: 1982
  end-page: 175
  ident: bib26
  article-title: From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology
  publication-title: The structure of phonological representations, part 1
– start-page: 170
  year: 2005
  end-page: 210
  ident: bib35
  article-title: Optimal paradigms
  publication-title: Paradigms in phonological theory
– reference: Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Kansas City, Nov. 2002.
– volume: 21
  start-page: 73
  year: 1985
  end-page: 93
  ident: bib19
  article-title: Level ordering in lexical development
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 13
  start-page: 537
  year: 1986
  end-page: 560
  ident: bib17
  article-title: The acquisition of the present perfect: explaining differences in the speech of Scottish and American children
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– volume: 15
  start-page: 169
  year: 1976
  end-page: 182
  ident: bib29
  article-title: On the retrieval and lexical structure of verbs
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
– year: 2004
  ident: bib24
  article-title: Kids’ slips: What young children’s slips of the tongue reveal about language development
– volume: 28
  start-page: 164
  year: 1989
  end-page: 188
  ident: bib45
  article-title: Speech errors in early child language production
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 29
  start-page: 103
  year: 1974
  end-page: 114
  ident: bib55
  article-title: The semantics of children’s language
  publication-title: American Psychologist
– volume: 40
  start-page: 21
  year: 1991
  end-page: 81
  ident: bib57
  article-title: Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children
  publication-title: Cognition
– start-page: 261
  year: 1996
  end-page: 271
  ident: bib22
  article-title: The concept of DO-insertion and the theory of INFL
  publication-title: Proceedings of the Groningen Assembly on Language Acquisition
– start-page: 119
  year: 2000
  end-page: 128
  ident: bib56
  article-title: The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of ’mixed’ verb-argument structure at stage 1
  publication-title: New directions in language development and disorders
– volume: 14
  start-page: 150
  year: 1958
  end-page: 177
  ident: bib4
  article-title: The child’s learning of English morphology
  publication-title: Word
– volume: 14
  start-page: 17
  year: 1986
  end-page: 26
  ident: bib53
  article-title: Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms
  publication-title: Memory and Cognition
– reference: .
– volume: 30
  start-page: 161
  year: 1991
  end-page: 185
  ident: bib46
  article-title: Apparent anti-frequency effects in language production: The addition bias and phonological underspecification
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 6
  start-page: 380
  year: 1974
  end-page: 420
  ident: bib6
  article-title: Imitation in language development: If, when and why
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 22
  start-page: 991
  year: 1999
  end-page: 1060
  ident: bib10
  article-title: Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection
  publication-title: Brain & Behavior Science
– volume: 68
  start-page: 492
  year: 1992
  end-page: 524
  ident: bib47
  article-title: Vocalic underspecification in English language production
  publication-title: Language
– volume: 26
  start-page: 737
  year: 2002
  end-page: 766
  ident: bib49
  article-title: Overtensing and the effect of regularity
  publication-title: Cognitive Science
– year: 2000
  ident: bib30
  article-title: The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk
– volume: 55
  start-page: 1379
  year: 1984
  end-page: 1385
  ident: bib58
  article-title: Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother-father speech
  publication-title: Child Development
– year: 1988
  ident: bib2
  article-title: From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms
– reference: Stemberger, J.P., & Bernhardt, B.H. (2001). U-shaped learning in language development, and restrictions on error correction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, April 2001. Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-472: <
– volume: 93
  start-page: 283
  year: 1986
  end-page: 321
  ident: bib13
  article-title: A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– year: 1984
  ident: bib38
  article-title: Language learnability and language development
– year: 1982
  ident: bib44
  article-title: The lexicon in a model of language production. Doctoral Dissertation, UCSD
– volume: 5
  start-page: 143
  year: 1978
  end-page: 149
  ident: bib14
  article-title: Transformations as mental operations: A reply to Kuczaj
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– year: 1995
  ident: bib21
  article-title: Belfast English and standard English: Dialect variation and parameter setting
– volume: 90
  start-page: 453
  year: 2004
  end-page: 464
  ident: bib8
  article-title: What “mice-trap” tells us about the mental lexicon
  publication-title: Brain & Language
– volume: 57
  year: 1992
  ident: bib33
  article-title: Overregularization in language acquisition
  publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
– year: 1981
  ident: bib59
  article-title: Learning through interaction: The study of language development
– year: 1980
  ident: bib18
  article-title: The acquisition of social speech and politeness formulae
  publication-title: Language: Social psychological perspectives
– volume: 58
  start-page: 265
  year: 1982
  end-page: 289
  ident: bib9
  article-title: Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past-tense
  publication-title: Language
– volume: 5
  start-page: 373
  year: 1978
  end-page: 378
  ident: bib12
  article-title: Assimilation in child language and elsewhere
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– volume: 18
  start-page: 329
  year: 1986
  end-page: 354
  ident: bib52
  article-title: Form-oriented errors in inflectional processing
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– start-page: 503
  year: 1993
  end-page: 521
  ident: bib48
  article-title: Vowel dominance in overregularization
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language,20
– year: 1998
  ident: bib5
  article-title: Handbook of phonological development: From the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology
– year: 1978
  ident: bib11
  article-title: Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do
  publication-title: The child’s conception of language
– volume: 16
  start-page: 589
  year: 1977
  end-page: 600
  ident: bib28
  article-title: The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
– year: 1973
  ident: bib7
  article-title: A first language: The early stages
– reference: Ramscar, M.J.A. (2002). Learning language from the input: Why innate constraints aren’t implicated in noun-noun compounding. Paper presented at the 43
– volume: 96
  start-page: 7592
  year: 1999
  end-page: 7597
  ident: bib25
  article-title: Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: A connectionist model
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
– reference: >.
– volume: 18
  start-page: 369
  year: 2003
  end-page: 404
  ident: bib54
  article-title: Vowel dominance and morphological processing
  publication-title: Language & Cognitive Processes
– volume: 57
  issue: whole no. 4
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib33
  article-title: Overregularization in language acquisition
  publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
– year: 1980
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib18
  article-title: The acquisition of social speech and politeness formulae
– year: 1981
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib59
– volume: 28
  start-page: 73
  year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib39
  article-title: On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90032-7
– volume: 5
  start-page: 337
  year: 1978
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib31
  article-title: Against the transformational account: A simpler analysis of auxiliary overmarkings
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900007510
– start-page: 119
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib56
  article-title: The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of ’mixed’ verb-argument structure at stage 1
– start-page: 170
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib35
  article-title: Optimal paradigms
– volume: 18
  start-page: 369
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib54
  article-title: Vowel dominance and morphological processing
  publication-title: Language & Cognitive Processes
  doi: 10.1080/01690960244000045
– volume: 6
  start-page: 380
  year: 1974
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib6
  article-title: Imitation in language development: If, when and why
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90018-8
– year: 1982
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib44
– year: 1973
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib7
– volume: 6
  start-page: 456
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib40
  article-title: The past and future of the past tense
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01990-3
– ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib50
– year: 1978
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib11
  article-title: Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do
– volume: 30
  start-page: 161
  year: 1991
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib46
  article-title: Apparent anti-frequency effects in language production: The addition bias and phonological underspecification
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90002-2
– volume: 26
  start-page: 737
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib49
  article-title: Overtensing and the effect of regularity
  publication-title: Cognitive Science
  doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2606_2
– volume: 29
  start-page: 103
  year: 1974
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib55
  article-title: The semantics of children’s language
  publication-title: American Psychologist
  doi: 10.1037/h0036026
– year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib2
– volume: 3
  start-page: 423
  year: 1976
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib27
  article-title: Arguments against Hurford’s ‘Aux Copying Rule’
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900007297
– volume: 8
  start-page: 1
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib41
  article-title: Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
  doi: 10.1080/01690969308406948
– ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib3
– ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib42
– volume: 14
  start-page: 17
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib53
  article-title: Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms
  publication-title: Memory and Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03209225
– volume: 28
  start-page: 164
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib45
  article-title: Speech errors in early child language production
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90042-9
– volume: 13
  start-page: 537
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib17
  article-title: The acquisition of the present perfect: explaining differences in the speech of Scottish and American children
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900006875
– volume: 5
  start-page: 373
  year: 1978
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib12
  article-title: Assimilation in child language and elsewhere
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900007558
– volume: 21
  start-page: 283
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib32
  article-title: Children’s productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure
  publication-title: Cognitive Science
  doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2103_2
– volume: 22
  start-page: 991
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib10
  article-title: Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection
  publication-title: Brain & Behavior Science
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002228
– start-page: 503
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib48
  article-title: Vowel dominance in overregularization
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language,20
  doi: 10.1017/S030500090000845X
– ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib36
– volume: 68
  start-page: 492
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib47
  article-title: Vocalic underspecification in English language production
  publication-title: Language
  doi: 10.2307/415792
– volume: 58
  start-page: 265
  year: 1982
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib9
  article-title: Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past-tense
  publication-title: Language
  doi: 10.2307/414099
– year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib24
– year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib21
– year: 1984
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib38
– start-page: 261
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib22
  article-title: The concept of DO-insertion and the theory of INFL
– volume: 90
  start-page: 453
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib8
  article-title: What “mice-trap” tells us about the mental lexicon
  publication-title: Brain & Language
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.001
– volume: 93
  start-page: 283
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib13
  article-title: A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
– year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib30
– volume: 14
  start-page: 150
  year: 1958
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib4
  article-title: The child’s learning of English morphology
  publication-title: Word
  doi: 10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661
– year: 1982
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib15
– volume: 96
  start-page: 7592
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib25
  article-title: Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: A connectionist model
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.13.7592
– volume: 18
  start-page: 329
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib52
  article-title: Form-oriented errors in inflectional processing
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90003-4
– start-page: 130
  year: 1982
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib26
  article-title: From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology
– volume: 2
  start-page: 299
  year: 1975
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib23
  article-title: A child and the English question formation rule
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900001148
– volume: Vol. 1
  start-page: 216
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib43
  article-title: On learning the past tenses of English verbs
– volume: 6
  start-page: 465
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib37
  article-title: Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out?
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01993-9
– volume: 15
  start-page: 169
  year: 1976
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib29
  article-title: On the retrieval and lexical structure of verbs
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90017-7
– volume: 44
  start-page: 562
  year: 1973
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib16
  article-title: Social speech and social interaction: Egocentrism revisited
  publication-title: Child Development
  doi: 10.2307/1128013
– volume: 5
  start-page: 143
  year: 1978
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib14
  article-title: Transformations as mental operations: A reply to Kuczaj
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900001999
– volume: 40
  start-page: 21
  year: 1991
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib57
  article-title: Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90046-7
– volume: 21
  start-page: 73
  year: 1985
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib19
  article-title: Level ordering in lexical development
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90046-0
– year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib20
– volume: 16
  start-page: 589
  year: 1977
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib28
  article-title: The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80021-2
– volume: 8
  start-page: 507
  year: 1990
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib1
  article-title: Unification and morphological blocking
  publication-title: Natural Language & Linguistic Theory
  doi: 10.1007/BF00133692
– ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib51
– volume: 100
  start-page: 302
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib34
  article-title: A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.001
– volume: 55
  start-page: 1379
  year: 1984
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib58
  article-title: Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother-father speech
  publication-title: Child Development
  doi: 10.2307/1130007
– year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003_bib5
SSID ssj0011572
Score 1.8775138
Snippet Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. ∗ didn’t br o ke for target didn’t br ea k) is common in early syntax. In a...
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a...
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *didn't broke for target didn't break) is common in early syntax. In a...
SourceID proquest
pascalfrancis
eric
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 49
SubjectTerms Biological and medical sciences
Child
Child development
Child Language
Children
Children & youth
Connectionist models
Developmental psychology
Dual route models
English
English past-tense
Error Analysis
Error Patterns
First language acquisition
Frequency effects
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Grammar
Inflection
Language acquisition
Language Processing
Language production
Linguistics
Memory
Morphemes
Morphology
Negation
Overtensing
Phonemes
Phonetics
Phonological Processing
Phonology
Phonology–morphology interactions
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Regularity
Rhyme
Symbolic models
Syntax
Tense
Word Frequency
Title Children’s overtensing errors: Phonological and lexical effects on syntax
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.003
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ763653
https://www.proquest.com/docview/215035364
https://www.proquest.com/docview/85666878
Volume 57
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NbtQwEB7151IJIVgKLIXFB8QBKWzs2M6GW7VqtbSlQoJKe7Ps2CmtirPabCV6QbwGr8eTMM46QRWiB26RY0fOzHh-7PE3AK-cLIUxGJbYCmNVznKb6MwVSWYwDkuNy3mLtv_hVM7O-NFczDdg2t2FCWmVUfevdXqrrWPLOFJzvLi4GH9C41eIQs5RaENwlW_CNssKiaK9vf_-eHbaHyZQ0dZwCv2TMKA73GzTvC6_XkUgw4Dcmf3LPMVk6HsL3SDtqnXRi7_0d2uUDh_A_ehNkv31hB_ChvODUIg5Jm0M4MlJ3I9syGty0kMoNwPY6VXfzSM4nsY73b9-_GxIyOpsE9v9OXHLZb1s3pGPX2rf6UmivSUBSDM8x4QQUnvS3PiV_rYLZ4cHn6ezJJZZSEq05qvEItFYijqb20IHrB7DqkqUNhWausrlVAvDpRGsym2F6x_jzlJwZ4zO0fhznT2GLV979xSIsDST1tEJBtZcOzaxjNrgkklDA9D9ENKOuqqMGOShFMaV6pLNLhUyJNTGzFXKAnDpEN70QxZrAI67OvOOZeqWFCk0EHcN2w3s7b9_cIRaVwpsH93i958J4A8yDE2HsNcJgIprv1HoRKWZyCQfwsv-LS7acBKjvauvGzVBJ1riN57933z3YKfbYk7pc9haLa_dC_SNVmYEm2-_01FcAaN2N-A3gBgN_A
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Pb9MwFH8a48AkhKCMUQabD4gDUmji2E7DDVWbytZNSGxSb5YdO7Bpc6qmk9gF8TX4evske06coAmxA7fIsSPH9vtn__x7AG-tKLjWGJaYEmNVRjMTqdTmUaoxDou1zVjDtn90LKan7GDO52sw6e7CeFhl0P2tTm-0dSgZhdEcLc7ORl_R-OU8F3NctD64yh7AQ4bi66Xzw88e5-HJZGjLxZlHvnp3tNmAvM4vLwKNoeftTP9lnAIU-vFC1ThyZZvy4i_t3Zik_afwJPiS5FPb3WewZt3Ap2EOkI0BbM3CbmRN3pFZT6BcD2CjV3zXz-FwEm503_z6XROP6Wxg7e4bsctltaw_ki_fK9dpSaKcIZ5G0z8HOAipHKmv3Ur92ITT_b2TyTQKSRaiAm35KjJpLmiMGpuZXHmmHk3Lkhcm5iqxpc0SxTUTmtMyMyVKP0adBWdWa5Wh6WcqfQHrrnL2JRBuklQYm4wxrGbK0rGhifEOmdCJp7kfQtyNriwCA7lPhHEhO6jZucQJ8ZkxMxlTT1s6hPd9k0VLv3FfZdZNmbyzhiSah_uabfrp7b-_d4A6V3As37kz3386gD9IMTAdwna3AGSQ_FqiCxWnPBVsCLv9WxRZfw6jnK2uajlGF1rgN179X3934dH05GgmZ5-PD7dho9tsjpPXsL5aXtk36CWt9E4jBbdh2Q2_
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Children%27s+overtensing+errors%3A+Phonological+and+lexical+effects+on+syntax&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+memory+and+language&rft.au=Stemberger%2C+Joseph+Paul&rft.date=2007-07-01&rft.issn=0749-596X&rft.volume=57&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=49&rft.epage=64&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.jml.2007.02.003&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon