Visual attention modulates mu suppression during biological motion perception in autistic individuals

There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recor...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inThe European journal of neuroscience Vol. 60; no. 11; pp. 6668 - 6685
Main Authors Sotoodeh, Mohammad Saber, Chien, Sarina Hui‐Lin, Hadjikhani, Nouchine
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published France Wiley Subscription Services, Inc 01.12.2024
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recording eye‐tracking and EEGs during BM tasks. Nineteen autistic children (15 boys, mean age = 11.57 ± 4.28 years) and 19 age‐matched neurotypical (NT) children (15 boys, mean age = 11.68 ± 5.22 years) participated in the study. Each participant's eye movement and EEG were simultaneously recorded while watching four BM stimuli (walking, cartwheeling, free‐throwing and underarm throwing) and a scrambled condition. Mu (8–13 Hz) suppression index (SI) for central regions was calculated. Fixation counts and percent of fixation time were calculated as indices of eye movements. EEG results revealed significant mu suppressions in the central region in both groups for all BM actions. Eye‐tracking results showed that NT children had greater fixation counts and a higher percentage of fixation time than autistic children, indicating greater overall visual attention to BM. Notably, correlational analyses for both groups further revealed that individuals' fixation time and fixation counts were negatively correlated with the mu suppression index for all actions, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu SI in the central region. Our findings suggest a critical role of visual attention in interpreting mu suppression during action perception in autism. This study directly explores the link between visual attention and mu suppression by simultaneously recording eye‐tracking and EEGs during BM tasks in autistic and NT children. Significant mu suppressions for BM in the central areas were observed in both groups. NT children had greater fixation time to BM than did autistic children. Notably, individuals' fixation time negatively correlated with mu suppression for BM, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu suppression in the perception of action in autism.
AbstractList There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recording eye‐tracking and EEGs during BM tasks. Nineteen autistic children (15 boys, mean age = 11.57 ± 4.28 years) and 19 age‐matched neurotypical (NT) children (15 boys, mean age = 11.68 ± 5.22 years) participated in the study. Each participant's eye movement and EEG were simultaneously recorded while watching four BM stimuli (walking, cartwheeling, free‐throwing and underarm throwing) and a scrambled condition. Mu (8–13 Hz) suppression index (SI) for central regions was calculated. Fixation counts and percent of fixation time were calculated as indices of eye movements. EEG results revealed significant mu suppressions in the central region in both groups for all BM actions. Eye‐tracking results showed that NT children had greater fixation counts and a higher percentage of fixation time than autistic children, indicating greater overall visual attention to BM. Notably, correlational analyses for both groups further revealed that individuals' fixation time and fixation counts were negatively correlated with the mu suppression index for all actions, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu SI in the central region. Our findings suggest a critical role of visual attention in interpreting mu suppression during action perception in autism. This study directly explores the link between visual attention and mu suppression by simultaneously recording eye‐tracking and EEGs during BM tasks in autistic and NT children. Significant mu suppressions for BM in the central areas were observed in both groups. NT children had greater fixation time to BM than did autistic children. Notably, individuals' fixation time negatively correlated with mu suppression for BM, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu suppression in the perception of action in autism.
There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recording eye-tracking and EEGs during BM tasks. Nineteen autistic children (15 boys, mean age = 11.57 +/- 4.28 years) and 19 age-matched neurotypical (NT) children (15 boys, mean age = 11.68 +/- 5.22 years) participated in the study. Each participant's eye movement and EEG were simultaneously recorded while watching four BM stimuli (walking, cartwheeling, free-throwing and underarm throwing) and a scrambled condition. Mu (8-13 Hz) suppression index (SI) for central regions was calculated. Fixation counts and percent of fixation time were calculated as indices of eye movements. EEG results revealed significant mu suppressions in the central region in both groups for all BM actions. Eye-tracking results showed that NT children had greater fixation counts and a higher percentage of fixation time than autistic children, indicating greater overall visual attention to BM. Notably, correlational analyses for both groups further revealed that individuals' fixation time and fixation counts were negatively correlated with the mu suppression index for all actions, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu SI in the central region. Our findings suggest a critical role of visual attention in interpreting mu suppression during action perception in autism.
There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recording eye‐tracking and EEGs during BM tasks. Nineteen autistic children (15 boys, mean age = 11.57 ± 4.28 years) and 19 age‐matched neurotypical (NT) children (15 boys, mean age = 11.68 ± 5.22 years) participated in the study. Each participant's eye movement and EEG were simultaneously recorded while watching four BM stimuli (walking, cartwheeling, free‐throwing and underarm throwing) and a scrambled condition. Mu (8–13 Hz) suppression index (SI) for central regions was calculated. Fixation counts and percent of fixation time were calculated as indices of eye movements. EEG results revealed significant mu suppressions in the central region in both groups for all BM actions. Eye‐tracking results showed that NT children had greater fixation counts and a higher percentage of fixation time than autistic children, indicating greater overall visual attention to BM. Notably, correlational analyses for both groups further revealed that individuals' fixation time and fixation counts were negatively correlated with the mu suppression index for all actions, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu SI in the central region. Our findings suggest a critical role of visual attention in interpreting mu suppression during action perception in autism.
There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recording eye-tracking and EEGs during BM tasks. Nineteen autistic children (15 boys, mean age = 11.57 ± 4.28 years) and 19 age-matched neurotypical (NT) children (15 boys, mean age = 11.68 ± 5.22 years) participated in the study. Each participant's eye movement and EEG were simultaneously recorded while watching four BM stimuli (walking, cartwheeling, free-throwing and underarm throwing) and a scrambled condition. Mu (8-13 Hz) suppression index (SI) for central regions was calculated. Fixation counts and percent of fixation time were calculated as indices of eye movements. EEG results revealed significant mu suppressions in the central region in both groups for all BM actions. Eye-tracking results showed that NT children had greater fixation counts and a higher percentage of fixation time than autistic children, indicating greater overall visual attention to BM. Notably, correlational analyses for both groups further revealed that individuals' fixation time and fixation counts were negatively correlated with the mu suppression index for all actions, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu SI in the central region. Our findings suggest a critical role of visual attention in interpreting mu suppression during action perception in autism.There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological motions (BM). Here, we directly explored the link between visual attention and brain activity in terms of mu suppression, by simultaneously recording eye-tracking and EEGs during BM tasks. Nineteen autistic children (15 boys, mean age = 11.57 ± 4.28 years) and 19 age-matched neurotypical (NT) children (15 boys, mean age = 11.68 ± 5.22 years) participated in the study. Each participant's eye movement and EEG were simultaneously recorded while watching four BM stimuli (walking, cartwheeling, free-throwing and underarm throwing) and a scrambled condition. Mu (8-13 Hz) suppression index (SI) for central regions was calculated. Fixation counts and percent of fixation time were calculated as indices of eye movements. EEG results revealed significant mu suppressions in the central region in both groups for all BM actions. Eye-tracking results showed that NT children had greater fixation counts and a higher percentage of fixation time than autistic children, indicating greater overall visual attention to BM. Notably, correlational analyses for both groups further revealed that individuals' fixation time and fixation counts were negatively correlated with the mu suppression index for all actions, indicating a strong association between visual attention and mu SI in the central region. Our findings suggest a critical role of visual attention in interpreting mu suppression during action perception in autism.
Author Hadjikhani, Nouchine
Sotoodeh, Mohammad Saber
Chien, Sarina Hui‐Lin
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Mohammad Saber
  orcidid: 0000-0002-7937-8393
  surname: Sotoodeh
  fullname: Sotoodeh, Mohammad Saber
  email: S.Sotoodeh@sussex.ac.uk
  organization: University of Sussex
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Sarina Hui‐Lin
  orcidid: 0000-0001-7588-833X
  surname: Chien
  fullname: Chien, Sarina Hui‐Lin
  email: sarinachien@mail.cmu.edu.tw
  organization: China Medical University
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Nouchine
  orcidid: 0000-0003-4075-3106
  surname: Hadjikhani
  fullname: Hadjikhani, Nouchine
  email: Nouchine.Hadjikhani@mgh.harvard.edu
  organization: University of Gothenburg
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/39537315$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
https://gup.ub.gu.se/publication/343576$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index
BookMark eNp1kctu1TAQhi1URE8LC14ARWIDi7T2cezYS1SVS1XBBhA7y7EnRz5K7OALVd8en0tZVMWbGc18_6_xzBk68cEDQq8JviD1XcLWXxDOJH-GVqTjuJWMixO0wpLRVhD-6xSdpbTFGAvesRfolNZGTwlbIfjpUtFTo3MGn13wzRxsmXSG1MylSWVZIqS0a9gSnd80gwtT2DhTRXPYKxaIBpZ96nyjS3YpO1Nz6_44W93TS_R8rAFeHeM5-vHx-vvV5_b226cvVx9uW0Ml4a3mZC0Mk2AJ6fUohOwltx0RWDO6NnSUHQzailFLQZiQFAYM3BA8rDuquaXnqD34pjtYyqCW6GYd71XQTm3KomppU1QCRTvKel75dwd-ieF3gZTV7JKBadIeQkmK1nkEkQLjir59hG5Dib7-plKUYdpzzir15kiVYQb7b4CHhVfg8gCYGFKKMCrjst7tLkftJkWw2p1U1ZOq_Umr4v0jxYPpU-zR_c5NcP9_UF3ffD0o_gKILrGp
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_3390_electronics14050827
Cites_doi 10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0
10.1093/cercor/bhz011
10.1177/1362361307089520
10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00173-3
10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.068
10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00557.x
10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012
10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045
10.1007/s11055-022-01329-w
10.1177/00315125211016814
10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.004
10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.004
10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.003
10.1007/s10803-006-0275-y
10.1177/1362361320936945
10.1093/brain/awm162
10.3389/fnhum.2016.00364
10.1196/annals.1416.007
10.1126/science.1070311
10.3758/BF03212378
10.1007/s100710050050
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.014
10.1371/journal.pone.0013491
10.1371/journal.pone.0190165
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4133-13.2015
10.1016/j.pneurobio.2021.102128
10.1080/17470910902826820
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.026
10.1038/nrn3475
10.3758/cabn.9.3.237
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.028
10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.019
10.1371/journal.pone.0177457
10.1186/s13229-019-0299-8
10.1523/jneurosci.5120-12.2013
10.1111/psyp.13740
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00985.x
10.1073/pnas.0707021105
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.11.003
10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.005
10.1068/p261539
10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02269.x
10.1177/1362361306062021
10.1037/bul0000031
10.1007/s10803-020-04707-w
10.1093/cercor/bhr156
10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014
10.1016/0006-3223(94)00247-z
10.1371/journal.pone.0088585
10.1093/brain/119.2.593
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02538.x
10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.074
10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061
10.1007/s10803-011-1256-3
10.1098/rsos.160662
10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3
10.1038/nature07868
10.1093/oso/9780198510697.003.0017
10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.005
10.1046/j.1469-8986.2003.00129.x
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.011
10.1523/jneurosci.2668-09.2009
10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.032
10.3758/PBR.16.5.761
10.1162/jocn.2010.21478
10.1016/j.dcn.2012.09.008
10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00725-8
10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.007
10.1111/jir.12565
10.1080/17470919.2011.614003
10.1002/aur.137
10.1371/journal.pone.0044473
10.1007/BF00230027
10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005
10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152
10.1007/s10803-006-0073-6
10.1080/1612197X.2007.9671837
10.1523/jneurosci.3340-04.2004
10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154
10.1038/scientificamerican1106-62
10.1186/s13229-022-00512-7
10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00608.x
10.1111/cdev.12473
10.1007/s00221-015-4371-0
10.1177/1073191112446655
10.1093/brain/awh404
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112
10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2
10.1111/jir.12623
10.1097/00001756-200111160-00024
10.1038/35090060
10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0
10.3389/fncom.2015.00079
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.033
10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2024 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
2024 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Copyright_xml – notice: 2024 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
– notice: 2024 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7QR
7TK
8FD
FR3
P64
7X8
ADTPV
AOWAS
F1U
DOI 10.1111/ejn.16596
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Engineering Research Database
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
SwePub
SwePub Articles
SWEPUB Göteborgs universitet
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Chemoreception Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Technology Research Database
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList

CrossRef
Chemoreception Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
Chemistry
EISSN 1460-9568
EndPage 6685
ExternalDocumentID oai_gup_ub_gu_se_343576
39537315
10_1111_ejn_16596
EJN16596
Genre article
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Taiwan Ministry of Science and Technology Council
  funderid: MOST111‐2410‐H‐039‐007
– fundername: Taiwan Ministry of Science and Technology Council
  grantid: MOST111-2410-H-039-007
GroupedDBID ---
-~X
.3N
.GA
.GJ
.Y3
05W
0R~
10A
1OB
1OC
29G
31~
33P
36B
3SF
4.4
50Y
50Z
51W
51X
52M
52N
52O
52P
52R
52S
52T
52U
52V
52W
52X
53G
5GY
5HH
5LA
5RE
5VS
66C
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
8UM
930
A01
A03
AAESR
AAEVG
AAHHS
AAHQN
AAIPD
AAMNL
AANHP
AANLZ
AAONW
AASGY
AAXRX
AAYCA
AAZKR
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABDBF
ABEML
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABPVW
ABQWH
ABXGK
ACAHQ
ACBWZ
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFS
ACGOF
ACIWK
ACMXC
ACPOU
ACPRK
ACRPL
ACSCC
ACUHS
ACXBN
ACXQS
ACYXJ
ADBBV
ADBTR
ADEOM
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADNMO
ADOZA
ADXAS
ADZMN
ADZOD
AEEZP
AEIGN
AEIMD
AENEX
AEQDE
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFEBI
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFPWT
AFWVQ
AFZJQ
AHBTC
AHEFC
AIACR
AITYG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
ALAGY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
ALVPJ
AMBMR
AMYDB
ASPBG
ATUGU
AVWKF
AZBYB
AZFZN
AZVAB
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BHBCM
BMXJE
BROTX
BRXPI
BY8
C45
CAG
COF
CS3
D-6
D-7
D-E
D-F
DC6
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRMAN
DRSTM
EAD
EAP
EAS
EBC
EBD
EBS
EBX
EJD
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPS
ESX
EX3
F00
F01
F04
F5P
FEDTE
FUBAC
FZ0
G-S
G.N
GAKWD
GODZA
H.X
HF~
HGLYW
HVGLF
HZI
HZ~
IHE
IX1
J0M
K48
KBYEO
LATKE
LC2
LC3
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LW6
LYRES
MEWTI
MK4
MRFUL
MRMAN
MRSTM
MSFUL
MSMAN
MSSTM
MXFUL
MXMAN
MXSTM
N04
N05
N9A
NF~
O66
O9-
OIG
OVD
P2P
P2W
P2X
P2Z
P4B
P4D
PALCI
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
Q~Q
R.K
RIG
RIWAO
RJQFR
ROL
RX1
SAMSI
SUPJJ
SV3
TEORI
TUS
UB1
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WHG
WIH
WIJ
WIK
WNSPC
WOHZO
WOW
WQJ
WRC
WUP
WXI
WXSBR
WYISQ
XG1
YFH
ZGI
ZZTAW
~IA
~WT
AAYXX
AEYWJ
AGHNM
AGQPQ
AGYGG
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7QR
7TK
8FD
AAMMB
AEFGJ
AGXDD
AIDQK
AIDYY
FR3
P64
7X8
ADTPV
AOWAS
F1U
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c3916-a6128c59ed117af889796d4180a532c3f94ebad8fa9815893eb0e6c10b243a6d3
IEDL.DBID DR2
ISSN 0953-816X
1460-9568
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 06:41:43 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 10:16:16 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 22:41:30 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 06:58:55 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 02:59:52 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:07:16 EDT 2025
Wed Jan 22 17:14:24 EST 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 11
Keywords mu suppression
point‐light display
autism spectrum disorder
biological motion perception
eye‐tracking
Language English
License 2024 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c3916-a6128c59ed117af889796d4180a532c3f94ebad8fa9815893eb0e6c10b243a6d3
Notes Edited by: John Foxe
ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0003-4075-3106
0000-0002-7937-8393
0000-0001-7588-833X
PMID 39537315
PQID 3135037665
PQPubID 34057
PageCount 18
ParticipantIDs swepub_primary_oai_gup_ub_gu_se_343576
proquest_miscellaneous_3128819800
proquest_journals_3135037665
pubmed_primary_39537315
crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_ejn_16596
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16596
wiley_primary_10_1111_ejn_16596_EJN16596
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate December 2024
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-12-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 12
  year: 2024
  text: December 2024
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace France
PublicationPlace_xml – name: France
– name: Chichester
PublicationTitle The European journal of neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate Eur J Neurosci
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
References 2015; 35
2009; 47
2009; 41
2013; 3
2017; 4
2007; 183
1995; 38
2009; 80
2000; 3
2019; 10
2021; 205
2002; 113
2021; 128
1973; 14
2006; 36
2004a; 41
2004; 24
2006; 295
2012; 19
2022; 26
2003; 17
2008; 105
2011; 14
2012; 59
2016; 142
2009; 49
1993; 4
2012; 53
2016; 79
2005; 23
2007; 37
2005; 24
2004; 134
1992; 91
2013; 14
2019; 63
2007; 130
2016; 87
2007; 8
2019; 29
2016; 82
2007; 5
2011; 23
2014; 19
2010; 3
2014; 9
1998; 95
2001; 12
2010; 5
2012; 22
2009; 16
2009; 1304
2011; 2
2013; 49
2013; 43
2002; 297
2006; 10
1997; 26
2015; 11
2016; 10
2008; 12
2014; 47
2008; 11
2002
2009; 459
2015; 9
2021; 51
2007; 58
2014; 44
2009; 29
2012; 50
2021; 58
2004b; 115
2011; 1380
2013; 37
2008; 1145
2013; 33
2015; 233
2005; 128
2017; 12
2022; 13
2009; 9
2022; 52
2020; 24
2001; 2
2005; 3
2013
2009; 4
2012; 7
1996; 119
2012; 42
2010; 51
2010; 50
2001; 31
2018; 13
e_1_2_10_21_1
e_1_2_10_44_1
e_1_2_10_40_1
e_1_2_10_70_1
e_1_2_10_93_1
e_1_2_10_2_1
e_1_2_10_18_1
e_1_2_10_74_1
e_1_2_10_97_1
e_1_2_10_6_1
e_1_2_10_55_1
e_1_2_10_14_1
e_1_2_10_37_1
e_1_2_10_78_1
e_1_2_10_13_1
e_1_2_10_32_1
e_1_2_10_51_1
e_1_2_10_82_1
e_1_2_10_29_1
e_1_2_10_63_1
e_1_2_10_86_1
e_1_2_10_25_1
e_1_2_10_48_1
e_1_2_10_67_1
e_1_2_10_45_1
e_1_2_10_22_1
e_1_2_10_41_1
Association, A. P (e_1_2_10_4_1) 2013
e_1_2_10_90_1
e_1_2_10_71_1
e_1_2_10_94_1
e_1_2_10_52_1
e_1_2_10_3_1
e_1_2_10_19_1
e_1_2_10_75_1
e_1_2_10_38_1
e_1_2_10_98_1
e_1_2_10_56_1
e_1_2_10_79_1
e_1_2_10_7_1
e_1_2_10_15_1
e_1_2_10_10_1
e_1_2_10_33_1
e_1_2_10_60_1
e_1_2_10_83_1
e_1_2_10_64_1
e_1_2_10_49_1
e_1_2_10_87_1
e_1_2_10_26_1
e_1_2_10_68_1
e_1_2_10_23_1
e_1_2_10_46_1
e_1_2_10_69_1
e_1_2_10_42_1
e_1_2_10_91_1
e_1_2_10_72_1
e_1_2_10_95_1
e_1_2_10_53_1
e_1_2_10_16_1
e_1_2_10_39_1
e_1_2_10_76_1
e_1_2_10_99_1
e_1_2_10_8_1
e_1_2_10_57_1
e_1_2_10_58_1
e_1_2_10_34_1
e_1_2_10_11_1
e_1_2_10_30_1
e_1_2_10_80_1
e_1_2_10_61_1
e_1_2_10_84_1
e_1_2_10_27_1
e_1_2_10_65_1
e_1_2_10_88_1
e_1_2_10_24_1
e_1_2_10_43_1
e_1_2_10_20_1
e_1_2_10_92_1
e_1_2_10_73_1
e_1_2_10_96_1
e_1_2_10_54_1
e_1_2_10_5_1
e_1_2_10_17_1
e_1_2_10_77_1
e_1_2_10_36_1
e_1_2_10_12_1
e_1_2_10_35_1
e_1_2_10_9_1
e_1_2_10_59_1
e_1_2_10_31_1
e_1_2_10_50_1
e_1_2_10_81_1
e_1_2_10_62_1
e_1_2_10_85_1
e_1_2_10_28_1
e_1_2_10_66_1
e_1_2_10_100_1
e_1_2_10_47_1
e_1_2_10_89_1
References_xml – volume: 49
  start-page: 2705
  issue: 22
  year: 2009
  end-page: 2739
  article-title: Vision in autism spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Vision Research
– volume: 63
  start-page: 72
  issue: 1
  year: 2019
  end-page: 84
  article-title: Perception of biological motions is preserved in people with autism spectrum disorder: Electrophysiological and behavioural evidences
  publication-title: Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
– volume: 41
  start-page: 152
  issue: 1
  year: 2004a
  end-page: 156
  article-title: Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip [article]
  publication-title: Psychophysiology
– volume: 1145
  start-page: 283
  issue: 1
  year: 2008
  end-page: 299
  article-title: Brain mechanisms for social perception
  publication-title: Annals of the new York Academy of Sciences
– volume: 33
  start-page: 6776
  issue: 16
  year: 2013
  end-page: 6781
  article-title: Tunnel vision: Sharper gradient of spatial attention in autism
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 42
  start-page: 401
  issue: 3
  year: 2012
  end-page: 408
  article-title: Young children with autism Spectrum disorder do not preferentially attend to biological motion
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 23
  start-page: 125
  issue: 2
  year: 2005
  end-page: 141
  article-title: Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area
  publication-title: International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience
– volume: 12
  start-page: 225
  issue: 6
  year: 2008
  end-page: 229
  article-title: Unbroken mirrors: Challenging a theory of autism
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 79
  start-page: 657
  issue: 8
  year: 2016
  end-page: 666
  article-title: Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric images as an early biomarker of an autism Spectrum disorder subtype associated with increased symptom severity
  publication-title: Biological Psychiatry
– volume: 7
  start-page: 301
  issue: 3
  year: 2012
  end-page: 310
  article-title: Socially tuned: Brain responses differentiating human and animal motion
  publication-title: Social Neuroscience
– volume: 52
  start-page: 999
  issue: 7
  year: 2022
  end-page: 1016
  article-title: The mu rhythm in current research: Theoretical and methodological aspects
  publication-title: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology
– volume: 295
  start-page: 62
  issue: 5
  year: 2006
  end-page: 69
  article-title: Broken mirrors: A theory of autism
  publication-title: Scientific American
– volume: 23
  start-page: 1379
  issue: 6
  year: 2011
  end-page: 1394
  article-title: From Prestimulus alpha oscillation to visual‐evoked response: An inverted‐U function and its attentional modulation
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 1304
  start-page: 113
  year: 2009
  end-page: 121
  article-title: EEG study of the mirror neuron system in children with high functioning autism
  publication-title: Brain Research
– volume: 14
  start-page: 353
  issue: 2
  year: 2011
  end-page: 359
  article-title: Biological motion preference in humans at birth: Role of dynamic and configural properties
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1816
  issue: 4
  year: 2019
  end-page: 1833
  article-title: Anterior intraparietal area: A hub in the observed manipulative action network
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
– volume: 31
  start-page: 155
  issue: 1
  year: 2001
  end-page: 165
  article-title: I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study [article]
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 10
  start-page: 49
  year: 2019
  article-title: Biological motion perception in autism spectrum disorder: A meta‐analysis
  publication-title: Molecular Autism
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1849
  issue: 5
  year: 2015
  end-page: 1857
  article-title: Action perception is intact in autism spectrum disorder
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 12
  start-page: 3493
  issue: 16
  year: 2001
  end-page: 3495
  article-title: Stronger reactivity of the human primary motor cortex during observation of live rather than video motor acts [article]
  publication-title: Neuroreport
– volume: 205
  year: 2021
  article-title: What modulates the Mirror neuron system during action observation?: Multiple factors involving the action, the actor, the observer, the relationship between actor and observer, and the context
  publication-title: Progress in Neurobiology
– volume: 11
  start-page: 63
  year: 2015
  end-page: 76
  article-title: Children with autism spectrum disorders are less proficient in action identification and lacking a preference for upright point‐light biological motion displays
  publication-title: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
– volume: 19
  start-page: 354
  issue: 3
  year: 2012
  end-page: 369
  article-title: Development of abbreviated nine‐item forms of the Raven's standard progressive matrices test
  publication-title: Assessment
– volume: 233
  start-page: 3013
  issue: 10
  year: 2015
  end-page: 3022
  article-title: What you see is what you get: Motor resonance in peripheral vision
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
– volume: 47
  start-page: 559
  year: 2014
  end-page: 577
  article-title: Impairments to visual disengagement in autism spectrum disorder: A review of experimental studies from infancy to adulthood
  publication-title: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
– volume: 2
  year: 2011
  article-title: The role of alpha‐band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention [review]
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
– volume: 130
  start-page: 2452
  issue: 9
  year: 2007
  end-page: 2461
  article-title: Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception
  publication-title: Brain
– volume: 36
  start-page: 343
  year: 2006
  end-page: 350
  article-title: The autism‐spectrum quotient (AQ)—adolescent version
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 115
  start-page: 1760
  issue: 8
  year: 2004b
  end-page: 1766
  article-title: Primary motor cortex activation during action observation revealed by wavelet analysis of the EEG
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 24
  start-page: 190
  issue: 2
  year: 2005
  end-page: 198
  article-title: EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Cognitive Brain Research
– volume: 3
  issue: 7
  year: 2005
  article-title: Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns
  publication-title: PLoS Biology
– volume: 95
  start-page: 15061
  issue: 25
  year: 1998
  end-page: 15065
  article-title: Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: A neuromagnetic study [article]
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
– volume: 183
  start-page: 188
  issue: 2
  year: 2007
  end-page: 194
  article-title: Recognition of point‐light biological motion: Mu rhythms and mirror neuron activity
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
– volume: 24
  start-page: 9228
  issue: 42
  year: 2004
  end-page: 9231
  article-title: Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 22
  start-page: 981
  issue: 5
  year: 2012
  end-page: 995
  article-title: Biological motion processing as a Hallmark of social cognition
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
– volume: 4
  issue: 3
  year: 2017
  article-title: The interpretation of mu suppression as an index of mirror neuron activity: Past, present and future
  publication-title: Royal Society Open Science
– volume: 4
  start-page: 54
  issue: 1
  year: 1993
  end-page: 57
  article-title: Cats perceive biological motion
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 43
  start-page: 323
  issue: 2
  year: 2013
  end-page: 344
  article-title: Motor abilities in autism: A review using a computational context
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 80
  start-page: 306
  issue: 3
  year: 2009
  end-page: 314
  article-title: Mirroring and mu rhythm involvement in social cognition: Are there dissociable subcomponents of theory of mind?
  publication-title: Biological Psychology
– year: 2002
– volume: 82
  start-page: 290
  year: 2016
  end-page: 310
  article-title: Mu suppression – A good measure of the human mirror neuron system?
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 113
  start-page: 16
  issue: 1
  year: 2002
  end-page: 24
  article-title: Somatosensory processing during movement observation in humans [article]
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 16
  start-page: 761
  issue: 5
  year: 2009
  end-page: 777
  article-title: The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A review and synthesis
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
– volume: 37
  start-page: 164
  issue: 2
  year: 2013
  end-page: 183
  article-title: Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism
  publication-title: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
– volume: 7
  issue: 9
  year: 2012
  article-title: Recognizing biological motion and emotions from point‐light displays in autism Spectrum disorders
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 58
  start-page: 47
  issue: 1
  year: 2007
  end-page: 73
  article-title: Perception of human motion
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
– volume: 12
  issue: 5
  year: 2017
  article-title: The role of attention in human motor resonance
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 49
  start-page: 1025
  issue: 4
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1033
  article-title: Zoom‐out attentional impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 1380
  start-page: 246
  year: 2011
  end-page: 254
  article-title: Limited activity monitoring in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder
  publication-title: Brain Research
– volume: 14
  start-page: 365
  issue: 5
  year: 2013
  end-page: 376
  article-title: Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
– volume: 24
  start-page: 1945
  issue: 8
  year: 2020
  end-page: 1959
  article-title: Continuing to look in the mirror: A review of neuroscientific evidence for the broken mirror hypothesis, EP‐M model and STORM model of autism spectrum conditions
  publication-title: Autism
– volume: 17
  start-page: 599
  issue: 3
  year: 2003
  end-page: 611
  article-title: Neuromagnetic imaging of cortical oscillations accompanying tactile stimulation [article]
  publication-title: Cognitive Brain Research
– year: 2013
– volume: 43
  start-page: 1140
  issue: 5
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1150
  article-title: From action to interaction: Exploring the Contribution of body motion cues to social understanding in typical development and in autism Spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 142
  start-page: 291
  issue: 3
  year: 2016
  end-page: 313
  article-title: Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta‐analysis
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
– volume: 38
  start-page: 150
  issue: 3
  year: 1995
  end-page: 165
  article-title: Auditory event‐related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups
  publication-title: Biological Psychiatry
– volume: 12
  start-page: 261
  issue: 3
  year: 2008
  end-page: 274
  article-title: Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Autism
– volume: 297
  start-page: 846
  issue: 5582
  year: 2002
  end-page: 848
  article-title: Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons [article]
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 128
  start-page: 1038
  issue: 5
  year: 2005
  end-page: 1048
  article-title: Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism
  publication-title: Brain
– volume: 26
  start-page: 767
  issue: 9
  year: 2022
  end-page: 781
  article-title: Mirror neurons 30 years later: Implications and applications
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 50
  start-page: 1148
  issue: 3
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1167
  article-title: ALE meta‐analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 3
  start-page: 191
  issue: 4
  year: 2010
  end-page: 195
  article-title: Comparison of visual sensitivity to human and object motion in autism spectrum disorder
  publication-title: Autism Research
– volume: 14
  start-page: 201
  issue: 2
  year: 1973
  end-page: 211
  article-title: Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
– volume: 58
  issue: 3
  year: 2021
  article-title: Preserved action recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: Evidence from an EEG and eye‐tracking study
  publication-title: Psychophysiology
– volume: 50
  start-page: 3207
  issue: 14
  year: 2012
  end-page: 3217
  article-title: Physical experience leads to enhanced object perception in parietal cortex: Insights from knot tying
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 11
  start-page: 40
  issue: 1
  year: 2008
  end-page: 46
  article-title: Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15‐month‐old infant with autism
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 5
  issue: 10
  year: 2010
  article-title: Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non‐biological form‐from‐motion perception in autism Spectrum conditions
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 91
  start-page: 176
  issue: 1
  year: 1992
  end-page: 180
  article-title: Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study [article]
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1149
  issue: 4
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1160
  article-title: Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
– volume: 59
  start-page: 1524
  issue: 2
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1533
  article-title: Do distinct atypical cortical networks process biological motion information in adults with autism spectrum disorders?
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 9
  issue: 2
  year: 2014
  article-title: Visual processing of biological motion in children and adolescents with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An event related potential‐study
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 13
  start-page: 33
  issue: 1
  year: 2022
  article-title: Attentional influences on neural processing of biological motion in typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum
  publication-title: Molecular Autism
– volume: 9
  start-page: 237
  issue: 3
  year: 2009
  end-page: 241
  article-title: Oxytocin enhances the perception of biological motion in humans
  publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
– volume: 87
  start-page: 543
  issue: 2
  year: 2016
  end-page: 557
  article-title: Multilevel differences in spontaneous social attention in toddlers with autism Spectrum disorder
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 10
  start-page: 155
  issue: 2
  year: 2006
  end-page: 172
  article-title: Sensory and attention abnormalities in autistic spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Autism
– volume: 9
  year: 2015
  article-title: Embodied learning of a generative neural model for biological motion perception and inference [original research]
  publication-title: Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience
– volume: 4
  start-page: 332
  issue: 4
  year: 2009
  end-page: 346
  article-title: The visual perception of human and animal motion in point‐light displays
  publication-title: Social Neuroscience
– volume: 51
  start-page: 2369
  issue: 7
  year: 2021
  end-page: 2380
  article-title: Visual preference for biological motion in children and adults with autism Spectrum disorder: An eye‐tracking study
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 2
  start-page: 661
  issue: 9
  year: 2001
  end-page: 670
  article-title: Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action [article]
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
– volume: 8
  start-page: 159
  issue: 3
  year: 2007
  end-page: 166
  article-title: Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system
  publication-title: Cognitive Processing
– volume: 134
  start-page: 9
  issue: 1
  year: 2004
  end-page: 21
  article-title: EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single‐trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods
– volume: 13
  issue: 1
  year: 2018
  article-title: Directing visual attention during action observation modulates corticospinal excitability
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 29
  start-page: 10153
  issue: 32
  year: 2009
  end-page: 10159
  article-title: Evidence of Mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 459
  start-page: 257
  issue: 7244
  year: 2009
  end-page: 261
  article-title: Two‐year‐olds with autism orient to non‐social contingencies rather than biological motion
  publication-title: Nature
– volume: 63
  start-page: 1111
  issue: 9
  year: 2019
  end-page: 1124
  article-title: Adults with autism are less proficient in identifying biological motion actions portrayed with point‐light displays
  publication-title: Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
– volume: 47
  start-page: 3225
  issue: 14
  year: 2009
  end-page: 3235
  article-title: No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 44
  start-page: 431
  issue: 2
  year: 2014
  end-page: 442
  article-title: Gaze response to dyadic bids at 2 years related to outcomes at 3 years in autism spectrum disorders: A subtyping analysis
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 105
  start-page: 809
  issue: 2
  year: 2008
  end-page: 813
  article-title: A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
– volume: 119
  start-page: 593
  issue: 2
  year: 1996
  end-page: 609
  article-title: Action recognition in the premotor cortex [article]
  publication-title: Brain
– volume: 37
  start-page: 1386
  issue: 7
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1392
  article-title: Brief report: Recognition of emotional and non‐emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic Spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
– volume: 3
  start-page: 91
  year: 2013
  end-page: 105
  article-title: Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories
  publication-title: Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 5
  start-page: 291
  issue: 3
  year: 2007
  end-page: 303
  article-title: The effects of attentional focusing strategies on novice dart throwing performance and their task experiences
  publication-title: International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology
– volume: 19
  start-page: 164
  issue: 2
  year: 2014
  end-page: 177
  article-title: Edinburgh handedness inventory – Short form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor analysis
  publication-title: Laterality
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1539
  issue: 12
  year: 1997
  end-page: 1548
  article-title: Perception of biological motion
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 51
  start-page: 981
  issue: 9
  year: 2010
  end-page: 988
  article-title: Unbroken mirror neurons in autism spectrum disorders
  publication-title: Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
– volume: 128
  start-page: 1485
  issue: 4
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1503
  article-title: A point‐light display model for teaching motor skills to children with autism Spectrum disorder: An eye‐tracking study
  publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills
– volume: 3
  start-page: 53
  issue: 1
  year: 2000
  end-page: 60
  article-title: Visual perception of biological motion in newly hatched chicks as revealed by an imprinting procedure
  publication-title: Animal Cognition
– volume: 53
  start-page: 903
  issue: 8
  year: 2012
  end-page: 913
  article-title: Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism
  publication-title: Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
– volume: 10
  year: 2016
  article-title: Amplitude of sensorimotor mu rhythm is correlated with BOLD from multiple brain regions: A simultaneous EEG‐fMRI study
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– ident: e_1_2_10_28_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0
– ident: e_1_2_10_52_1
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz011
– ident: e_1_2_10_63_1
  doi: 10.1177/1362361307089520
– ident: e_1_2_10_18_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00173-3
– ident: e_1_2_10_74_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.068
– ident: e_1_2_10_9_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00557.x
– ident: e_1_2_10_84_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012
– ident: e_1_2_10_96_1
  doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045
– ident: e_1_2_10_53_1
  doi: 10.1007/s11055-022-01329-w
– ident: e_1_2_10_88_1
  doi: 10.1177/00315125211016814
– ident: e_1_2_10_60_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.004
– ident: e_1_2_10_97_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.004
– ident: e_1_2_10_11_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.003
– ident: e_1_2_10_34_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0275-y
– ident: e_1_2_10_99_1
  doi: 10.1177/1362361320936945
– volume-title: Primary care version
  year: 2013
  ident: e_1_2_10_4_1
– ident: e_1_2_10_81_1
  doi: 10.1093/brain/awm162
– ident: e_1_2_10_100_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00364
– ident: e_1_2_10_66_1
  doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.007
– ident: e_1_2_10_50_1
  doi: 10.1126/science.1070311
– ident: e_1_2_10_36_1
  doi: 10.3758/BF03212378
– ident: e_1_2_10_75_1
  doi: 10.1007/s100710050050
– ident: e_1_2_10_41_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.014
– ident: e_1_2_10_82_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013491
– ident: e_1_2_10_98_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190165
– ident: e_1_2_10_20_1
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4133-13.2015
– ident: e_1_2_10_42_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2021.102128
– ident: e_1_2_10_70_1
  doi: 10.1080/17470910902826820
– ident: e_1_2_10_58_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.026
– ident: e_1_2_10_12_1
  doi: 10.1038/nrn3475
– ident: e_1_2_10_44_1
  doi: 10.3758/cabn.9.3.237
– ident: e_1_2_10_19_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.028
– ident: e_1_2_10_31_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.019
– ident: e_1_2_10_71_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177457
– ident: e_1_2_10_92_1
  doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0299-8
– ident: e_1_2_10_77_1
  doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5120-12.2013
– ident: e_1_2_10_89_1
  doi: 10.1111/psyp.13740
– ident: e_1_2_10_5_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00985.x
– ident: e_1_2_10_86_1
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707021105
– ident: e_1_2_10_69_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.11.003
– ident: e_1_2_10_91_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.005
– ident: e_1_2_10_2_1
  doi: 10.1068/p261539
– ident: e_1_2_10_23_1
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
– ident: e_1_2_10_22_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02269.x
– ident: e_1_2_10_55_1
  doi: 10.1177/1362361306062021
– ident: e_1_2_10_24_1
  doi: 10.1037/bul0000031
– ident: e_1_2_10_40_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04707-w
– ident: e_1_2_10_64_1
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr156
– ident: e_1_2_10_62_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014
– ident: e_1_2_10_43_1
  doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00247-z
– ident: e_1_2_10_51_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088585
– ident: e_1_2_10_26_1
  doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593
– ident: e_1_2_10_16_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02538.x
– ident: e_1_2_10_85_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.074
– ident: e_1_2_10_30_1
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061
– ident: e_1_2_10_3_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1256-3
– ident: e_1_2_10_32_1
  doi: 10.1098/rsos.160662
– ident: e_1_2_10_94_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3
– ident: e_1_2_10_48_1
  doi: 10.1038/nature07868
– ident: e_1_2_10_27_1
  doi: 10.1093/oso/9780198510697.003.0017
– ident: e_1_2_10_78_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.005
– ident: e_1_2_10_59_1
  doi: 10.1046/j.1469-8986.2003.00129.x
– ident: e_1_2_10_80_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.011
– ident: e_1_2_10_46_1
  doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2668-09.2009
– ident: e_1_2_10_68_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.032
– ident: e_1_2_10_38_1
  doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.5.761
– ident: e_1_2_10_72_1
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21478
– ident: e_1_2_10_29_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.09.008
– ident: e_1_2_10_79_1
  doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00725-8
– ident: e_1_2_10_93_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.007
– ident: e_1_2_10_90_1
  doi: 10.1111/jir.12565
– ident: e_1_2_10_39_1
  doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.614003
– ident: e_1_2_10_37_1
  doi: 10.1002/aur.137
– ident: e_1_2_10_61_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044473
– ident: e_1_2_10_65_1
  doi: 10.1007/BF00230027
– ident: e_1_2_10_87_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005
– ident: e_1_2_10_10_1
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152
– ident: e_1_2_10_6_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0073-6
– ident: e_1_2_10_56_1
  doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2007.9671837
– ident: e_1_2_10_7_1
  doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3340-04.2004
– ident: e_1_2_10_25_1
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154
– ident: e_1_2_10_73_1
  doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1106-62
– ident: e_1_2_10_49_1
  doi: 10.1186/s13229-022-00512-7
– ident: e_1_2_10_13_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9
– ident: e_1_2_10_47_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00608.x
– ident: e_1_2_10_17_1
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12473
– ident: e_1_2_10_54_1
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4371-0
– ident: e_1_2_10_8_1
  doi: 10.1177/1073191112446655
– ident: e_1_2_10_67_1
  doi: 10.1093/brain/awh404
– ident: e_1_2_10_14_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112
– ident: e_1_2_10_45_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2
– ident: e_1_2_10_33_1
  doi: 10.1111/jir.12623
– ident: e_1_2_10_35_1
  doi: 10.1097/00001756-200111160-00024
– ident: e_1_2_10_76_1
  doi: 10.1038/35090060
– ident: e_1_2_10_15_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0
– ident: e_1_2_10_83_1
  doi: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00079
– ident: e_1_2_10_57_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.033
– ident: e_1_2_10_95_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
– ident: e_1_2_10_21_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
SSID ssj0008645
Score 2.470411
Snippet There has been a lot of controversy regarding mirror neuron function in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in particular during the observation of biological...
SourceID swepub
proquest
pubmed
crossref
wiley
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 6668
SubjectTerms Adolescent
Attention
Attention - physiology
Autism
autism spectrum disorder
Autism Spectrum Disorder - physiopathology
Autistic children
Autistic Disorder - physiopathology
Autistic Disorder - psychology
biological motion perception
Child
Children
EEG
Electroencephalography - methods
Eye
Eye movements
Eye Movements - physiology
eye-tracking
Eye-Tracking Technology
Female
Humans
Male
Motion detection
Motion Perception - physiology
mu suppression
Neurosciences
Neurovetenskaper
Photic Stimulation - methods
point-light display
Temporal perception
Visual perception
Visual Perception - physiology
Title Visual attention modulates mu suppression during biological motion perception in autistic individuals
URI https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fejn.16596
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/39537315
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3135037665
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3128819800
https://gup.ub.gu.se/publication/343576
Volume 60
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9QwEB5VvcCFR8tjoSCDUNVLVnEcO7Y4VVWrqhI9IIr2gGTZjlMtsNkV2Rzg1zO2k0B5SIhbFI8Vx5mxPzufvwF4Vdgqd6KkmcGhLisN95kyCORKL6zB4CpzH_Yh31yK86vyYsEXO_B6PAuT9CGmDbcQGXG8DgFubPdTkPuP7ZwKroLcduBqBUD09od0lBQxQXGQU8skFYtBVSiweKaaN-ei3wDmpB56E7jGmefsLnwY25wIJ5_m_dbO3bdf5Bz_86XuwZ0BkZLj5EL3Yce3e7B_3OJqfPWVHJLIEY2b73tw62TMD7cP_v2y67FiEOiMlEmyWtchGZjvyKonXb8ZOLYtSWchSRJ8Cl5BUvIgsploNWTZEoNBEGSjyXI6JdY9gKuz03cn59mQtCFz4QxvZhAySceVrymtTCOlqpSoSypzw1nhWKNKb00tG6Mk5YiWvM29cDS3RcmMqNlD2G3XrX8MRDZOCc9owy3HssbkhbGFqnLvalk7P4Oj8fNpNyiah8Qan_W4ssHu1LE7Z_ByMt0kGY8_GR2MPqCHSO40o4znOAoLPoMXUzF2dfixYlq_7oNNIRFZIfaewaPkO9NTGDpgxSjWPkzONJUEYe_rfqPx1nWvO68ZQtcKW3EUPeTv7dSnF5fx4sm_mz6F2wXCsUTEOYDd7ZfeP0M4tbXPY9x8B54XHbo
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMw1V1Lb9QwEB6VcigXHi2UhQIGQdVLVnEcO86BQ9WHtq89oBb1ZhzHqRbY7IpshMpv4q_wnxjHSaA8JC49cItiJ3HsGc_Y_uYbgJdRloRGxDTQONUFseY2SDU6crEVmUblikPr9iFPxmJ0Fh-e8_Ml-NrFwnh-iH7DzWlGM187BXcb0j9puX1fDqngqWghlUf28jMu2KrXB7s4uq-iaH_vdGcUtDkFAuNCTAONFl0antqc0kQXUqZJKvKYylBzFhlWpLHNdC4LnUrK0ZjbLLTC0DCLYqZFzvC9N-CmyyDumPp33_wgq5KiSYnsCNwCScV5y2PkcEN9U69av99c2p6v9Kqr3Ni6_TvwreslD3H5MKwX2dB8-YVA8n_pxrtwu3W6ybbXknuwZMtVWNsu9WI2vSSbpIHBNucLq7Cy06XAWwP7dlLV-KDjIG1QoWQ6y12-M1uRaU2qet7CiEviwz2J57Rygk98fiQy75FDZFISjXrumLHJpA-Eq-7D2bX8_ANYLmelfQhEFiYVltGCZxzLCh1GOovSJLQml7mxA9jq5EWZlrTd5Q75qLrFGw6faoZvAC_6qnPPVPKnShud0Kl2sqoUo4yHaGgEH8Dzvhi72p0d6dLOalcnkug84vJiAOteWPuvMJT4hFF8etNLb1_iuMsv6rnCWxe1qqxi6J0n2IqtRiT_3k61dzhuLh79e9VnsDI6PTlWxwfjo8dwK0Lv0-OONmB58am2T9B7XGRPG6Ul8O66xfs7g996lw
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMw1V1Lb9QwEB6VIgEXHi3QhQIGQdVLVnEejn3gUHW76gNWCFG0N9dJnGopm43IRqj8Jf4KP4qxnQTKQ-LSA7codhLH_sYztme-AXgepImfsYh6Cqc6L1Kx9oRCQy7SLFUoXJGvzT7k6wnbP44Op_F0Bb52sTCOH6LfcDOSYedrI-BVXvwk5PpDOaQsFqz1qDzS559xvVa_PBjh4L4IgvHeu919r00p4GUmwtRTqNB5FgudU5qognORCJZHlPsqDoMsLESkU5XzQglOY9TlOvU1y6ifBlGoWB7ie6_A1Yj5wuSJGL39wVXFmc2IbPjbPE7ZtKUxMm5DfVMvKr_fLNqervSipWxV3fgWfOs6yXm4nA2bZTrMvvzCH_mf9OJtuNma3GTHycgdWNHlGqzvlGq5mJ-TLWKdYO3pwhpc3-0S4K2Dfj-rG3zQMJBan1AyX-Qm25muybwhdVO1TsQlccGexDFaGdgTlx2JVL3fEJmVRKGUG15sMuvD4Oq7cHwpP38PVstFqTeA8CITTIe0iNMYywrlByoNROLrLOd5pgew3cFFZi1lu8kc8lF2SzccPmmHbwDP-qqV4yn5U6XNDnOynapqGdIw9lHNsHgAT_ti7GpzcqRKvWhMnYCj6YiLiwHcd1jtvxIi4JOQ4tNbDrx9iWEuP20qibdOG1lrGaJtnmArti0i_95OuXc4sRcP_r3qE7j2ZjSWrw4mRw_hRoCmp3M62oTV5adGP0LTcZk-tiJL4OSy0f0djWx5Rg
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Visual+attention+modulates+mu+suppression+during+biological+motion+perception+in+autistic+individuals&rft.jtitle=The+European+journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Sotoodeh%2C+Mohammad+Saber&rft.au=Chien%2C+Sarina+Hui-Lin&rft.au=Hadjikhani%2C+Nouchine&rft.date=2024-12-01&rft.issn=0953-816X&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=6668&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fejn.16596&rft.externalDocID=oai_gup_ub_gu_se_343576
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon