Is perceived angular displacement the time integral of perceived angular velocity?

Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are e...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBrain research bulletin Vol. 40; no. 5; pp. 467 - 470
Main Authors Mergner, T., Rumberger, A., Becker, W.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Elsevier Inc 1996
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0361-9230
1873-2747
DOI10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8

Cover

Abstract Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 × n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40°/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320° total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.
AbstractList Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 × n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40°/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320° total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.
Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degrees/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degrees total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.
Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degrees/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degrees total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degrees/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degrees total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.
Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degree /s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degree total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.
Author Becker, W.
Rumberger, A.
Mergner, T.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: T.
  surname: Mergner
  fullname: Mergner, T.
  organization: Breisacherstr. 64, Neurologische Klinik, Universität Freiburg, 79106 Freiburg, Germany
– sequence: 2
  givenname: A.
  surname: Rumberger
  fullname: Rumberger, A.
  organization: Breisacherstr. 64, Neurologische Klinik, Universität Freiburg, 79106 Freiburg, Germany
– sequence: 3
  givenname: W.
  surname: Becker
  fullname: Becker, W.
  organization: Sektion Neurophysiologie, Universität Ulm, Oberer Eselsberg, 89069 Ulm, Germany
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8886375$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkc1rGzEQxUVJSG03_0EKeyrNYRPJsyvN9pASQr4gEAjNWWil2URlP1xJNvi_zxq7OZQSn4Zh3u_BvDdlB_3QE2Mngp8JLuQ5Bynyag78eyVPORcF5PiJTQQqyOeqUAds8i75zKYx_uacSyzlETtCRAmqnLCn-5gtKFjyK3KZ6V-WrQmZ83HRGksd9SlLr5Ql31Hm-0QvwbTZ0PyHWVE7WJ_WP7-ww8a0kY53c8aeb65_Xd3lD4-391eXD7kFxJQTGXRCYlHWtVWiUtDUHEoEciBqgnnhCKk2jSAoZdWQRGHKwqIQYMEZmLFvW99FGP4sKSbd-WipbU1PwzJqhQXOuZJ7haKUCKDEKPy6Ey7rjpxeBN-ZsNa7tMb7j-3dhiHGQI0eHzbJD30KxrdacL1pRm9i15vYdTUum2Y0jnDxD_zXfg92scVojHLlKehoPfWWnA9kk3aD_9jgDSb6pRA
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_3233_VES_201527
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11571_015_9362_0
crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193318
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4959_7
crossref_primary_10_1093_brain_122_7_1293
crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193556
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_05483_6
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00694_2006
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00804_2003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2006_04_005
crossref_primary_10_3233_VES_220016
crossref_primary_10_1016_S1074_7427_03_00041_8
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1749_6632_1999_tb09212_x
crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03212063
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_007_1072_3
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4638_0
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_015_4433_3
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnint_2021_742100
Cites_doi 10.1007/BF00229416
10.2307/1418112
10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.661
10.1037/h0025267
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 1996
Copyright_xml – notice: 1996
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7TK
7X8
DOI 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
MEDLINE
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1873-2747
EndPage 470
ExternalDocumentID 8886375
10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8
0361923096001438
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
-~X
.1-
.FO
.GJ
.~1
0R~
0SF
1B1
1P~
1RT
1~.
1~5
23N
4.4
41~
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
6J9
7-5
71M
8P~
9JM
AABNK
AACTN
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFWJ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABTEW
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACIUM
ACRLP
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADKLL
ADMUD
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AEVXI
AFCTW
AFKWA
AFMIJ
AFPKN
AFRHN
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
AJUYK
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
CS3
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
GBLVA
GROUPED_DOAJ
HMQ
HVGLF
HZ~
IHE
J1W
KOM
M2V
M41
MO0
MOBAO
N9A
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OP~
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SCC
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SNS
SPCBC
SSN
SSZ
T5K
WUQ
Z5R
ZGI
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADNMO
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7TK
EFKBS
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c388t-eea8d16845bbc71973fb03583ed31be324de8ebaf1e3569fe681a54c8113c3da3
IEDL.DBID .~1
ISSN 0361-9230
IngestDate Thu Sep 04 18:27:20 EDT 2025
Thu Sep 04 21:26:17 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:36:20 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:42:38 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:00:02 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:26:30 EST 2024
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Keywords Model
Velocity estimation
Vestibular system
Time constant
Displacement estimation
Language English
License https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c388t-eea8d16845bbc71973fb03583ed31be324de8ebaf1e3569fe681a54c8113c3da3
Notes ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 23
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
PMID 8886375
PQID 15683371
PQPubID 23462
PageCount 4
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_78482076
proquest_miscellaneous_15683371
pubmed_primary_8886375
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 1900
PublicationDate 1996
1996-1-00
1996-00-00
19960101
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 1996-01-01
PublicationDate_xml – year: 1996
  text: 1996
PublicationDecade 1990
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle Brain research bulletin
PublicationTitleAlternate Brain Res Bull
PublicationYear 1996
Publisher Elsevier Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Inc
References Robinson (BIB7) 1981; II
Wilson, Melvil Jones (BIB9) 1979
Poulton (BIB6) 1968; 69
Steven (BIB8) 1957; 69
Mach (BIB3) 1875
Guedry (BIB2) 1974
Fernandez, Goldberg (BIB1) 1971; 34
Mergner, Becker (BIB4) 1990
Mergner, Siebold, Schweigart, Becker (BIB5) 1991; 85
Wilson (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB9) 1979
Fernandez (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB1) 1971; 34
Guedry (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB2) 1974
Mergner (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB4) 1990
Mergner (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB5) 1991; 85
Steven (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB8) 1957; 69
Poulton (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB6) 1968; 69
Mach (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB3) 1875
Robinson (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB7) 1981; II
References_xml – volume: 69
  start-page: 1
  year: 1968
  end-page: 19
  ident: BIB6
  article-title: The new psychophysics: Six models for magnitude estimation
  publication-title: Psychol. Bull.
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1
  year: 1957
  end-page: 25
  ident: BIB8
  article-title: The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes—Loudness
  publication-title: Am. J. Psychol.
– volume: 85
  start-page: 389
  year: 1991
  end-page: 404
  ident: BIB5
  article-title: Human perception of horizontal trunk and head rotation in space during vestibular and neck stimulation
  publication-title: Exp. Brain Res.
– year: 1875
  ident: BIB3
  article-title: Grundlinien der Lehre von den Bewegungsempfindungen
– volume: 34
  start-page: 661
  year: 1971
  end-page: 675
  ident: BIB1
  article-title: Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. II. Response to sinusoidal stimulation and dynamics of peripheral vestibular system
  publication-title: J. Neurophysiol.
– year: 1979
  ident: BIB9
  article-title: Mammalian vestibular physiology
– volume: II
  start-page: 1230
  year: 1981
  end-page: 1277
  ident: BIB7
  article-title: Control of eye movements
  publication-title: Handbook of physiology—The nervous system
– start-page: 1
  year: 1974
  end-page: 154
  ident: BIB2
  article-title: Psychophysics of vestibular sensation
  publication-title: Vestibular system, Part 2: Psychophysics, applied aspects and general interpretations
– start-page: 210
  year: 1990
  end-page: 263
  ident: BIB4
  article-title: Perception of horizontal self-rotation: Multisensory and cognitive aspects
  publication-title: Perception and control of self-motion
– start-page: 1
  year: 1974
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB2
  article-title: Psychophysics of vestibular sensation
– volume: 85
  start-page: 389
  year: 1991
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB5
  article-title: Human perception of horizontal trunk and head rotation in space during vestibular and neck stimulation
  publication-title: Exp. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1007/BF00229416
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1
  year: 1957
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB8
  article-title: The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes—Loudness
  publication-title: Am. J. Psychol.
  doi: 10.2307/1418112
– year: 1875
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB3
– start-page: 210
  year: 1990
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB4
  article-title: Perception of horizontal self-rotation: Multisensory and cognitive aspects
– volume: 34
  start-page: 661
  year: 1971
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB1
  article-title: Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. II. Response to sinusoidal stimulation and dynamics of peripheral vestibular system
  publication-title: J. Neurophysiol.
  doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.661
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1
  year: 1968
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB6
  article-title: The new psychophysics: Six models for magnitude estimation
  publication-title: Psychol. Bull.
  doi: 10.1037/h0025267
– volume: II
  start-page: 1230
  year: 1981
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB7
  article-title: Control of eye movements
– year: 1979
  ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB9
SSID ssj0006856
Score 1.6107742
Snippet Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion...
Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 467
SubjectTerms Adult
Displacement estimation
Female
Humans
Male
Orientation - physiology
Perception - physiology
Posture - physiology
Time constant
Velocity estimation
Vestibular Nuclei - physiology
Vestibular system
Title Is perceived angular displacement the time integral of perceived angular velocity?
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8886375
https://www.proquest.com/docview/15683371
https://www.proquest.com/docview/78482076
Volume 40
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Pi1MxEA6yXryIWher7pqDiB5im50kb95pKWJpFXsQF3oLyUsChfW1bLsHL_u3b-b9qAqWgteQPMJM3uSbyTczjL0NIbkADqlN6lgo8Fpgdm2Fdq66kDEERMod_rYwsyv1ZamXf-TCEK2ys_2tTW-sdTcy6qQ52qxWo2x6CZ0QBKcadZTvq1RBR_3j3W-Wh0HdPVdKQbP77DlpRvux96X50HxD4KHb6RD6bG6h6RP2uIOPfNLu8Cl7EOtnbDCps-v88xd_xxtCZxMpH7Dv8y3fEG8lG7TAKS6ZvVgeVtuGh0VRQZ7RH6fu8ryrGnHN1-kfa4hXVGW4fvmcXU0___g0E10LBVEB4k7E6DBIg0p7XxWyLCD5MWiEGED6mNFUiBi9SzKCNmWKBqXTqkIpoYLg4JSd1Os6vmB87LWTyZUpgVfKubKsojJoVNaov0h6yKAXna26-uLU5uLa9kQyErglgduy4dIpsDhkYr9q09bXODK_6LVi_zonNl8BR1a-6ZVo8y9E7yKujuvbrc0uLAIU8vCMAlVGSoUZstNW-_u9IqKBQr_87229Yo9aIjhFdV6zk93NbTzLOGfnz5uTfM4eTuZfZ4t7fgz3rw
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LaxsxEBatc2gvIU0a6jwaHUppD8JW9NjZUzAhwc7Dh5JAbkJaSWBI16Z2Dvn30exqTQM1gV6FZhEz2tE3o08zhHzzPlovLGCb1CGTwikGKbRlytrqlAfvAfDt8O1Uj-_l1YN6-OstDNIqs-9vfXrjrfPIIGtzsJjNBsn1IjpBCI416uA92ZIqBXs9sjWaXI-na3-sQeUbS85QoHtAx_VgPfaj1D-bzzDYdEBtAqDNQXS5Q7YzgqSjdpGfyLtQ75K9UZ2i59_P9DttOJ1NsnyP_Jos6QKpK8mneYqpyRTIUj9bNlQsTAzSBAApNpinuXDEI53Hf8ggtahKiP3sM7m_vLg7H7PcRYFVAmDFQrDguQapnKsKXhYiuqFQIIIX3IUEqHyA4GzkQShdxqCBWyUr4FxUwluxT3r1vA5fCB06ZXm0ZYzCSWltWVZBatAyGdWdRtUnolOdqXKJcex08Wg6Lhkq3KDCTdnQ6aQw0CdsLbVoS2y8Mb_orGJebRWTToE3JE86I5r0F-HViK3D_GlpUhQLQhR884wCZAJLhe6T_db667UCgBaFOvjvZZ2QD-O72xtzM5leH5KPLS8ckzxHpLf68xSOE-xZua95X78AFxT6Zg
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Is+perceived+angular+displacement+the+time+integral+of+perceived+angular+velocity%3F&rft.jtitle=Brain+research+bulletin&rft.au=Mergner%2C+T.&rft.au=Rumberger%2C+A.&rft.au=Becker%2C+W.&rft.date=1996&rft.pub=Elsevier+Inc&rft.issn=0361-9230&rft.eissn=1873-2747&rft.volume=40&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=467&rft.epage=470&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2F0361-9230%2896%2900143-8&rft.externalDocID=0361923096001438
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0361-9230&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0361-9230&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0361-9230&client=summon