Is perceived angular displacement the time integral of perceived angular velocity?
Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are e...
Saved in:
Published in | Brain research bulletin Vol. 40; no. 5; pp. 467 - 470 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Elsevier Inc
1996
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0361-9230 1873-2747 |
DOI | 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8 |
Cover
Abstract | Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 ×
n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40°/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320° total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 ×
n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40°/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320° total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used. Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degrees/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degrees total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used. Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degrees/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degrees total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used.Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degrees/s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degrees total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used. Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion perception. However, the assumption underlying this indiscriminate use has never been tested. The assumption holds that the two estimates are equivalent, with the displacement estimates reflecting the time integral of the signal underlying the velocity estimate. We tested this hypothesis by directly comparing displacement and velocity estimates. Two groups of healthy young subjects (2 x n = 15) were presented with the same vestibular stimuli (horizontal whole body rotations in the dark in the form of velocity steps of 5, 10, 20, and 40 degree /s with 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 s duration, yielding position ramps of 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 degree total displacement). The first subject group estimated peak velocity, and the second group estimated total displacement, both groups using a comparable psychophysical procedure (Stevens' magnitude estimation). The experimentally obtained velocity estimates were used to predict the displacement estimates. To this end, the velocity signal was assumed to decay exponentially from the reported peak value (reflecting the dynamics of peripheral and early central vestibular mechanisms) and was mathematically integrated. Predicted and measured displacement estimates were similar when a time constant of 20 s was assumed, which is in good agreement with earlier studies. We conclude that vestibular displacement estimates can, indeed, be considered equivalent to vestibular velocity estimates, at least for the stimulus parameters used. |
Author | Becker, W. Rumberger, A. Mergner, T. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: T. surname: Mergner fullname: Mergner, T. organization: Breisacherstr. 64, Neurologische Klinik, Universität Freiburg, 79106 Freiburg, Germany – sequence: 2 givenname: A. surname: Rumberger fullname: Rumberger, A. organization: Breisacherstr. 64, Neurologische Klinik, Universität Freiburg, 79106 Freiburg, Germany – sequence: 3 givenname: W. surname: Becker fullname: Becker, W. organization: Sektion Neurophysiologie, Universität Ulm, Oberer Eselsberg, 89069 Ulm, Germany |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8886375$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkc1rGzEQxUVJSG03_0EKeyrNYRPJsyvN9pASQr4gEAjNWWil2URlP1xJNvi_zxq7OZQSn4Zh3u_BvDdlB_3QE2Mngp8JLuQ5Bynyag78eyVPORcF5PiJTQQqyOeqUAds8i75zKYx_uacSyzlETtCRAmqnLCn-5gtKFjyK3KZ6V-WrQmZ83HRGksd9SlLr5Ql31Hm-0QvwbTZ0PyHWVE7WJ_WP7-ww8a0kY53c8aeb65_Xd3lD4-391eXD7kFxJQTGXRCYlHWtVWiUtDUHEoEciBqgnnhCKk2jSAoZdWQRGHKwqIQYMEZmLFvW99FGP4sKSbd-WipbU1PwzJqhQXOuZJ7haKUCKDEKPy6Ey7rjpxeBN-ZsNa7tMb7j-3dhiHGQI0eHzbJD30KxrdacL1pRm9i15vYdTUum2Y0jnDxD_zXfg92scVojHLlKehoPfWWnA9kk3aD_9jgDSb6pRA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_3233_VES_201527 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11571_015_9362_0 crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193318 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4959_7 crossref_primary_10_1093_brain_122_7_1293 crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193556 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_05483_6 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00694_2006 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00804_2003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2006_04_005 crossref_primary_10_3233_VES_220016 crossref_primary_10_1016_S1074_7427_03_00041_8 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1749_6632_1999_tb09212_x crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03212063 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_007_1072_3 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4638_0 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_015_4433_3 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnint_2021_742100 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/BF00229416 10.2307/1418112 10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.661 10.1037/h0025267 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 1996 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 1996 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1873-2747 |
EndPage | 470 |
ExternalDocumentID | 8886375 10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8 0361923096001438 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M -~X .1- .FO .GJ .~1 0R~ 0SF 1B1 1P~ 1RT 1~. 1~5 23N 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 6J9 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM AABNK AACTN AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFWJ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABMAC ABTEW ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACIUM ACRLP ADBBV ADEZE ADKLL ADMUD AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AENEX AEVXI AFCTW AFKWA AFMIJ AFPKN AFRHN AFTJW AFXIZ AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AJUYK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA GROUPED_DOAJ HMQ HVGLF HZ~ IHE J1W KOM M2V M41 MO0 MOBAO N9A O-L O9- OAUVE OP~ OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SSN SSZ T5K WUQ Z5R ZGI ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADNMO ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK EFKBS 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c388t-eea8d16845bbc71973fb03583ed31be324de8ebaf1e3569fe681a54c8113c3da3 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0361-9230 |
IngestDate | Thu Sep 04 18:27:20 EDT 2025 Thu Sep 04 21:26:17 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:36:20 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:42:38 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:00:02 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:26:30 EST 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 5 |
Keywords | Model Velocity estimation Vestibular system Time constant Displacement estimation |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c388t-eea8d16845bbc71973fb03583ed31be324de8ebaf1e3569fe681a54c8113c3da3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 8886375 |
PQID | 15683371 |
PQPubID | 23462 |
PageCount | 4 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_78482076 proquest_miscellaneous_15683371 pubmed_primary_8886375 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8 crossref_primary_10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_0361_9230_96_00143_8 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 1900 |
PublicationDate | 1996 1996-1-00 1996-00-00 19960101 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 1996-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – year: 1996 text: 1996 |
PublicationDecade | 1990 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Brain research bulletin |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Brain Res Bull |
PublicationYear | 1996 |
Publisher | Elsevier Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Inc |
References | Robinson (BIB7) 1981; II Wilson, Melvil Jones (BIB9) 1979 Poulton (BIB6) 1968; 69 Steven (BIB8) 1957; 69 Mach (BIB3) 1875 Guedry (BIB2) 1974 Fernandez, Goldberg (BIB1) 1971; 34 Mergner, Becker (BIB4) 1990 Mergner, Siebold, Schweigart, Becker (BIB5) 1991; 85 Wilson (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB9) 1979 Fernandez (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB1) 1971; 34 Guedry (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB2) 1974 Mergner (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB4) 1990 Mergner (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB5) 1991; 85 Steven (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB8) 1957; 69 Poulton (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB6) 1968; 69 Mach (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB3) 1875 Robinson (10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB7) 1981; II |
References_xml | – volume: 69 start-page: 1 year: 1968 end-page: 19 ident: BIB6 article-title: The new psychophysics: Six models for magnitude estimation publication-title: Psychol. Bull. – volume: 69 start-page: 1 year: 1957 end-page: 25 ident: BIB8 article-title: The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes—Loudness publication-title: Am. J. Psychol. – volume: 85 start-page: 389 year: 1991 end-page: 404 ident: BIB5 article-title: Human perception of horizontal trunk and head rotation in space during vestibular and neck stimulation publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. – year: 1875 ident: BIB3 article-title: Grundlinien der Lehre von den Bewegungsempfindungen – volume: 34 start-page: 661 year: 1971 end-page: 675 ident: BIB1 article-title: Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. II. Response to sinusoidal stimulation and dynamics of peripheral vestibular system publication-title: J. Neurophysiol. – year: 1979 ident: BIB9 article-title: Mammalian vestibular physiology – volume: II start-page: 1230 year: 1981 end-page: 1277 ident: BIB7 article-title: Control of eye movements publication-title: Handbook of physiology—The nervous system – start-page: 1 year: 1974 end-page: 154 ident: BIB2 article-title: Psychophysics of vestibular sensation publication-title: Vestibular system, Part 2: Psychophysics, applied aspects and general interpretations – start-page: 210 year: 1990 end-page: 263 ident: BIB4 article-title: Perception of horizontal self-rotation: Multisensory and cognitive aspects publication-title: Perception and control of self-motion – start-page: 1 year: 1974 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB2 article-title: Psychophysics of vestibular sensation – volume: 85 start-page: 389 year: 1991 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB5 article-title: Human perception of horizontal trunk and head rotation in space during vestibular and neck stimulation publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/BF00229416 – volume: 69 start-page: 1 year: 1957 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB8 article-title: The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes—Loudness publication-title: Am. J. Psychol. doi: 10.2307/1418112 – year: 1875 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB3 – start-page: 210 year: 1990 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB4 article-title: Perception of horizontal self-rotation: Multisensory and cognitive aspects – volume: 34 start-page: 661 year: 1971 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB1 article-title: Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. II. Response to sinusoidal stimulation and dynamics of peripheral vestibular system publication-title: J. Neurophysiol. doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.661 – volume: 69 start-page: 1 year: 1968 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB6 article-title: The new psychophysics: Six models for magnitude estimation publication-title: Psychol. Bull. doi: 10.1037/h0025267 – volume: II start-page: 1230 year: 1981 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB7 article-title: Control of eye movements – year: 1979 ident: 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8_BIB9 |
SSID | ssj0006856 |
Score | 1.6107742 |
Snippet | Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psychophysics to characterize vestibular ego-motion... Estimates of rotational self-displacement and self-velocity have been used interchangeably in vestibular psycho-physics to characterize vestibular ego-motion... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 467 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Displacement estimation Female Humans Male Orientation - physiology Perception - physiology Posture - physiology Time constant Velocity estimation Vestibular Nuclei - physiology Vestibular system |
Title | Is perceived angular displacement the time integral of perceived angular velocity? |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(96)00143-8 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8886375 https://www.proquest.com/docview/15683371 https://www.proquest.com/docview/78482076 |
Volume | 40 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Pi1MxEA6yXryIWher7pqDiB5im50kb95pKWJpFXsQF3oLyUsChfW1bLsHL_u3b-b9qAqWgteQPMJM3uSbyTczjL0NIbkADqlN6lgo8Fpgdm2Fdq66kDEERMod_rYwsyv1ZamXf-TCEK2ys_2tTW-sdTcy6qQ52qxWo2x6CZ0QBKcadZTvq1RBR_3j3W-Wh0HdPVdKQbP77DlpRvux96X50HxD4KHb6RD6bG6h6RP2uIOPfNLu8Cl7EOtnbDCps-v88xd_xxtCZxMpH7Dv8y3fEG8lG7TAKS6ZvVgeVtuGh0VRQZ7RH6fu8ryrGnHN1-kfa4hXVGW4fvmcXU0___g0E10LBVEB4k7E6DBIg0p7XxWyLCD5MWiEGED6mNFUiBi9SzKCNmWKBqXTqkIpoYLg4JSd1Os6vmB87LWTyZUpgVfKubKsojJoVNaov0h6yKAXna26-uLU5uLa9kQyErglgduy4dIpsDhkYr9q09bXODK_6LVi_zonNl8BR1a-6ZVo8y9E7yKujuvbrc0uLAIU8vCMAlVGSoUZstNW-_u9IqKBQr_87229Yo9aIjhFdV6zk93NbTzLOGfnz5uTfM4eTuZfZ4t7fgz3rw |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LaxsxEBatc2gvIU0a6jwaHUppD8JW9NjZUzAhwc7Dh5JAbkJaSWBI16Z2Dvn30exqTQM1gV6FZhEz2tE3o08zhHzzPlovLGCb1CGTwikGKbRlytrqlAfvAfDt8O1Uj-_l1YN6-OstDNIqs-9vfXrjrfPIIGtzsJjNBsn1IjpBCI416uA92ZIqBXs9sjWaXI-na3-sQeUbS85QoHtAx_VgPfaj1D-bzzDYdEBtAqDNQXS5Q7YzgqSjdpGfyLtQ75K9UZ2i59_P9DttOJ1NsnyP_Jos6QKpK8mneYqpyRTIUj9bNlQsTAzSBAApNpinuXDEI53Hf8ggtahKiP3sM7m_vLg7H7PcRYFVAmDFQrDguQapnKsKXhYiuqFQIIIX3IUEqHyA4GzkQShdxqCBWyUr4FxUwluxT3r1vA5fCB06ZXm0ZYzCSWltWVZBatAyGdWdRtUnolOdqXKJcex08Wg6Lhkq3KDCTdnQ6aQw0CdsLbVoS2y8Mb_orGJebRWTToE3JE86I5r0F-HViK3D_GlpUhQLQhR884wCZAJLhe6T_db667UCgBaFOvjvZZ2QD-O72xtzM5leH5KPLS8ckzxHpLf68xSOE-xZua95X78AFxT6Zg |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Is+perceived+angular+displacement+the+time+integral+of+perceived+angular+velocity%3F&rft.jtitle=Brain+research+bulletin&rft.au=Mergner%2C+T.&rft.au=Rumberger%2C+A.&rft.au=Becker%2C+W.&rft.date=1996&rft.pub=Elsevier+Inc&rft.issn=0361-9230&rft.eissn=1873-2747&rft.volume=40&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=467&rft.epage=470&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2F0361-9230%2896%2900143-8&rft.externalDocID=0361923096001438 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0361-9230&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0361-9230&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0361-9230&client=summon |