The surface structure and the deep structure of sequential control: What can we learn from task span switch costs?

A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might reflect passive priming rather than endogenous control. In contrast, the task span procedure requires subjects to guide task selection via sequenc...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inPsychonomic bulletin & review Vol. 17; no. 5; pp. 693 - 698
Main Author Mayr, Ulrich
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published New York Springer New York 01.10.2010
Springer
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1069-9384
1531-5320
1531-5320
DOI10.3758/PBR.17.5.693

Cover

Abstract A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might reflect passive priming rather than endogenous control. In contrast, the task span procedure requires subjects to guide task selection via sequences of memorized task cues and therefore may be better suited to reflect endogenous switch processes (Logan, 2004). The present experiments combined the task span procedure with a 2:1 mapping between cues and tasks, allowing separation of cue-switch costs from true task-switch costs. Replicating findings with the cued task-switching paradigm, results showed both substantial cue-switch costs and actual task-switch costs (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as sensitivity of cue-switch costs, but not of task-switch costs, to opportunity for preparation (Experiment 2). Apparently, simple action plans use “surface level” phonological or articulatory codes that contain no task information. These results suggest that the distinction between cue-related and task-related processes is critical no matter whether tasks are cued exogenously or endogenously.
AbstractList A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might reflect passive priming rather than endogenous control. In contrast, the task span procedure requires subjects to guide task selection via sequences of memorized task cues and therefore may be better suited to reflect endogenous switch processes (Logan, 2004). The present experiments combined the task span procedure with a 2:1 mapping between cues and tasks, allowing separation of cue-switch costs from true task-switch costs. Replicating findings with the cued task-switching paradigm, results showed both substantial cue-switch costs and actual task-switch costs (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as sensitivity of cue-switch costs, but not of task-switch costs, to opportunity for preparation (Experiment 2). Apparently, simple action plans use "surface level" phonological or articulatory codes that contain no task information. These results suggest that the distinction between cue-related and task-related processes is critical no matter whether tasks are cued exogenously or endogenously. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might reflect passive priming rather than endogenous control. In contrast, the task span procedure requires subjects to guide task selection via sequences of memorized task cues and therefore may be better suited to reflect endogenous switch processes (Logan, 2004). The present experiments combined the task span procedure with a 2:1 mapping between cues and tasks, allowing separation of cue-switch costs from true task-switch costs. Replicating findings with the cued task-switching paradigm, results showed both substantial cue-switch costs and actual task-switch costs (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as sensitivity of cue-switch costs, but not of task-switch costs, to opportunity for preparation (Experiment 2). Apparently, simple action plans use "surface level" phonological or articulatory codes that contain no task information. These results suggest that the distinction between cue-related and task-related processes is critical no matter whether tasks are cued exogenously or endogenously.A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might reflect passive priming rather than endogenous control. In contrast, the task span procedure requires subjects to guide task selection via sequences of memorized task cues and therefore may be better suited to reflect endogenous switch processes (Logan, 2004). The present experiments combined the task span procedure with a 2:1 mapping between cues and tasks, allowing separation of cue-switch costs from true task-switch costs. Replicating findings with the cued task-switching paradigm, results showed both substantial cue-switch costs and actual task-switch costs (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as sensitivity of cue-switch costs, but not of task-switch costs, to opportunity for preparation (Experiment 2). Apparently, simple action plans use "surface level" phonological or articulatory codes that contain no task information. These results suggest that the distinction between cue-related and task-related processes is critical no matter whether tasks are cued exogenously or endogenously.
A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might reflect passive priming rather than endogenous control. In contrast, the task span procedure requires subjects to guide task selection via sequences of memorized task cues and therefore may be better suited to reflect endogenous switch processes (Logan, 2004). The present experiments combined the task span procedure with a 2:1 mapping between cues and tasks, allowing separation of cue-switch costs from true task-switch costs. Replicating findings with the cued task-switching paradigm, results showed both substantial cue-switch costs and actual task-switch costs (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as sensitivity of cue-switch costs, but not of task-switch costs, to opportunity for preparation (Experiment 2). Apparently, simple action plans use "surface level" phonological or articulatory codes that contain no task information. These results suggest that the distinction between cue-related and task-related processes is critical no matter whether tasks are cued exogenously or endogenously.
Author Mayr, Ulrich
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Ulrich
  surname: Mayr
  fullname: Mayr, Ulrich
  email: mayr@uoregon.edu
  organization: Department of Psychology, University of Oregon
BackLink http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=23529775$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21037168$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp1kc2LFDEQxYPs4n7ozbMEQbxsj_noJB0voou7CguKrHgMmXTF6bUnPabSLP73ZpjRlUVPFSq_96iqd0IO0pSAkCecLaRR3ctPbz8vuFmohbbyATnmSvJGScEO6ptp21jZtUfkBPGGMaa01Q_JkeBMGq67Y5KvV0BxztGHWkueQ5kzUJ96WupPD7D5qz1FivBjhlQGP9IwpZKn8RX9uvKFBp_oLdARfE405mlNi8fvFDe1j7dDCasqwIKvH5HD6EeEx_t6Sr5cvLs-f99cfbz8cP7mqgmyU6XR3CwlV20QkgepRRt11FL2S6us8rwFa5TsGFjfdRGYWIJuhbE99IybVjN5Sl7sfDd5qjNjcesBA4yjTzDN6IwWTHLGt-Sze-TNNOdUh3Mda7mQ1qoKPd1D83INvdvkYe3zT_f7lhV4vgc8Bj_G7FMY8I6TSlhjtkZnOy7kCTFD_INw5raRuhqp48YpVyOtuLiHh6H4MmyP74fxf6JmJ8Lqnb5Bvtvon_wvTVqxLw
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2014_06_009
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2012_659191
crossref_primary_10_1027_2151_2604_a000125
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_014_0556_6
Cites_doi 10.3758/BF03213918
10.3758/BF03193297
10.1111/1467-8721.01223
10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.623
10.3758/CABN.8.1.74
10.1007/978-3-0348-8030-5_13
10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00728.x
10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00511-9
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01781.x
10.3758/BF03196549
10.3758/BF03195328
10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.4
10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.218
10.3758/BF03196511
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.004
10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.641
10.3758/BF03193999
10.1037/a0021967
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2010
2015 INIST-CNRS
Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Oct 2010
Copyright_xml – notice: Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2010
– notice: 2015 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Oct 2010
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
4U-
K9.
7X8
DOI 10.3758/PBR.17.5.693
DatabaseName CrossRef
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Docstoc
University Readers
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
University Readers
Docstoc
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
Psychology
EISSN 1531-5320
EndPage 698
ExternalDocumentID 2191380141
21037168
23529775
10_3758_PBR_17_5_693
Genre Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 AG19296-01A1
GroupedDBID ---
--Z
-55
-5G
-BR
-DZ
-EM
-~C
-~X
06D
0R~
0VY
123
186
199
1N0
2.D
203
29P
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2VQ
2WC
30V
3V.
4.4
406
408
40E
53G
7X7
85S
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8G5
8UJ
95.
96X
9M8
AAAVM
AABHQ
AACDK
AAEWM
AAGAY
AAHBH
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANZL
AARHV
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABDPE
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABJUD
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPPZ
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABUWG
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBNA
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACKIV
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACPIV
ACSFO
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETCA
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFDYV
AFFDN
AFFNX
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFQWF
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJUXI
AJZVZ
ALEEW
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
BAWUL
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BVXVI
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CSCUP
DDRTE
DIK
DNIVK
DPUIP
DU5
DWQXO
E3Z
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FYUFA
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ3
GQ6
GQ7
GUQSH
H13
HF~
HMCUK
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IKXTQ
IRVIT
ITM
IWAJR
J-C
JBSCW
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M1P
M2M
M2O
M4Y
MQGED
MVM
N2Q
N9A
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9I
O9J
OK1
P2P
P9L
PF-
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
QII
R9I
RHV
ROL
RPV
RSV
S10
S16
S1Z
S27
S3B
SBS
SBU
SCLPG
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TN5
TSG
TUC
U2A
U9L
UG4
UHB
UKHRP
UOJIU
UQL
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
VQA
VXZ
W48
WH7
WK8
XJT
XOL
XSW
YNT
YQJ
YR2
YYQ
Z81
Z83
Z92
ZCG
ZHY
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
ZUP
AAPKM
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
ADHKG
ADXHL
AETEA
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFOHR
AGQPQ
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
ATHPR
AYFIA
CITATION
PHGZM
PHGZT
AAEOY
AFQWV
B-7
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
4U-
ABRTQ
K9.
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c385t-617b3154c231c3624f6f633db9595a14e975380e9a88fe02be64279ded0174603
IEDL.DBID AGYKE
ISSN 1069-9384
1531-5320
IngestDate Fri Sep 05 05:02:42 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 03:57:05 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 07:07:26 EDT 2025
Wed Nov 20 03:16:42 EST 2024
Thu Apr 24 22:59:49 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:27:49 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:36:51 EST 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Keywords Task Sequence
Task Label
Task Switching
Task Span
Switch Cost
Human
Costs
Response time
Memory
Task switching
Cognition
Experimental study
Language English
License http://www.springer.com/tdm
CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c385t-617b3154c231c3624f6f633db9595a14e975380e9a88fe02be64279ded0174603
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
PMID 21037168
PQID 804123995
PQPubID 976349
PageCount 6
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_762031010
proquest_journals_804123995
pubmed_primary_21037168
pascalfrancis_primary_23529775
crossref_primary_10_3758_PBR_17_5_693
crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_PBR_17_5_693
springer_journals_10_3758_PBR_17_5_693
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2010-10-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2010-10-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 10
  year: 2010
  text: 2010-10-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace New York
PublicationPlace_xml – name: New York
– name: New York, NY
– name: United States
PublicationTitle Psychonomic bulletin & review
PublicationTitleAbbrev Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
PublicationTitleAlternate Psychon Bull Rev
PublicationYear 2010
Publisher Springer New York
Springer
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer New York
– name: Springer
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Bryck, Mayr (CR5) 2005; 33
Lien, Ruthruff (CR10) 2004; 30
Logan, Bundesen (CR14) 2003; 29
Mayr, Kluwe, Luer, Rösler (CR16) 2003
Koch (CR9) 2003; 10
Rogers, Monsell (CR25) 1995; 124
Altmann (CR1) 2006; 13
Arrington, Logan (CR2) 2004; 15
Logan (CR11) 2003; 12
Mayr (CR17) 2006; 13
Gotler, Meiran, Tzelgov (CR7) 2003; 10
Arrington, Logan, Schneider (CR3) 2007; 33
Vygotsky, Luria, van der Veer, Valsiner (CR28) 1994
Baddeley, Chincotta, Adlam (CR4) 2001; 130
Mayr, Kliegl (CR21) 2003; 29
Meiran (CR22) 1996; 22
Mayr, Keele (CR20) 2000; 129
Schneider, Logan (CR26) 2006; 135
Monsell (CR23) 2003; 7
Logan (CR12) 2004; 133
Monsell, Mizon (CR24) 2006; 32
CR27
Logan (CR13) 2007; 35
Mayr, Bell (CR19) 2006; 17
Mayr (CR18) 2009; 59
Jost, Mayr, Rösler (CR8) 2008; 8
Emerson, Miyake (CR6) 2003; 48
Logan, Schneider (CR15) 2006; 32
M. J. Emerson (170500693_CR6) 2003; 48
U. Mayr (170500693_CR16) 2003
E. M. Altmann (170500693_CR1) 2006; 13
U. Mayr (170500693_CR18) 2009; 59
N. Meiran (170500693_CR22) 1996; 22
U. Mayr (170500693_CR17) 2006; 13
I. Koch (170500693_CR9) 2003; 10
G. D. Logan (170500693_CR15) 2006; 32
S. Monsell (170500693_CR23) 2003; 7
S. Monsell (170500693_CR24) 2006; 32
U. Mayr (170500693_CR19) 2006; 17
C. M. Arrington (170500693_CR3) 2007; 33
R. L. Bryck (170500693_CR5) 2005; 33
A. Gotler (170500693_CR7) 2003; 10
G. D. Logan (170500693_CR11) 2003; 12
G. D. Logan (170500693_CR14) 2003; 29
U. Mayr (170500693_CR20) 2000; 129
C. M. Arrington (170500693_CR2) 2004; 15
M.-C. Lien (170500693_CR10) 2004; 30
U. Mayr (170500693_CR21) 2003; 29
R. D. Rogers (170500693_CR25) 1995; 124
G. D. Logan (170500693_CR13) 2007; 35
K. Jost (170500693_CR8) 2008; 8
170500693_CR27
D. W. Schneider (170500693_CR26) 2006; 135
G. D. Logan (170500693_CR12) 2004; 133
L. S. Vygotsky (170500693_CR28) 1994
A. Baddeley (170500693_CR4) 2001; 130
16984294 - Psychol Sci. 2006 Sep;17(9):774-80
15149251 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004 Jun;133(2):218-36
15000536 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2003 Dec;10(4):890-6
11757873 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001 Dec;130(4):641-57
16569151 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2006 Mar;32(2):347-63
17328375 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Oct;13(5):794-9
12921429 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2003 Jun;10(2):488-92
17470002 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2007 May;33(3):484-502
17484428 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Dec;13(6):1016-22
12639695 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Mar;7(3):134-140
17087577 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2006 Nov;135(4):623-40
18405048 - Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008 Mar;8(1):74-84
17848017 - Mem Cognit. 2007 Jun;35(4):591-602
16822121 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2006 Jun;32(3):493-516
16248326 - Mem Cognit. 2005 Jun;33(4):611-23
12848327 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003 Jun;29(3):575-99
15327632 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Sep;15(9):610-5
19427636 - Cogn Psychol. 2009 Sep;59(2):123-53
15099137 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004 May;30(3):697-713
10756484 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2000 Mar;129(1):4-26
21299334 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2011 Mar;37(2):405-15
12776747 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003 May;29(3):362-72
References_xml – volume: 13
  start-page: 1016
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1022
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Task switching is not cue switching
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03213918
– volume: 35
  start-page: 591
  year: 2007
  end-page: 602
  ident: CR13
  article-title: What it costs to implement a plan: Plan-level and task-level contributions to switch costs
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193297
– volume: 12
  start-page: 45
  year: 2003
  end-page: 48
  ident: CR11
  article-title: Executive control of thought and action: In search of the wild homunculus
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01223
– volume: 135
  start-page: 623
  year: 2006
  end-page: 640
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: Switching tasks in sequences
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.623
– volume: 8
  start-page: 74
  year: 2008
  end-page: 84
  ident: CR8
  article-title: Is task switching nothing but cue priming? Evidence from ERPs
  publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.1.74
– start-page: 223
  year: 2003
  end-page: 240
  ident: CR16
  article-title: Towards principles of executive control: How mental sets are selected
  publication-title: Principles of learning and memory
  doi: 10.1007/978-3-0348-8030-5_13
– volume: 124
  start-page: 207
  year: 1995
  end-page: 231
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
– volume: 7
  start-page: 134
  year: 2003
  end-page: 140
  ident: CR23
  article-title: Task switching
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
– volume: 15
  start-page: 610
  year: 2004
  end-page: 615
  ident: CR2
  article-title: The cost of a voluntary task switch
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00728.x
– volume: 32
  start-page: 347
  year: 2006
  end-page: 363
  ident: CR15
  article-title: Interpreting instructional cues in task switching procedures: The role of mediator retrieval
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 48
  start-page: 148
  year: 2003
  end-page: 168
  ident: CR6
  article-title: The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation
  publication-title: Journal of Memory & Language
  doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00511-9
– volume: 32
  start-page: 493
  year: 2006
  end-page: 516
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an “endogenous” task-set reconfiguration process?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
– volume: 17
  start-page: 774
  year: 2006
  end-page: 780
  ident: CR19
  article-title: On how to be unpredictable: Evidence from the voluntary task-switching paradigm
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01781.x
– volume: 10
  start-page: 890
  year: 2003
  end-page: 896
  ident: CR7
  article-title: Nonintentional task set activation: Evidence from implicit task sequence learning
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196549
– ident: CR27
– volume: 33
  start-page: 611
  year: 2005
  end-page: 623
  ident: CR5
  article-title: On the role of verbalization during task set selection: Switching or serial order control?
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03195328
– volume: 129
  start-page: 4
  year: 2000
  end-page: 26
  ident: CR20
  article-title: Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.4
– volume: 29
  start-page: 362
  year: 2003
  end-page: 372
  ident: CR21
  article-title: Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 133
  start-page: 218
  year: 2004
  end-page: 236
  ident: CR12
  article-title: Working memory, task switching, and executive control in the task span procedure
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.218
– volume: 10
  start-page: 488
  year: 2003
  end-page: 492
  ident: CR9
  article-title: The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196511
– start-page: 99
  year: 1994
  end-page: 174
  ident: CR28
  article-title: Tool and symbol in child development
  publication-title: The Vygotsky reader
– volume: 29
  start-page: 575
  year: 2003
  end-page: 599
  ident: CR14
  article-title: Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
– volume: 59
  start-page: 123
  year: 2009
  end-page: 153
  ident: CR18
  article-title: Sticky plans: Inhibition and binding during serial task control
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.004
– volume: 33
  start-page: 484
  year: 2007
  end-page: 502
  ident: CR3
  article-title: Separating cue encoding from target processing in the explicit task-cuing procedure: Are there “true” task switch effects?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 30
  start-page: 697
  year: 2004
  end-page: 713
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Task switching in a hierarchical task structure: Evidence for the fragility of the task repetition benefit
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 130
  start-page: 641
  year: 2001
  end-page: 657
  ident: CR4
  article-title: Working memory and the control of action: Evidence from task switching
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.641
– volume: 13
  start-page: 794
  year: 2006
  end-page: 799
  ident: CR17
  article-title: What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193999
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1423
  year: 1996
  end-page: 1442
  ident: CR22
  article-title: Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 30
  start-page: 697
  year: 2004
  ident: 170500693_CR10
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1423
  year: 1996
  ident: 170500693_CR22
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 10
  start-page: 890
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR7
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196549
– volume: 15
  start-page: 610
  year: 2004
  ident: 170500693_CR2
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00728.x
– volume: 124
  start-page: 207
  year: 1995
  ident: 170500693_CR25
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
– volume: 10
  start-page: 488
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR9
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196511
– volume: 33
  start-page: 484
  year: 2007
  ident: 170500693_CR3
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 29
  start-page: 575
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR14
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
– volume: 17
  start-page: 774
  year: 2006
  ident: 170500693_CR19
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01781.x
– volume: 129
  start-page: 4
  year: 2000
  ident: 170500693_CR20
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.4
– volume: 13
  start-page: 1016
  year: 2006
  ident: 170500693_CR1
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03213918
– volume: 130
  start-page: 641
  year: 2001
  ident: 170500693_CR4
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.641
– volume: 8
  start-page: 74
  year: 2008
  ident: 170500693_CR8
  publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.1.74
– volume: 33
  start-page: 611
  year: 2005
  ident: 170500693_CR5
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03195328
– volume: 29
  start-page: 362
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR21
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 7
  start-page: 134
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR23
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
– volume: 13
  start-page: 794
  year: 2006
  ident: 170500693_CR17
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193999
– ident: 170500693_CR27
  doi: 10.1037/a0021967
– volume: 35
  start-page: 591
  year: 2007
  ident: 170500693_CR13
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193297
– volume: 32
  start-page: 493
  year: 2006
  ident: 170500693_CR24
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
– volume: 135
  start-page: 623
  year: 2006
  ident: 170500693_CR26
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.623
– start-page: 99
  volume-title: The Vygotsky reader
  year: 1994
  ident: 170500693_CR28
– start-page: 223
  volume-title: Principles of learning and memory
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR16
  doi: 10.1007/978-3-0348-8030-5_13
– volume: 133
  start-page: 218
  year: 2004
  ident: 170500693_CR12
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.218
– volume: 59
  start-page: 123
  year: 2009
  ident: 170500693_CR18
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.004
– volume: 48
  start-page: 148
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR6
  publication-title: Journal of Memory & Language
  doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00511-9
– volume: 12
  start-page: 45
  year: 2003
  ident: 170500693_CR11
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01223
– volume: 32
  start-page: 347
  year: 2006
  ident: 170500693_CR15
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– reference: 10756484 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2000 Mar;129(1):4-26
– reference: 15000536 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2003 Dec;10(4):890-6
– reference: 15099137 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004 May;30(3):697-713
– reference: 16569151 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2006 Mar;32(2):347-63
– reference: 16248326 - Mem Cognit. 2005 Jun;33(4):611-23
– reference: 11757873 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001 Dec;130(4):641-57
– reference: 17328375 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Oct;13(5):794-9
– reference: 16984294 - Psychol Sci. 2006 Sep;17(9):774-80
– reference: 12776747 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003 May;29(3):362-72
– reference: 17484428 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Dec;13(6):1016-22
– reference: 15327632 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Sep;15(9):610-5
– reference: 12848327 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003 Jun;29(3):575-99
– reference: 21299334 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2011 Mar;37(2):405-15
– reference: 19427636 - Cogn Psychol. 2009 Sep;59(2):123-53
– reference: 15149251 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004 Jun;133(2):218-36
– reference: 17848017 - Mem Cognit. 2007 Jun;35(4):591-602
– reference: 18405048 - Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008 Mar;8(1):74-84
– reference: 12921429 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2003 Jun;10(2):488-92
– reference: 16822121 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2006 Jun;32(3):493-516
– reference: 12639695 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Mar;7(3):134-140
– reference: 17470002 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2007 May;33(3):484-502
– reference: 17087577 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2006 Nov;135(4):623-40
SSID ssj0005696
Score 1.9746336
Snippet A large component of response time switch costs in the cued task-switching paradigm is linked to cue changes without task changes, suggesting costs might...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
crossref
springer
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 693
SubjectTerms Behavioral Science and Psychology
Biological and medical sciences
Brief Reports
Cognition - physiology
Cognition. Intelligence
Cognitive Psychology
Color Perception - physiology
Costs
Cues
Data collection
Discrimination (Psychology) - physiology
Form Perception - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Miscellaneous
Photic Stimulation
Psychology
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Reaction Time - physiology
Serial Learning - physiology
Visual task performance
Title The surface structure and the deep structure of sequential control: What can we learn from task span switch costs?
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/PBR.17.5.693
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21037168
https://www.proquest.com/docview/804123995
https://www.proquest.com/docview/762031010
Volume 17
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV1Lb9QwEB7B9lIJUWihpIXVHKAXlJCHnQcXtKCWQsVDiJXKyYodW0It2dUmq6r8esZOsktpkXpJpMSJHHs88038zQzAc6MNGTrG_MyY1Gc8Ur5MMu7HBavI-qksdMUmPn1Oj6fs4yk_7R3FZmC7D1uSTlNbv5JA7auvb78FURbwIC2Su7DBo7zIR7Axef_j5HBN6khdRS7ycwq_SHLWUd2vPX_FCN2blw2Nh-kKWdyENK_tkjrjc7QFX4Zud5yTs2DZykD9_iej4-2_6wHc73EoTjrBeQh3dL0NO5OafPBfl3iAjhnqfrlvw-ZKS17uwIIEC5vlwpSKzi777HKhsawrJCyJldbzvy7PDHZkbVIk59jz4l-jTRiONKl4odHVrUAb5oJt2Zwhqbgam4ufJE70QNM2bx7B9Ojw-7tjvy_c4Ksk560NO5QJYTNF4FGRhWQmNWmSVLLgBS8jpm00bx7qosxzo8NYavKCsqLSFekHlobJYxjVs1o_AZR0yUjyo0ulmNShNAQotCZYFsZVKrUHL4eZFKrPam6La5wL8m7s-AoaXxFlggsaXw9erFrPu2we_2k3viIUq8Yx4VVCzNyD_UFKRL_oG2FTOblQYQ9wdZdWq92CKWs9WzaCTI_NxRqFHux2srV-tY3YjNLcg4NBTtavvqmTe7dtuA-bjvPgKIhPYUQyoJ8RlGrluF8_Y3ImPpzQcRpP_gDDpRoW
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV1Lb9QwEB7B9kClikfLIxTKHKAXlJBsnBcXtKCWhT5AqJXKyYodW6pasqt1VlX59YydZJfSIvUUyXEsxxnPfBN_MwPwWitNho4xP9M69VkSSV_EWeIPC1aR9ZNZ6IpNHBym42P29SQ56RxF07Pd-yNJp6mtX0mg9t33jz-CKAuSIC3iu7DCyPsOB7Ay-vxzb2dJ6khdRS7ycwq_iHPWUt2vPX_FCK1NS0ProdtCFjchzWunpM747D6Ab_20W87JWTBvRCB__5PR8fbv9RDudzgUR63gPII7ql6HjVFNPvivS9xGxwx1v9zXYXWhJS83YEaChWY-06Wkq8s-O58pLOsKCUtipdT0r-aJxpasTYrkHDte_Hu0CcORPipeKHR1K9CGuWBTmjMkFVejuTglcaIHTGM-PIbj3Z2jT2O_K9zgyzhPGht2KGLCZpLAoyQLyXSq0ziuRJEUSRkxZaN581AVZZ5rFQ6FIi8oKypVkX5gaRg_gUE9qdUzQEFNWpAfXUrJhAqFJkChFMGycFilQnnwtv-SXHZZzW1xjXNO3o1dX07ry6OMJ5zW14M3i97TNpvHf_ptXRGKRech4VVCzIkHm72U8G7TG25TOblQYQ9wcZd2qz2CKWs1mRtOpsfmYo1CD562srUc2kZsRmnuwXYvJ8uhb5rk89t2fAX3xkcH-3z_y-HeJqw6_oOjI76AAcmDekmwqhFb3V76A63AGxE
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV1La9wwEB7aFEqglDRpUzdtOoc2l-KNH5Js9xK2jyV9hVC6kJuwbAlCUu-y8hLy7zuS7d2GpNCTQZaE0Yw031jfzAC8MdqQoWMszIwRIeNxFao042FSsJqsX5VFvtjEjxNxPGVfz_hZz6q0A9t9uJLsYhpclqamPZzXxm1xmig_PP3wcxRnIz4SRXofHtBBHDsdnybjNb1D-Npc5PEUYZHmrCO93xp9wxw9mpeWVsZ0JS3uwpy37ku9GZpsweMeP-K4E_gTuKebbdgZN-Q7_77GA_SMTv-rfBs2V6fb9Q4sSCHQLhemrOjps8YuFxrLpkbCgFhrPf-reWawI1nTAXCJPZ_9PbpE30jCwCuNvt4EuvAUbEt7gXQ0NWivzkkNaIBt7dFTmE4-__p4HPYFF8IqzXnrwgVVSpiqItBXkWVjRhiRprUqeMHLmGkXhZtHuijz3OgoUZq8l6yodU37mokofQYbzazRzwEVNRlF_m9ZVUzpSBkCAloTnIqSWigdwLth3WXVZyN3RTEuJXklTkqSpCTjTHJJUgrg7ar3vMvC8Y9--zdEuOqcEM4kpMsD2BtkKvvNaqVLweRDfAPA1VvaZe7qpGz0bGklmQyXQzWOAtjtNGE9tYu0jEUewMGgGuup7_rIF__b8TU8PP00kd-_nHzbg01PW_AswpewQeqgXxEaatW-V_s_LXQDaQ
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+surface+structure+and+the+deep+structure+of+sequential+control%3A+what+can+we+learn+from+task+span+switch+costs%3F&rft.jtitle=Psychonomic+bulletin+%26+review&rft.au=Mayr%2C+Ulrich&rft.date=2010-10-01&rft.issn=1531-5320&rft.eissn=1531-5320&rft.volume=17&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=693&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2FPBR.17.5.693&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1069-9384&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1069-9384&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1069-9384&client=summon