Modal preferences in creative problem solving

Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inCognitive processing Vol. 13; no. Suppl 1; pp. 147 - 150
Main Authors Deininger, Gina, Loudon, Gareth, Norman, Stefanie
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Berlin/Heidelberg Springer-Verlag 01.08.2012
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1612-4782
1612-4790
1612-4790
DOI10.1007/s10339-012-0479-3

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a puzzle based on De Bono’s lateral thinking puzzles. The puzzle consisted of rotating and joining two-dimensional shapes to make a three-dimensional one. In one condition, participants were given the choice of either solving the puzzle mentally or through manipulation of the images on a computer screen. In another condition, the subjects had to solve the puzzle first mentally and then report which mode they would have preferred to solve the puzzle. Two more conditions were applied with slight variations. In all conditions, an overwhelming majority of participants chose to solve the puzzle by manipulation, even though there was not a significant increase on performance. It appeared that participants were making a conscious choice for the body to play a feedback-driven role in creative cognitive processing. This strong preference for manual manipulation over just mental representation, regardless of the impact on performance, would seem to suggest that it is our natural tendency to involve the body in complex cognitive functioning. This would support the theory that cognition may be more than just a neural process, and that it is a dynamic interplay between body, brain and world. The experiential feedback of the body moving through space and time may be an inherently important factor in creative cognition.
AbstractList Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a puzzle based on De Bono's lateral thinking puzzles. The puzzle consisted of rotating and joining two-dimensional shapes to make a three-dimensional one. In one condition, participants were given the choice of either solving the puzzle mentally or through manipulation of the images on a computer screen. In another condition, the subjects had to solve the puzzle first mentally and then report which mode they would have preferred to solve the puzzle. Two more conditions were applied with slight variations. In all conditions, an overwhelming majority of participants chose to solve the puzzle by manipulation, even though there was not a significant increase on performance. It appeared that participants were making a conscious choice for the body to play a feedback-driven role in creative cognitive processing. This strong preference for manual manipulation over just mental representation, regardless of the impact on performance, would seem to suggest that it is our natural tendency to involve the body in complex cognitive functioning. This would support the theory that cognition may be more than just a neural process, and that it is a dynamic interplay between body, brain and world. The experiential feedback of the body moving through space and time may be an inherently important factor in creative cognition.
Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a puzzle based on De Bono’s lateral thinking puzzles. The puzzle consisted of rotating and joining two-dimensional shapes to make a three-dimensional one. In one condition, participants were given the choice of either solving the puzzle mentally or through manipulation of the images on a computer screen. In another condition, the subjects had to solve the puzzle first mentally and then report which mode they would have preferred to solve the puzzle. Two more conditions were applied with slight variations. In all conditions, an overwhelming majority of participants chose to solve the puzzle by manipulation, even though there was not a significant increase on performance. It appeared that participants were making a conscious choice for the body to play a feedback-driven role in creative cognitive processing. This strong preference for manual manipulation over just mental representation, regardless of the impact on performance, would seem to suggest that it is our natural tendency to involve the body in complex cognitive functioning. This would support the theory that cognition may be more than just a neural process, and that it is a dynamic interplay between body, brain and world. The experiential feedback of the body moving through space and time may be an inherently important factor in creative cognition.
Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a puzzle based on De Bono's lateral thinking puzzles. The puzzle consisted of rotating and joining two-dimensional shapes to make a three-dimensional one. In one condition, participants were given the choice of either solving the puzzle mentally or through manipulation of the images on a computer screen. In another condition, the subjects had to solve the puzzle first mentally and then report which mode they would have preferred to solve the puzzle. Two more conditions were applied with slight variations. In all conditions, an overwhelming majority of participants chose to solve the puzzle by manipulation, even though there was not a significant increase on performance. It appeared that participants were making a conscious choice for the body to play a feedback-driven role in creative cognitive processing. This strong preference for manual manipulation over just mental representation, regardless of the impact on performance, would seem to suggest that it is our natural tendency to involve the body in complex cognitive functioning. This would support the theory that cognition may be more than just a neural process, and that it is a dynamic interplay between body, brain and world. The experiential feedback of the body moving through space and time may be an inherently important factor in creative cognition.Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a puzzle based on De Bono's lateral thinking puzzles. The puzzle consisted of rotating and joining two-dimensional shapes to make a three-dimensional one. In one condition, participants were given the choice of either solving the puzzle mentally or through manipulation of the images on a computer screen. In another condition, the subjects had to solve the puzzle first mentally and then report which mode they would have preferred to solve the puzzle. Two more conditions were applied with slight variations. In all conditions, an overwhelming majority of participants chose to solve the puzzle by manipulation, even though there was not a significant increase on performance. It appeared that participants were making a conscious choice for the body to play a feedback-driven role in creative cognitive processing. This strong preference for manual manipulation over just mental representation, regardless of the impact on performance, would seem to suggest that it is our natural tendency to involve the body in complex cognitive functioning. This would support the theory that cognition may be more than just a neural process, and that it is a dynamic interplay between body, brain and world. The experiential feedback of the body moving through space and time may be an inherently important factor in creative cognition.
Author Norman, Stefanie
Deininger, Gina
Loudon, Gareth
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Gina
  surname: Deininger
  fullname: Deininger, Gina
  email: gideininger@cardiffmet.ac.uk
  organization: Cardiff Metropolitan University
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Gareth
  surname: Loudon
  fullname: Loudon, Gareth
  organization: Cardiff Metropolitan University
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Stefanie
  surname: Norman
  fullname: Norman, Stefanie
  organization: Ludwig Maximilian Universität München
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22806663$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkD1PwzAQhi1URD_gB7CgjiyBc5zY7ogqvqQiFpgt27lUqRKn2Ekl_j2uUjowlMk--3nv7GdKRq51SMg1hTsKIO4DBcYWCdA0gUwsEnZGJpTHKhYwOu5lOibTEDYA6QJYdkHGaSqBc84mJHlrC13Ptx5L9Ogshnnl5taj7qodxvPW1NjMQ1vvKre-JOelrgNeHdYZ-Xx6_Fi-JKv359flwyqxTIguQbBG5mUeH5ZJnTG0jBeyKABsyfOCF0xLwQ01Jc8MCiNFvDRWWgnCFBrYjNwOfeP8rx5Dp5oqWKxr7bDtg6I5yyRdZIz9jwLjsX9OZURvDmhvGizU1leN9t_q10YE6ABY34YQlRwRCmpvXA3GVfyY2htX-4z4k7FVF-W1rvO6qk8m0yEZ4hS3Rq82be9d9Hoi9AOdTJMg
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1111_hequ_12201
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tsc_2021_100811
crossref_primary_10_4236_jbbs_2016_61004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_sbspro_2015_02_012
crossref_primary_10_4236_jbbs_2017_73011
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2592354
Cites_doi 10.1080/03640210701704004
10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333213.001.0001
10.3758/BF03196322
10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.364
10.1159/000407161
10.7551/mitpress/2524.001.0001
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2012
Copyright_xml – notice: Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2012
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
DOI 10.1007/s10339-012-0479-3
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE

MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1612-4790
EndPage 150
ExternalDocumentID 22806663
10_1007_s10339_012_0479_3
Genre Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
-56
-5G
-BR
-EM
-Y2
-~C
.86
.VR
06C
06D
0R~
0VY
1N0
203
29F
29~
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2LR
2P1
2VQ
2~H
30V
4.4
406
408
409
40D
40E
53G
5GY
5VS
67N
67Z
6NX
8TC
8UJ
95-
95.
95~
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AACDK
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANXM
AANZL
AARHV
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
ABAKF
ABBBX
ABBXA
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTD
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABHQN
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABKCH
ABKTR
ABMNI
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABWNU
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACOKC
ACOMO
ACPIV
ACSNA
ACZOJ
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETLH
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFGCZ
AFLOW
AFQWF
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
AKMHD
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALWAN
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AZFZN
B-.
BA0
BDATZ
BGNMA
BSONS
CAG
COF
CS3
CSCUP
DDRTE
DL5
DNIVK
DPUIP
DU5
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
EN4
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FWDCC
G-Y
G-Z
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNWQR
GQ6
GQ7
GQ8
GXS
H13
HF~
HG5
HG6
HLICF
HMJXF
HQYDN
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
I09
IHE
IJ-
IKXTQ
IWAJR
IXC
IXD
IXE
IZIGR
IZQ
I~X
I~Z
J-C
J0Z
JBSCW
JCJTX
JZLTJ
KDC
KOV
KPH
LAS
LLZTM
M4Y
MA-
N2Q
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9I
O9J
OAM
PF0
PT4
QOR
QOS
R89
R9I
RNS
ROL
RPX
RSV
S16
S1Z
S27
S3A
S3B
SAP
SBL
SDH
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TSG
TSK
TSV
TUC
U2A
U9L
UG4
UOJIU
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
W23
W48
WJK
WK8
YLTOR
Z45
Z83
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
~A9
AAPKM
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
ADHKG
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFOHR
AGQPQ
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
ATHPR
AYFIA
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
ABRTQ
7TK
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c377t-e0cb85f501248a43ec36d8dd00cf65d6d3a876b1bf64be7b878ddbc8c807bda03
IEDL.DBID U2A
ISSN 1612-4782
1612-4790
IngestDate Fri Jul 11 01:59:20 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 10:54:26 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 01:52:38 EST 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:04:29 EDT 2025
Sun Jul 06 05:06:06 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:29:53 EST 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue Suppl 1
Keywords Play
Embodied cognition
Creative problem solving
Thinking
Language English
License http://www.springer.com/tdm
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c377t-e0cb85f501248a43ec36d8dd00cf65d6d3a876b1bf64be7b878ddbc8c807bda03
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ObjectType-Article-2
ObjectType-Feature-1
PMID 22806663
PQID 1036878518
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 4
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_1534819433
proquest_miscellaneous_1036878518
pubmed_primary_22806663
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10339_012_0479_3
crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s10339_012_0479_3
springer_journals_10_1007_s10339_012_0479_3
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2012-08-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2012-08-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 08
  year: 2012
  text: 2012-08-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace Berlin/Heidelberg
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Berlin/Heidelberg
– name: Germany
PublicationSubtitle International Quarterly of Cognitive Science
PublicationTitle Cognitive processing
PublicationTitleAbbrev Cogn Process
PublicationTitleAlternate Cogn Process
PublicationYear 2012
Publisher Springer-Verlag
Publisher_xml – name: Springer-Verlag
References PeplerDJPeplerDJRubinKHPlay and divergent thinkingThe play of children: current theory and research1982BaselKarger6478
SullivanGBiggsMKarlssonHArtistic cognition and creativityThe Routledge companion to research in the arts2011New YorkRoutledge107117
Merleau-PontyMPhenomenology of perception1962LondonRoutledge
ThelenESmithLBA dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action1994CambridgeMIT Press
BohmDPeatDScience, order and creativity1987New YorkBantam Books
Loudon GH, Deininger GM, Gordon BS (2012) Play, autonomy and the creative process. The 2nd international conference on design creativity (ICDC2012). Glasgow, UK, 18th–20th September 2012
SlovicPThe construction of preference. Award address May 1995Am Psychol19955036437110.1037/0003-066X.50.5.364
ClarkASupersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension2008New YorkOxford University Press
WilsonMSix views of embodied cognitionPsychon Bull Rev200296256361261367010.3758/BF03196322
HameroffSThe brain is both neurocomputer and quantum computerCogn Sci200731103510452163532810.1080/03640210701704004
MayerRESternbergRJFifty years of creativity researchHandbook of creativity1999CambridgeCambridge University Press169186
De BonoELateral thinking: a textbook of creativity1977EnglandPenguin Books
Clark A (1998) Embodied, situated, and distributed cognition. In: Bechtel W, Graham G (eds) A companion to cognitive science. Blackwell Publishers, Malden, pp 506–517
VarelaFJThompsonERoschEThe embodied mind. Cognitive science and human experience1991CambridgeMIT Press
NoeAOut of our heads: why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness2009New YorkHill and Wang
RE Mayer (479_CR7) 1999
D Bohm (479_CR2) 1987
E Thelen (479_CR13) 1994
A Noe (479_CR9) 2009
479_CR100
S Hameroff (479_CR5) 2007; 31
A Clark (479_CR3) 2008
E De Bono (479_CR4) 1977
DJ Pepler (479_CR10) 1982
G Sullivan (479_CR12) 2011
FJ Varela (479_CR14) 1991
479_CR6
P Slovic (479_CR11) 1995; 50
M Merleau-Ponty (479_CR8) 1962
M Wilson (479_CR15) 2002; 9
12613670 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Dec;9(4):625-36
21635328 - Cogn Sci. 2007 Nov 12;31(6):1035-45
References_xml – reference: VarelaFJThompsonERoschEThe embodied mind. Cognitive science and human experience1991CambridgeMIT Press
– reference: WilsonMSix views of embodied cognitionPsychon Bull Rev200296256361261367010.3758/BF03196322
– reference: PeplerDJPeplerDJRubinKHPlay and divergent thinkingThe play of children: current theory and research1982BaselKarger6478
– reference: HameroffSThe brain is both neurocomputer and quantum computerCogn Sci200731103510452163532810.1080/03640210701704004
– reference: Clark A (1998) Embodied, situated, and distributed cognition. In: Bechtel W, Graham G (eds) A companion to cognitive science. Blackwell Publishers, Malden, pp 506–517
– reference: NoeAOut of our heads: why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness2009New YorkHill and Wang
– reference: BohmDPeatDScience, order and creativity1987New YorkBantam Books
– reference: ThelenESmithLBA dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action1994CambridgeMIT Press
– reference: MayerRESternbergRJFifty years of creativity researchHandbook of creativity1999CambridgeCambridge University Press169186
– reference: ClarkASupersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension2008New YorkOxford University Press
– reference: Loudon GH, Deininger GM, Gordon BS (2012) Play, autonomy and the creative process. The 2nd international conference on design creativity (ICDC2012). Glasgow, UK, 18th–20th September 2012
– reference: SullivanGBiggsMKarlssonHArtistic cognition and creativityThe Routledge companion to research in the arts2011New YorkRoutledge107117
– reference: De BonoELateral thinking: a textbook of creativity1977EnglandPenguin Books
– reference: Merleau-PontyMPhenomenology of perception1962LondonRoutledge
– reference: SlovicPThe construction of preference. Award address May 1995Am Psychol19955036437110.1037/0003-066X.50.5.364
– volume: 31
  start-page: 1035
  year: 2007
  ident: 479_CR5
  publication-title: Cogn Sci
  doi: 10.1080/03640210701704004
– volume-title: The embodied mind. Cognitive science and human experience
  year: 1991
  ident: 479_CR14
  doi: 10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001
– volume-title: Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension
  year: 2008
  ident: 479_CR3
  doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333213.001.0001
– volume: 9
  start-page: 625
  year: 2002
  ident: 479_CR15
  publication-title: Psychon Bull Rev
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196322
– volume-title: Phenomenology of perception
  year: 1962
  ident: 479_CR8
– volume: 50
  start-page: 364
  year: 1995
  ident: 479_CR11
  publication-title: Am Psychol
  doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.364
– start-page: 107
  volume-title: The Routledge companion to research in the arts
  year: 2011
  ident: 479_CR12
– start-page: 64
  volume-title: The play of children: current theory and research
  year: 1982
  ident: 479_CR10
  doi: 10.1159/000407161
– volume-title: Lateral thinking: a textbook of creativity
  year: 1977
  ident: 479_CR4
– volume-title: Science, order and creativity
  year: 1987
  ident: 479_CR2
– start-page: 169
  volume-title: Handbook of creativity
  year: 1999
  ident: 479_CR7
– ident: 479_CR100
– ident: 479_CR6
– volume-title: Out of our heads: why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness
  year: 2009
  ident: 479_CR9
– volume-title: A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action
  year: 1994
  ident: 479_CR13
  doi: 10.7551/mitpress/2524.001.0001
– reference: 12613670 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Dec;9(4):625-36
– reference: 21635328 - Cogn Sci. 2007 Nov 12;31(6):1035-45
SSID ssj0029034
Score 1.9396436
Snippet Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to...
Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
springer
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 147
SubjectTerms Adult
Artificial Intelligence
Behavioral Sciences
Biomedical and Life Sciences
Biomedicine
Choice Behavior
Female
Humans
Male
Models, Psychological
Neuropsychological Tests
Neurosciences
Pattern Recognition, Visual
Photic Stimulation
Problem Solving
Short Report
Young Adult
Title Modal preferences in creative problem solving
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10339-012-0479-3
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22806663
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1036878518
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1534819433
Volume 13
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlR3LSgMxcND20ov4tj7KCp6UQLrJ7ibHIq1FqScL9bTkVRBkW2wr-PdO9qVSLXjObDJMJvPYeQFcaS2VlE6h9OMMHRTFiRLG4ItnNuaUGZkH2keP8XDM7yfRpKzjXlTZ7lVIMpfU34rdGPO5PSHxbdEJ24Zm5F13ZOJx2Ku9LEmLUDKqbsJR_1WhzN-2-KmM1izMtehornQGu7BTWotBr7jePdhy2T60aqH1cQBkNLMIMa_HhSyClywoTMF3F5TzYgLkMP_n4BDGg_7T7ZCUIxCIYUmyJI4aLaJphChzoThzhsVWWEupmcaRjS1TKM50V09jrl2iRYKL2ggjaKKtouwIGtkscycQONyAJaEKvTvMFZf4kpX1hbdKO-tkG2hFi9SU_cH9mIrX9KuzsSdfiriknnwpa8N1_cm8aI6xCfiyInCKLOzjEipzs9UC4VmMiEddsQEm8hXDkjPc57i4nfpI39EHvTBcuamuKy1f4uJvfE7_BX0GrTDnG5_6dw6N5dvKXaA5stQdaPbunh_6nZwNPwHPHtWU
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlR3LSgMxMGg92Iv4tj5X8KQE0k12NzkWsVRte2qht5BXQZBtcVvBv3eyL5VqwXNmk2EymcfOC6EbrYUSwimQfoyCg6IYVtwYePHUxoxQI_JA-2AY98bsaRJNyjrurMp2r0KSuaT-VuxGqc_tCbFvi47pJtoCW4D7PK5x2Km9LEGKUDKobsxA_1WhzN-2-KmMVizMlehornS6u2intBaDTnG9e2jDpfuoWQutjwOEBzMLEPN6XEgWvKRBYQq-u6CcFxMAh_k_B4do3H0Y3fdwOQIBG5okC-yI0TyaRoAy44pRZ2hsubWEmGkc2dhSBeJMt_U0ZtolmiewqA03nCTaKkKPUCOdpe4EBQ42oEmoQu8OM8UEvGRlfeGt0s460UKkooU0ZX9wP6biVX51Nvbkk4CL9OSTtIVu60_mRXOMdcDXFYElsLCPS6jUzZYZwNMYEI_afA1M5CuGBaOwz3FxO_WRvqMPeGGwclddlyxfYvY3Pqf_gr5C273RoC_7j8PnM9QMcx7yaYDnqLF4W7oLME0W-jJnxU9Q2dbz
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlR3ZSgMxMGgF6Yt4W88VfFKWppvsbvJY1FKPFh8s9C3kKgiyLe5W8O-d7KVSLfic2WSYTObYuRC6UIpLzq0E6UcJOCiS-pJpDS-emIhionkeaB8Mo_6I3o_DcTnnNK2y3auQZFHT4Lo0JVl7Zibtb4VvhLg8n8B3LdJ9sorWQBp3HFuPgm7tcXFchJVBjfsUdGEV1vxti5-KacHaXIiU5gqot4k2SsvR6xZXvYVWbLKNmrUA-9hB_mBqAGJWjw5JvZfEK8zCd-uVs2M84Db3F2EXjXq3z9d9vxyH4GsSx5lvsVYsnISAMmWSEqtJZJgxGOtJFJrIEAmiTXXUJKLKxorFsKg00wzHykhM9lAjmSb2AHkWNiBxIAPnGlNJObxqaVwRrlTWWN5CuKKF0GWvcDey4lV8dTl25BOAi3DkE6SFLutPZkWjjGXA5xWBBbCzi1HIxE7nKcCTCBAPO2wJTOiqhzklsM9-cTv1ka67D3hksHJVXZcoX2X6Nz6H_4I-Q-tPNz3xeDd8OELNIGchlxF4jBrZ29yegJWSqdOcEz8B_eDbLw
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Modal+preferences+in+creative+problem+solving&rft.jtitle=Cognitive+processing&rft.au=Deininger%2C+Gina&rft.au=Loudon%2C+Gareth&rft.au=Norman%2C+Stefanie&rft.date=2012-08-01&rft.issn=1612-4790&rft.eissn=1612-4790&rft.volume=13+Suppl+1&rft.spage=S147&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs10339-012-0479-3&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1612-4782&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1612-4782&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1612-4782&client=summon