Battling the coronavirus ‘infodemic’ among social media users in Kenya and Nigeria
How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya ( n = 7,498) and Nigeria ( n = 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing...
Saved in:
Published in | Nature human behaviour Vol. 8; no. 5; pp. 823 - 834 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
London
Nature Publishing Group UK
01.05.2024
Nature Publishing Group |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (
n
= 7,498) and Nigeria (
n
= 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing intentions to share factual posts. The initial stage of the study incorporated: (1) a factorial design with 40 intervention combinations; and (2) a contextual adaptive design, increasing the probability of assignment to treatments that worked better for previous subjects with similar characteristics. The second stage evaluated the best-performing treatments and a targeted treatment assignment policy estimated from the data. We precisely estimate null effects from warning flags and related article suggestions, tactics used by social media platforms. However, nudges to consider the accuracy of information reduced misinformation sharing relative to control by 4.9% (estimate = −2.3 percentage points, 95% CI = [−4.2, −0.35]). Such low-cost scalable interventions may improve the quality of information circulating online.
Which interventions limit the spread of COVID-19 misinformation online? In an experiment on Facebook Messenger in Kenya and Nigeria, nudges to consider an information’s accuracy worked best. |
---|---|
AbstractList | How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (n = 7,498) and Nigeria (n = 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing intentions to share factual posts. The initial stage of the study incorporated: (1) a factorial design with 40 intervention combinations; and (2) a contextual adaptive design, increasing the probability of assignment to treatments that worked better for previous subjects with similar characteristics. The second stage evaluated the best-performing treatments and a targeted treatment assignment policy estimated from the data. We precisely estimate null effects from warning flags and related article suggestions, tactics used by social media platforms. However, nudges to consider the accuracy of information reduced misinformation sharing relative to control by 4.9% (estimate = -2.3 percentage points, 95% CI = [-4.2, -0.35]). Such low-cost scalable interventions may improve the quality of information circulating online.How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (n = 7,498) and Nigeria (n = 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing intentions to share factual posts. The initial stage of the study incorporated: (1) a factorial design with 40 intervention combinations; and (2) a contextual adaptive design, increasing the probability of assignment to treatments that worked better for previous subjects with similar characteristics. The second stage evaluated the best-performing treatments and a targeted treatment assignment policy estimated from the data. We precisely estimate null effects from warning flags and related article suggestions, tactics used by social media platforms. However, nudges to consider the accuracy of information reduced misinformation sharing relative to control by 4.9% (estimate = -2.3 percentage points, 95% CI = [-4.2, -0.35]). Such low-cost scalable interventions may improve the quality of information circulating online. How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya ( n = 7,498) and Nigeria ( n = 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing intentions to share factual posts. The initial stage of the study incorporated: (1) a factorial design with 40 intervention combinations; and (2) a contextual adaptive design, increasing the probability of assignment to treatments that worked better for previous subjects with similar characteristics. The second stage evaluated the best-performing treatments and a targeted treatment assignment policy estimated from the data. We precisely estimate null effects from warning flags and related article suggestions, tactics used by social media platforms. However, nudges to consider the accuracy of information reduced misinformation sharing relative to control by 4.9% (estimate = −2.3 percentage points, 95% CI = [−4.2, −0.35]). Such low-cost scalable interventions may improve the quality of information circulating online. Which interventions limit the spread of COVID-19 misinformation online? In an experiment on Facebook Messenger in Kenya and Nigeria, nudges to consider an information’s accuracy worked best. How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (n = 7,498) and Nigeria (n = 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing intentions to share factual posts. The initial stage of the study incorporated: (1) a factorial design with 40 intervention combinations; and (2) a contextual adaptive design, increasing the probability of assignment to treatments that worked better for previous subjects with similar characteristics. The second stage evaluated the best-performing treatments and a targeted treatment assignment policy estimated from the data. We precisely estimate null effects from warning flags and related article suggestions, tactics used by social media platforms. However, nudges to consider the accuracy of information reduced misinformation sharing relative to control by 4.9% (estimate = -2.3 percentage points, 95% CI = [-4.2, -0.35]). Such low-cost scalable interventions may improve the quality of information circulating online. How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (n = 7,498) and Nigeria (n = 7,794) tested interventions designed to decrease intentions to share COVID-19 misinformation without decreasing intentions to share factual posts. The initial stage of the study incorporated: (1) a factorial design with 40 intervention combinations; and (2) a contextual adaptive design, increasing the probability of assignment to treatments that worked better for previous subjects with similar characteristics. The second stage evaluated the best-performing treatments and a targeted treatment assignment policy estimated from the data. We precisely estimate null effects from warning flags and related article suggestions, tactics used by social media platforms. However, nudges to consider the accuracy of information reduced misinformation sharing relative to control by 4.9% (estimate = −2.3 percentage points, 95% CI = [−4.2, −0.35]). Such low-cost scalable interventions may improve the quality of information circulating online.Which interventions limit the spread of COVID-19 misinformation online? In an experiment on Facebook Messenger in Kenya and Nigeria, nudges to consider an information’s accuracy worked best. |
Author | Offer-Westort, Molly Athey, Susan Rosenzweig, Leah R. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Molly orcidid: 0000-0003-2787-9919 surname: Offer-Westort fullname: Offer-Westort, Molly email: mollyow@uchicago.edu organization: Department of Political Science, University of Chicago – sequence: 2 givenname: Leah R. orcidid: 0000-0002-6043-6143 surname: Rosenzweig fullname: Rosenzweig, Leah R. organization: Development Innovation Lab, University of Chicago – sequence: 3 givenname: Susan surname: Athey fullname: Athey, Susan organization: Stanford Graduate School of Business, Stanford University |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38499773$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kc1OHSEYhonRqLXnBlwYkm7cTOVvBlhaU9vGE91Yt-Q7DHOKmQGFmSbuvAy9Pa-k6FFrXLgBFs8DH-_7Ca2HGBxCu5R8pYSrgyxo3bCKMF4Rqiip5BraZlzLinMp1t-ct9As50tCCNVcaNlsoi2uhNZS8m108Q3Gsfdhicc_DtuYYoC_Pk0ZP9ze-dDF1g3ePtzeYxhioXK0Hno8uNYDnrJLGfuAT1y4AQyhxad-6ZKHz2ijgz672fO-g34ffz8_-lnNz378OjqcV5bLeqxa3nYgOlFrVVPdOVrDogZJKTBVW0LqllupmJIdUVy2kjdCl7VrGQFtheA7aH9171WK15PLoxl8tq7vIbg4ZcN0ozSjoqEF_fIOvYxTCmU6w0lDtCj5qELtPVPTonzSXCU_QLoxL4kVgK0Am2LOyXWvCCXmsRmzasaUZsxTM0YWSb2TrB9h9DGMCXz_scpXai7vhBLu_7E_sP4BNDqh6A |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1017_S0003055424001394 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_71599_6 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0307090 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_024_01881_0 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/s11109-019-09533-0 10.1086/727605 10.1038/s41562-021-01056-1 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476494 10.1371/journal.pone.0197066 10.1038/s44159-021-00006-y 10.1371/journal.pone.0228882 10.1080/01621459.1994.10476818 10.37016/mr-2020-75 10.1038/s41467-022-30073-5 10.1093/restud/rdac065 10.1038/s41586-021-03344-2 10.1145/3447548.3467456 10.3982/ECTA17527 10.1002/poi3.214 10.1016/j.jdeveco.2023.103108 10.1073/pnas.1920498117 10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.006 10.1017/S0003055401002222 10.1177/19485506211000217 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 10.1017/pan.2017.27 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2012.00896.x 10.1111/jopy.12476 10.1007/s11109-010-9112-2 10.1080/02699931.2022.2090318 10.1186/s41235-020-00252-3 10.1111/jcom.12166 10.1017/S0003055421000459 10.1214/18-AOS1709 10.1177/1461444820969893 10.1037/xge0000729 10.1126/sciadv.abo6169 10.1371/journal.pone.0240005 10.1177/0956797620939054 10.48550/arXiv.1711.07077 10.1073/pnas.2020043118 10.48550/arXiv.2211.12004 10.1073/pnas.2104235118 10.2139/ssrn.3624510 10.1038/s41562-020-00994-6 10.1038/s41562-023-01641-6 10.1073/pnas.2216614120 10.31235/osf.io/dka8f 10.2139/ssrn.3689456 10.1287/mnsc.2023.4921 10.1027/1016-9040/a000492 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited 2024. Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law. 2024. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited 2024. Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law. – notice: 2024. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 0-V 3V. 7XB 88G 88J 8BJ 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AFKRA ALSLI AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO FQK FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ JBE M2M M2R PHGZM PHGZT PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PRQQA PSYQQ Q9U 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1038/s41562-023-01810-7 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】 ProQuest Central (Corporate) ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Psychology Database (Alumni) Social Science Database (Alumni Edition) International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) ProQuest Hospital Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea International Bibliography of the Social Sciences Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student International Bibliography of the Social Sciences Psychology Database Social Science Database ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Basic MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central Student ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central Health Research Premium Collection International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Central (New) Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni) ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Social Science Journals ProQuest Psychology Journals ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE ProQuest One Psychology |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 2397-3374 |
EndPage | 834 |
ExternalDocumentID | 38499773 10_1038_s41562_023_01810_7 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | Nigeria Kenya |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: Kenya – name: Nigeria |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: We received advertising credits for this study from Facebook Health and funding from the Golub Capital Social Impact Lab. |
GroupedDBID | 0R~ 53G 8FI 8FJ AAEEF AARCD AAYZH ABJNI ABLJU ABUWG ACGFS ADBBV AFKRA AFSHS AFWHJ AHSBF AIBTJ ALFFA ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ARMCB AXYYD AZQEC BENPR BKKNO CCPQU DWQXO EBS EJD FSGXE FYUFA FZEXT GNUQQ M2M M2R NNMJJ O9- ODYON PQQKQ PSYQQ RNT SHXYY SIXXV SNYQT SOJ TAOOD TBHMF TDRGL TSG UKHRP AAYXX ACBWK AFANA ATHPR CITATION NFIDA PHGZM PHGZT CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PRQQA 0-V 3V. 7XB 8BJ 8FK FQK JBE PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQUKI PRINS Q9U 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-d3dfa4f4598519fe15ab5a711a285c005d3c78287f0837d73649d73fd20a9c443 |
IEDL.DBID | BENPR |
ISSN | 2397-3374 |
IngestDate | Thu Jul 10 23:13:59 EDT 2025 Sat Aug 23 12:27:05 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:04:35 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:09:52 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:00:34 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:39:24 EST 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 5 |
Language | English |
License | 2024. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c375t-d3dfa4f4598519fe15ab5a711a285c005d3c78287f0837d73649d73fd20a9c443 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-2787-9919 0000-0002-6043-6143 |
PMID | 38499773 |
PQID | 3060940198 |
PQPubID | 4560800 |
PageCount | 12 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2968921461 proquest_journals_3060940198 pubmed_primary_38499773 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_023_01810_7 crossref_citationtrail_10_1038_s41562_023_01810_7 springer_journals_10_1038_s41562_023_01810_7 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2024-05-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2024-05-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 05 year: 2024 text: 2024-05-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | London |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: London – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Nature human behaviour |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Nat Hum Behav |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Nat Hum Behav |
PublicationYear | 2024 |
Publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK Nature Publishing Group |
Publisher_xml | – name: Nature Publishing Group UK – name: Nature Publishing Group |
References | Bago, B., Rand, D. G. & Pennycook, G. Fake news, fast and slow: deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. J. Exp. Psychol.149, 1608–1613 (2020). RobinsJMRotnitzkyASemiparametric efficiency in multivariate regression models with missing dataJ. Am. Stat. Assoc.19959012212910.1080/01621459.1995.10476494 GrossJJThe emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative reviewRev. Gen. Psychol.1998227129910.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 Swire-ThompsonBDeGutisJLazerDSearching for the backfire effect: measurement and design considerationsJ. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn.2020928629910.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.006329050237462781 KasyMSautmannAAdaptive treatment assignment in experiments for policy choiceEconometrica20218911313210.3982/ECTA17527 PennycookGShifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation onlineNature20215925905951:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXmslagtro%3D10.1038/s41586-021-03344-233731933 Tibshirani, J., Athey, S. & Wager, S. grf: Generalized Random Forests. R package version 2.3.0 (2020). PennycookGRandDGWho falls for fake news? the roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity and analytic thinkingJ. Personal.20208818520010.1111/jopy.12476 BodeLVragaEKIn related news, that was wrong: the correction of misinformation through related stories functionality in social mediaJ. Commun.20156561963810.1111/jcom.12166 Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Accuracy prompts are a replicable and generalizable approach for reducing the spread of misinformation. Nat. Commun.13, 2333 (2022). RobinsJMRotnitzkyAZhaoLPEstimation of regression coefficients when some regressors are not always observedJ. Am. Stat. Assoc.19948984686610.1080/01621459.1994.10476818 Bago, B., Rosenzweig, L. R., Berinsky, A. J. & Rand, D. G. Emotion may predict susceptibility to fake news but emotion regulation does not seem to help. Cogn. Emot.36, 1166–1180 (2022). World Population Review (Facebook, 2022); worldpopulationreview.com/country-rankings/facebook-users-by-country Rosenzweig, L. R., Bergquist, P., Hoffmann Pham, K., Rampazzo, F. & Mildenberger, M. Survey sampling in the global south using Facebook advertisements. Preprint at SocArXivhttps://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/dka8f (2020). Haghdoost, Y. Alcohol poisoning kills 100 Iranians seeking virus protection. Bloomberg Markets (18 March 2020); www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2020-03-18/alcohol-poisoning-kills-100-iranians-seeking-virus-protection AliAQaziIACountering misinformation on social media through educational interventions: evidence from a randomized experiment in PakistanJ. Dev. Econ.202316310310810.1016/j.jdeveco.2023.103108 LoombaSde FigueiredoAPiatekSJde GraafKLarsonHJMeasuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USANat. Hum. Behav.2021533734810.1038/s41562-021-01056-133547453 Martel, C., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic.5, 47 (2020). Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Zhang, Y., Lu, J. G. & Rand, D. G. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychol. Sci.31, 770–780 (2020). EckerUKThe psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correctionNat. Rev. Psychol.20221132910.1038/s44159-021-00006-y Even-Dar, E., Mannor, S., Mansour, Y. & Mahadevan, S. Action elimination and stopping conditions for the multi-armed bandit and reinforcement learning problems. J. Mach. Learn. Res.7, 1079–1105 (2006). Roozenbeek, J., Suiter, J. & Culloty, E. Countering misinformation: evidence, knowledge gaps and implications of current interventions. Eur. Psychol.https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000492 (2023). Athey, S. et al. Contextual bandits in a survey experiment on charitable giving: within-experiment outcomes versus policy learning. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2211.12004 (2022). BroockmanDEKallaJLSekhonJSThe design of field experiments with survey outcomes: a framework for selecting more efficient, robust and ethical designsPolit. Anal.20172543546410.1017/pan.2017.27 PorterEWoodTJThe global effectiveness of fact-checking: evidence from simultaneous experiments in Argentina, Nigeria, South Africa and the United KingdomProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2021118e21042351181:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXitVCnu7bJ10.1073/pnas.2104235118345079968449384 Zhan, R., Hadad, V., Hirshberg, D. A. & Athey, S. Off-policy evaluation via adaptive weighting with data from contextual bandits. in Proc. 27th ACM SIGKDD Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining (KDD '21) 2125–2135 (2021). Meixler, E. Facebook is dropping its fake news red flag warning after finding it had the opposite effect. TIME (22 December 2017); time.com/5077002/facebook-fake-news-articles Rosenzweig, L. R., Bago, B., Berinsky, A. J. & Rand, D. G. Happiness and surprise are associated with worse truth discernment of COVID-19 headlines among social media users in Nigeria. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review (10 August 2021); misinforeview.hks.harvard.edu/article/happiness-and-surprise-are-associated-with-worse-truth-discernment-of-covid-19-headlines-among-social-media-users-in-nigeria Bursztyn, L., Rao, A., Roth, C. & Yanagizawa-Drott, D. Opinions as Facts (ECONtribute, 2022). Arechar, A. A. et al. Understanding and combatting misinformation across 16 countries on six continents. Nat. Hum. Behav.7, 1502–1513 (2023). PummererLConspiracy theories and their societal effects during the COVID-19 pandemicSoc. Psychol. Personal. Sci.202213495910.1177/19485506211000217 AtheySTibshiraniJWagerSGeneralized random forestsAnn. Stat.2019471148117810.1214/18-AOS1709 Top 10 African countries with the most Facebook users. ITNews Africa (2016); www.howwe.ug/news/lifestyle/14791/top-10-countries-with-the-most-facebook-users-in-africa DimakopoulouMZhouZAtheySImbensGBalanced linear contextual banditsProc. AAAI Conf. Artif. Intell.20193334453453 NyhanBReiflerJWhen corrections fail: the persistence of political misperceptionsPolit. Behav.20103230333010.1007/s11109-010-9112-2 CostaMSchaffnerBFPrevostAWalking the walk? Experiments on the effect of pledging to vote on youth turnoutPLoS ONE201813e019706610.1371/journal.pone.0197066298130755973556 ClaytonKReal solutions for fake news? Measuring the effectiveness of general warnings and fact-check tags in reducing belief in false stories on social mediaPolit. Behav.2020421073109510.1007/s11109-019-09533-0 Ghosh, S. Facebook will show people anti-fake news articles when they post false stories. Insider.com (3 August 2017); www.insider.com/facebook-related-articles-feature-will-show-you-anti-fake-news-2017-8 BowlesJLarreguyHLiuSCountering misinformation via Whatsapp: preliminary evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic in ZimbabwePLoS ONE202015e02400051:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXitFelt7zP10.1371/journal.pone.0240005330529677556529 EpsteinZSirlinNArecharAPennycookGRandDThe social media context interferes with truth discernmentSci. Adv.20239eabo616910.1126/sciadv.abo6169368677049984169 BadrinathanSEducative interventions to combat misinformation: evidence from a field experiment in IndiaAm. Polit. Sci. Rev.20211151325134110.1017/S0003055421000459 Kreps, S. E. & Kriner, D. Medical Misinformation in the COVID-19 Pandemic (SSRN, 2020). CeylanGandersonIAWoodWSharing of misinformation is habitual, not just lazy or biasedProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2023120e22166141201:CAS:528:DC%2BB3sXivFShsbg%3D10.1073/pnas.2216614120366494149942822 GallottiRValleFCastaldoNSaccoPDe DomenicoMAssessing the risks of ‘infodemics’ in response to COVID-19 epidemicsNat. Hum. Behav.202041285129310.1038/s41562-020-00994-633122812 Yadlowsky, S., Fleming, S., Shah, N., Brunskill, E. & Wager, S. Evaluating treatment prioritization rules via rank-weighted average treatment effects. Preprint at arxiv.org/abs/2111.07966 (2021). GuessAMA digital media literacy intervention increases discernment between mainstream and false news in the United States and IndiaProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2020117155361554510.1073/pnas.1920498117325719507355018 Goldstein, J. A., Grossman, S. & Startz, M. Belief in COVID-19 misinformation in Nigeria. J. Polit.https://doi.org/10.1086/727605 Dimakopoulou, M., Athey, S. & Imbens, G. Estimation considerations in contextual bandits. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1711.07077 (2017). AltaySHacquinA-SMercierHWhy do so few people share fake news? It hurts their reputationN. Media Soc.2022241303132410.1177/1461444820969893 Caria, S. et al. An Adaptive Targeted Field Experiment: Job Search Assistance for Refugees in Jordan (CESifo, 2020). MoslehMPennycookGRandDGSelf-reported willingness to share political news articles in online surveys correlates with actual sharing on TwitterPLoS ONE202015e02288821:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXjtVCrurw%3D10.1371/journal.pone.0228882320405397010247 Zhan, R., Ren, Z., Athey, S. & Zhou, Z. Policy learning with adaptively collected data. Manage. Sci.https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2023.4921 (2023). Gilens, M. Political ignorance and collective policy preferences. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev.95, 379–396 (2001). MenaPCleaning up social media: the effect of warning labels on likelihood of sharing false news on FacebookPolicy Internet20201216518310.1002/poi3.214 CotterillSJohnPRichardsonLThe impact of a pledge request and the promise of publicity: a randomized controlled trial of charitable donationsSoc. Sci. Q.20139420021610.1111/j.1540-6237.2012.00896.x BrashierNMPennycookGBerinskyAJRandDGTiming matters when correcting fake newsProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2021118e20200431181:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXjtV2lsrw%3D10.1073/pnas.2020043118334953367865139 1810_CR39 S Altay (1810_CR41) 2022; 24 K Clayton (1810_CR17) 2020; 42 1810_CR35 M Dimakopoulou (1810_CR30) 2019; 33 1810_CR4 1810_CR32 1810_CR31 1810_CR6 J Bowles (1810_CR22) 2020; 15 R Gallotti (1810_CR12) 2020; 4 S Badrinathan (1810_CR24) 2021; 115 1810_CR1 1810_CR3 B Swire-Thompson (1810_CR2) 2020; 9 B Nyhan (1810_CR37) 2010; 32 A Ali (1810_CR25) 2023; 163 1810_CR29 1810_CR27 1810_CR26 M Costa (1810_CR53) 2018; 13 NM Brashier (1810_CR21) 2021; 118 1810_CR23 JM Robins (1810_CR34) 1995; 90 L Bode (1810_CR36) 2015; 65 S Athey (1810_CR33) 2019; 47 E Porter (1810_CR20) 2021; 118 G Pennycook (1810_CR7) 2021; 592 G Ceylan (1810_CR9) 2023; 120 L Pummerer (1810_CR14) 2022; 13 UK Ecker (1810_CR16) 2022; 1 1810_CR13 1810_CR56 1810_CR11 1810_CR52 1810_CR51 1810_CR50 G Pennycook (1810_CR10) 2020; 88 S Loomba (1810_CR15) 2021; 5 1810_CR19 M Mosleh (1810_CR42) 2020; 15 1810_CR49 AM Guess (1810_CR8) 2020; 117 1810_CR48 1810_CR47 S Cotterill (1810_CR54) 2013; 94 1810_CR45 1810_CR44 1810_CR43 P Mena (1810_CR18) 2020; 12 M Kasy (1810_CR5) 2021; 89 1810_CR40 Z Epstein (1810_CR38) 2023; 9 JJ Gross (1810_CR55) 1998; 2 DE Broockman (1810_CR28) 2017; 25 JM Robins (1810_CR46) 1994; 89 |
References_xml | – reference: GuessAMA digital media literacy intervention increases discernment between mainstream and false news in the United States and IndiaProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2020117155361554510.1073/pnas.1920498117325719507355018 – reference: Bursztyn, L., Rao, A., Roth, C. & Yanagizawa-Drott, D. Opinions as Facts (ECONtribute, 2022). – reference: MoslehMPennycookGRandDGSelf-reported willingness to share political news articles in online surveys correlates with actual sharing on TwitterPLoS ONE202015e02288821:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXjtVCrurw%3D10.1371/journal.pone.0228882320405397010247 – reference: PorterEWoodTJThe global effectiveness of fact-checking: evidence from simultaneous experiments in Argentina, Nigeria, South Africa and the United KingdomProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2021118e21042351181:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXitVCnu7bJ10.1073/pnas.2104235118345079968449384 – reference: Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Zhang, Y., Lu, J. G. & Rand, D. G. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychol. Sci.31, 770–780 (2020). – reference: MenaPCleaning up social media: the effect of warning labels on likelihood of sharing false news on FacebookPolicy Internet20201216518310.1002/poi3.214 – reference: CostaMSchaffnerBFPrevostAWalking the walk? Experiments on the effect of pledging to vote on youth turnoutPLoS ONE201813e019706610.1371/journal.pone.0197066298130755973556 – reference: Even-Dar, E., Mannor, S., Mansour, Y. & Mahadevan, S. Action elimination and stopping conditions for the multi-armed bandit and reinforcement learning problems. J. Mach. Learn. Res.7, 1079–1105 (2006). – reference: Top 10 African countries with the most Facebook users. ITNews Africa (2016); www.howwe.ug/news/lifestyle/14791/top-10-countries-with-the-most-facebook-users-in-africa – reference: Kreps, S. E. & Kriner, D. Medical Misinformation in the COVID-19 Pandemic (SSRN, 2020). – reference: Yadlowsky, S., Fleming, S., Shah, N., Brunskill, E. & Wager, S. Evaluating treatment prioritization rules via rank-weighted average treatment effects. Preprint at arxiv.org/abs/2111.07966 (2021). – reference: EckerUKThe psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correctionNat. Rev. Psychol.20221132910.1038/s44159-021-00006-y – reference: Rosenzweig, L. R., Bergquist, P., Hoffmann Pham, K., Rampazzo, F. & Mildenberger, M. Survey sampling in the global south using Facebook advertisements. Preprint at SocArXivhttps://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/dka8f (2020). – reference: BodeLVragaEKIn related news, that was wrong: the correction of misinformation through related stories functionality in social mediaJ. Commun.20156561963810.1111/jcom.12166 – reference: Bago, B., Rosenzweig, L. R., Berinsky, A. J. & Rand, D. G. Emotion may predict susceptibility to fake news but emotion regulation does not seem to help. Cogn. Emot.36, 1166–1180 (2022). – reference: BadrinathanSEducative interventions to combat misinformation: evidence from a field experiment in IndiaAm. Polit. Sci. Rev.20211151325134110.1017/S0003055421000459 – reference: Roozenbeek, J., Suiter, J. & Culloty, E. Countering misinformation: evidence, knowledge gaps and implications of current interventions. Eur. Psychol.https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000492 (2023). – reference: PennycookGRandDGWho falls for fake news? the roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity and analytic thinkingJ. Personal.20208818520010.1111/jopy.12476 – reference: NyhanBReiflerJWhen corrections fail: the persistence of political misperceptionsPolit. Behav.20103230333010.1007/s11109-010-9112-2 – reference: Caria, S. et al. An Adaptive Targeted Field Experiment: Job Search Assistance for Refugees in Jordan (CESifo, 2020). – reference: Bago, B., Rand, D. G. & Pennycook, G. Fake news, fast and slow: deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. J. Exp. Psychol.149, 1608–1613 (2020). – reference: GallottiRValleFCastaldoNSaccoPDe DomenicoMAssessing the risks of ‘infodemics’ in response to COVID-19 epidemicsNat. Hum. Behav.202041285129310.1038/s41562-020-00994-633122812 – reference: GrossJJThe emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative reviewRev. Gen. Psychol.1998227129910.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 – reference: Tibshirani, J., Athey, S. & Wager, S. grf: Generalized Random Forests. R package version 2.3.0 (2020). – reference: Ghosh, S. Facebook will show people anti-fake news articles when they post false stories. Insider.com (3 August 2017); www.insider.com/facebook-related-articles-feature-will-show-you-anti-fake-news-2017-8 – reference: World Population Review (Facebook, 2022); worldpopulationreview.com/country-rankings/facebook-users-by-country – reference: Athey, S. et al. Contextual bandits in a survey experiment on charitable giving: within-experiment outcomes versus policy learning. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2211.12004 (2022). – reference: RobinsJMRotnitzkyASemiparametric efficiency in multivariate regression models with missing dataJ. Am. Stat. Assoc.19959012212910.1080/01621459.1995.10476494 – reference: AtheySTibshiraniJWagerSGeneralized random forestsAnn. Stat.2019471148117810.1214/18-AOS1709 – reference: EpsteinZSirlinNArecharAPennycookGRandDThe social media context interferes with truth discernmentSci. Adv.20239eabo616910.1126/sciadv.abo6169368677049984169 – reference: Arechar, A. A. et al. Understanding and combatting misinformation across 16 countries on six continents. Nat. Hum. Behav.7, 1502–1513 (2023). – reference: Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Accuracy prompts are a replicable and generalizable approach for reducing the spread of misinformation. Nat. Commun.13, 2333 (2022). – reference: KasyMSautmannAAdaptive treatment assignment in experiments for policy choiceEconometrica20218911313210.3982/ECTA17527 – reference: Haghdoost, Y. Alcohol poisoning kills 100 Iranians seeking virus protection. Bloomberg Markets (18 March 2020); www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2020-03-18/alcohol-poisoning-kills-100-iranians-seeking-virus-protection – reference: BrashierNMPennycookGBerinskyAJRandDGTiming matters when correcting fake newsProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2021118e20200431181:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXjtV2lsrw%3D10.1073/pnas.2020043118334953367865139 – reference: DimakopoulouMZhouZAtheySImbensGBalanced linear contextual banditsProc. AAAI Conf. Artif. Intell.20193334453453 – reference: PummererLConspiracy theories and their societal effects during the COVID-19 pandemicSoc. Psychol. Personal. Sci.202213495910.1177/19485506211000217 – reference: AliAQaziIACountering misinformation on social media through educational interventions: evidence from a randomized experiment in PakistanJ. Dev. Econ.202316310310810.1016/j.jdeveco.2023.103108 – reference: Dimakopoulou, M., Athey, S. & Imbens, G. Estimation considerations in contextual bandits. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1711.07077 (2017). – reference: AltaySHacquinA-SMercierHWhy do so few people share fake news? It hurts their reputationN. Media Soc.2022241303132410.1177/1461444820969893 – reference: CotterillSJohnPRichardsonLThe impact of a pledge request and the promise of publicity: a randomized controlled trial of charitable donationsSoc. Sci. Q.20139420021610.1111/j.1540-6237.2012.00896.x – reference: Rosenzweig, L. R., Bago, B., Berinsky, A. J. & Rand, D. G. Happiness and surprise are associated with worse truth discernment of COVID-19 headlines among social media users in Nigeria. Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review (10 August 2021); misinforeview.hks.harvard.edu/article/happiness-and-surprise-are-associated-with-worse-truth-discernment-of-covid-19-headlines-among-social-media-users-in-nigeria/ – reference: BowlesJLarreguyHLiuSCountering misinformation via Whatsapp: preliminary evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic in ZimbabwePLoS ONE202015e02400051:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXitFelt7zP10.1371/journal.pone.0240005330529677556529 – reference: Zhan, R., Hadad, V., Hirshberg, D. A. & Athey, S. Off-policy evaluation via adaptive weighting with data from contextual bandits. in Proc. 27th ACM SIGKDD Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining (KDD '21) 2125–2135 (2021). – reference: Martel, C., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic.5, 47 (2020). – reference: PennycookGShifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation onlineNature20215925905951:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXmslagtro%3D10.1038/s41586-021-03344-233731933 – reference: Zhan, R., Ren, Z., Athey, S. & Zhou, Z. Policy learning with adaptively collected data. Manage. Sci.https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2023.4921 (2023). – reference: Swire-ThompsonBDeGutisJLazerDSearching for the backfire effect: measurement and design considerationsJ. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn.2020928629910.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.006329050237462781 – reference: RobinsJMRotnitzkyAZhaoLPEstimation of regression coefficients when some regressors are not always observedJ. Am. Stat. Assoc.19948984686610.1080/01621459.1994.10476818 – reference: LoombaSde FigueiredoAPiatekSJde GraafKLarsonHJMeasuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USANat. Hum. Behav.2021533734810.1038/s41562-021-01056-133547453 – reference: Goldstein, J. A., Grossman, S. & Startz, M. Belief in COVID-19 misinformation in Nigeria. J. Polit.https://doi.org/10.1086/727605 – reference: BroockmanDEKallaJLSekhonJSThe design of field experiments with survey outcomes: a framework for selecting more efficient, robust and ethical designsPolit. Anal.20172543546410.1017/pan.2017.27 – reference: ClaytonKReal solutions for fake news? Measuring the effectiveness of general warnings and fact-check tags in reducing belief in false stories on social mediaPolit. Behav.2020421073109510.1007/s11109-019-09533-0 – reference: Meixler, E. Facebook is dropping its fake news red flag warning after finding it had the opposite effect. TIME (22 December 2017); time.com/5077002/facebook-fake-news-articles/ – reference: Gilens, M. Political ignorance and collective policy preferences. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev.95, 379–396 (2001). – reference: CeylanGandersonIAWoodWSharing of misinformation is habitual, not just lazy or biasedProc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA2023120e22166141201:CAS:528:DC%2BB3sXivFShsbg%3D10.1073/pnas.2216614120366494149942822 – volume: 42 start-page: 1073 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR17 publication-title: Polit. Behav. doi: 10.1007/s11109-019-09533-0 – ident: 1810_CR23 doi: 10.1086/727605 – volume: 5 start-page: 337 year: 2021 ident: 1810_CR15 publication-title: Nat. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01056-1 – volume: 90 start-page: 122 year: 1995 ident: 1810_CR34 publication-title: J. Am. Stat. Assoc. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476494 – volume: 13 start-page: e0197066 year: 2018 ident: 1810_CR53 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197066 – volume: 1 start-page: 13 year: 2022 ident: 1810_CR16 publication-title: Nat. Rev. Psychol. doi: 10.1038/s44159-021-00006-y – volume: 15 start-page: e0228882 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR42 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228882 – volume: 89 start-page: 846 year: 1994 ident: 1810_CR46 publication-title: J. Am. Stat. Assoc. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1994.10476818 – ident: 1810_CR50 doi: 10.37016/mr-2020-75 – ident: 1810_CR35 – ident: 1810_CR40 doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30073-5 – ident: 1810_CR13 doi: 10.1093/restud/rdac065 – volume: 592 start-page: 590 year: 2021 ident: 1810_CR7 publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03344-2 – ident: 1810_CR32 doi: 10.1145/3447548.3467456 – volume: 33 start-page: 3445 year: 2019 ident: 1810_CR30 publication-title: Proc. AAAI Conf. Artif. Intell. – volume: 89 start-page: 113 year: 2021 ident: 1810_CR5 publication-title: Econometrica doi: 10.3982/ECTA17527 – volume: 12 start-page: 165 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR18 publication-title: Policy Internet doi: 10.1002/poi3.214 – ident: 1810_CR45 – volume: 163 start-page: 103108 year: 2023 ident: 1810_CR25 publication-title: J. Dev. Econ. doi: 10.1016/j.jdeveco.2023.103108 – volume: 117 start-page: 15536 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR8 publication-title: Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.1920498117 – volume: 9 start-page: 286 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR2 publication-title: J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.06.006 – ident: 1810_CR48 doi: 10.1017/S0003055401002222 – volume: 13 start-page: 49 year: 2022 ident: 1810_CR14 publication-title: Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. doi: 10.1177/19485506211000217 – volume: 2 start-page: 271 year: 1998 ident: 1810_CR55 publication-title: Rev. Gen. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 – ident: 1810_CR3 – volume: 25 start-page: 435 year: 2017 ident: 1810_CR28 publication-title: Polit. Anal. doi: 10.1017/pan.2017.27 – volume: 94 start-page: 200 year: 2013 ident: 1810_CR54 publication-title: Soc. Sci. Q. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2012.00896.x – volume: 88 start-page: 185 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR10 publication-title: J. Personal. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12476 – volume: 32 start-page: 303 year: 2010 ident: 1810_CR37 publication-title: Polit. Behav. doi: 10.1007/s11109-010-9112-2 – ident: 1810_CR51 doi: 10.1080/02699931.2022.2090318 – ident: 1810_CR49 doi: 10.1186/s41235-020-00252-3 – volume: 65 start-page: 619 year: 2015 ident: 1810_CR36 publication-title: J. Commun. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12166 – volume: 115 start-page: 1325 year: 2021 ident: 1810_CR24 publication-title: Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. doi: 10.1017/S0003055421000459 – volume: 47 start-page: 1148 year: 2019 ident: 1810_CR33 publication-title: Ann. Stat. doi: 10.1214/18-AOS1709 – volume: 24 start-page: 1303 year: 2022 ident: 1810_CR41 publication-title: N. Media Soc. doi: 10.1177/1461444820969893 – ident: 1810_CR52 doi: 10.1037/xge0000729 – volume: 9 start-page: eabo6169 year: 2023 ident: 1810_CR38 publication-title: Sci. Adv. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abo6169 – ident: 1810_CR56 – volume: 15 start-page: e0240005 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR22 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240005 – ident: 1810_CR44 doi: 10.1177/0956797620939054 – ident: 1810_CR29 doi: 10.48550/arXiv.1711.07077 – volume: 118 start-page: e2020043118 year: 2021 ident: 1810_CR21 publication-title: Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.2020043118 – ident: 1810_CR6 doi: 10.48550/arXiv.2211.12004 – ident: 1810_CR43 – volume: 118 start-page: e2104235118 year: 2021 ident: 1810_CR20 publication-title: Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.2104235118 – ident: 1810_CR47 – ident: 1810_CR19 doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3624510 – volume: 4 start-page: 1285 year: 2020 ident: 1810_CR12 publication-title: Nat. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00994-6 – ident: 1810_CR11 – ident: 1810_CR39 doi: 10.1038/s41562-023-01641-6 – ident: 1810_CR1 – volume: 120 start-page: e2216614120 year: 2023 ident: 1810_CR9 publication-title: Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.2216614120 – ident: 1810_CR27 doi: 10.31235/osf.io/dka8f – ident: 1810_CR4 doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3689456 – ident: 1810_CR31 doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2023.4921 – ident: 1810_CR26 doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000492 |
SSID | ssj0001934976 |
Score | 2.3397696 |
Snippet | How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (
n... How can we induce social media users to be discerning when sharing information during a pandemic? An experiment on Facebook Messenger with users from Kenya (n... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 823 |
SubjectTerms | 4014/4013 4014/4045 706/134 Accuracy Adaptive designs Adult Behavioral Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences Communication Coronaviruses COVID-19 COVID-19 - prevention & control Experimental Psychology False information Female Humans Information Dissemination - methods Intention Intervention Kenya Life Sciences Male Microeconomics Misinformation Neurosciences Nigeria Pandemics Personality and Social Psychology Social Media Social networks Tactics Young Adult |
Title | Battling the coronavirus ‘infodemic’ among social media users in Kenya and Nigeria |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.1038/s41562-023-01810-7 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38499773 https://www.proquest.com/docview/3060940198 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2968921461 |
Volume | 8 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3NThsxEB6VcOGCgBZYSCtX6q1dkV3bsX2qaJUIITWqEFTcLK-9iyKhTWATJG48BrweT9Kx10mEEFxy2OzPaGY883lmPAPwTfSMy0zuUnSuRcrKLEsV9UWOXBRGlijx0GLjz6h_csFOL_llDLg1saxyYRODoXYT62PkRwhtfas33CP_nN6kfmqUz67GERprsI4mWOLma_3XYPT3bBVlUZShw42nZXpUHjV-x4K05L6MSKIREi890iuY-SpFGjzPcAs2I2Qkx62Mt-FDWe_AxtJy3X-Ef75Lpj9XThDOEeubEpi78e28Ic8Pj16FQgn888MTCbOFSBsoJ-HUCPFhioaMa4I2994QUzsyGl95xfwEF8PB-e-TNA5MSC0VfJY66irDKsYV4ihVlRk3BTciQ2FIbnG9OWqF73BfIfASTtA-U_hbubxnlGWM7kKnntTlPpCMS7yW2UIZxRBUKmO4KAthqWSG91gC2YJp2sZu4n6oxbUOWW0qdctojYzWgdFaJPB9-cy07aXx7t3dhSx0XFeNXmlBAl-Xf-OK8GkOU5eTeaNz1ZcqzCtPYK-V4fJzSL1CxEsT-LEQ6urlb9Ny8D4th7CRI9Zp6yC70JndzsvPiFVmxZeokP8BOIjlFw |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3LTtwwFL2iw6Jsqpa-Qim4UrtqIyaxPY4XVUV5aCgwqiqo2BnHdqqRqgyQmVaz4zPan-Cj-JJeO8mMECo7NlnkeXNfPrbvA-Ct6Gqb6NTGOLjmMXNJEkvqgxy5yHXmUOKhxMbhoNc_Zl9O-MkCXLW5MD6ssvWJwVHbkfFr5BsIbX2pN5wjfzo7j33XKL-72rbQqNVi301_45St-ri3jfJ9l6a7O0db_bjpKhAbKvg4ttQWmhWMSwQbsnAJ1znXIkGKM25QKS01wpeBLxCdCCtoj0k8FjbtamkYo_jeB7DIKE5lOrD4eWfw9dt8VUdShgN8k53TpdlG5WdI-O-pD1vK0OmJmyPgLVh7a0s2jHS7j-FRA1HJZq1TT2DBlcuwNPOU06fw3Vfl9HnsBOEjMb4Igv41vJhU5Pryj1fZEHJ_ffmXhF5GpF6YJyFLhfhlkYoMS4I-fqqJLi0ZDH94Q3gGx_fCyufQKUelewkk4RmeS0wutWQIYqXWXLhcGJoxzbssgqRlmjJN9XLfROOnCrvoNFM1oxUyWgVGKxHB-9kzZ3XtjjvvXm1loRo7rtRc6yJ4M7uMFui3VXTpRpNKpbKXydAfPYIXtQxnn0PqJSJsGsGHVqjzl_-flpW7aVmHh_2jwwN1sDfYfwVLKeKsOgZzFTrji4l7jThpnK81ykng9L7t4R-SYCDJ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Battling+the+coronavirus+%27infodemic%27+among+social+media+users+in+Kenya+and+Nigeria&rft.jtitle=Nature+human+behaviour&rft.au=Offer-Westort%2C+Molly&rft.au=Rosenzweig%2C+Leah+R&rft.au=Athey%2C+Susan&rft.date=2024-05-01&rft.issn=2397-3374&rft.eissn=2397-3374&rft.volume=8&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=823&rft_id=info:doi/10.1038%2Fs41562-023-01810-7&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2397-3374&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2397-3374&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2397-3374&client=summon |