Utility of Peripheral Visual Cues in Planning and Controlling Adaptive Gait
To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field. Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlu...
Saved in:
Published in | Optometry and vision science Vol. 87; no. 1; pp. 21 - 27 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
01.01.2010
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.
Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe.
Given that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body.
Our findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion. |
---|---|
AbstractList | To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.
Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe.
Given that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body.
Our findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion. To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.PURPOSETo determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe.METHODSTwelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe.Given that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body.RESULTSGiven that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body.Our findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion.CONCLUSIONSOur findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion. |
Author | Graci, Valentina Buckley, John G. Elliott, David B. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Valentina surname: Graci fullname: Graci, Valentina – sequence: 2 givenname: David B. surname: Elliott fullname: Elliott, David B. – sequence: 3 givenname: John G. surname: Buckley fullname: Buckley, John G. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19918210$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9UDtPwzAYtFARfcA_QCgbU4ofcWyzVRUURKV2oIjNcvwAozQJsYPUf0-qAkMHpvs-6e50d2MwqOrKAnCJ4BRBwW5W69cpLCAiliCONDI0YydghCjhqcCEDvobZjClGedDMA7hA0LECM3PwBAJgThGcASeNtGXPu6S2iVr2_rm3baqTF586HqYdzYkvkrWpaoqX70lqjLJvK5iW5fl_p8Z1UT_ZZOF8vEcnDpVBnvxgxOwub97nj-ky9XicT5bppowHFNHtTB5hrFAGBoNC2PyghXQMYWpdhnmhhci487k2uU2h5hSm2mGnCgKlGsyAdcH36atP_uEUW590LbsQ9q6C5IRwgkSDPfMqx9mV2ytkU3rt6rdyd_-PeH2QNBtHUJrndQ-quj3FZUvJYJyP7bsx5bHY_fi7Ej85_-f7BvFuIOD |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jbiomech_2011_02_010 crossref_primary_10_1080_00140139_2015_1105304 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2023_103063 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_51926_y crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4721_6 crossref_primary_10_1093_milmed_usae317 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2016_11_023 crossref_primary_10_1080_00140139_2022_2141347 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2020_102615 crossref_primary_10_1177_00315125211050322 crossref_primary_10_1177_1477153516659783 crossref_primary_10_18273_saluduis_54_e_22064 crossref_primary_10_34108_eujhs_960187 crossref_primary_10_1177_00315125221077566 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2019_02_033 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep41547 crossref_primary_10_1080_00222895_2010_530307 crossref_primary_10_3310_phr03080 crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_8838 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0019079 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2020_102676 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_57217_w crossref_primary_10_3390_healthcare12010042 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2011_07_003 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0267476 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_00484 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_017_1417_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2019_03_012 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00169_2016 crossref_primary_10_1123_mc_2023_0028 crossref_primary_10_1093_milmed_usac327 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_13864 crossref_primary_10_3390_s24237789 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2019_04_014 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_archger_2020_104265 crossref_primary_10_11596_asiajot_17_79 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10439_015_1440_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2012_02_008 crossref_primary_10_1093_milmed_usad082 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00931_2017 crossref_primary_10_1097_OPX_0000000000000268 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2014_10_021 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1475_1313_2010_00811_x |
Cites_doi | 10.1167/iovs.06-1193 10.1097/00001756-199712010-00002 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.06.011 10.1016/S0021-9290(96)00161-3 10.1111/j.1444-0938.1997.tb04841.x 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00476.x 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01485.x 10.1167/iovs.06-0586 10.1109/86.662619 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2005.tb06699.x 10.1007/s00221-008-1335-7 10.1207/s15326969eco103&4_7 10.1167/iovs.05-1043 10.1167/iovs.07-0326 10.1097/01.opx.0000134903.13651.8e 10.1016/S0021-9290(98)00081-5 10.1016/S0966-6362(96)01109-5 10.1007/s00221-005-0345-y 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02436.x 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.063 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90078-7 10.1167/iovs.03-1199 10.1016/0966-6362(93)90042-Y 10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.297 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181c1d547 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine |
EISSN | 1538-9235 |
EndPage | 27 |
ExternalDocumentID | 19918210 10_1097_OPX_0b013e3181c1d547 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Comparative Study |
GroupedDBID | --- -~X .GJ .Z2 0R~ 123 4Q1 4Q2 4Q3 53G 5VS 71W AAAAV AAHPQ AAIQE AAMTA AARTV AASCR AAWTL AAYXX ABASU ABBUW ABDIG ABDPE ABJNI ABPXF ABVCZ ABXRP ABXVJ ABZAD ABZZY ACDDN ACEWG ACGFO ACGFS ACILI ACNCT ACWDW ACWRI ACXJB ACXNZ ADBBV ADFPA ADGGA ADHPY ADNKB AE3 AEETU AENEX AFBFQ AFDTB AFFNX AFUWQ AHQNM AHRYX AHVBC AINUH AJCLO AJIOK AJNWD AJNYG AJZMW AKCTQ ALKUP ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALMTX AMJPA AMKUR AMNEI AOHHW AOQMC BAWUL BQLVK BS7 C45 CITATION CS3 DIK DIWNM DU5 DUNZO E.X EBS EEVPB EJD EX3 F5P FCALG FL- GNXGY GQDEL H0~ HLJTE HZ~ IKREB IN~ JF9 JG8 JK3 JK8 K8S KD2 L-C N~M O9- OAG OAH OCUKA ODA OK1 OLG OPUJH ORVUJ OUVQU OVD OVDNE OWU OWV OWW OWX OWY OWZ OXXIT P-K P2P P6G R58 RLZ S4R S4S T8P TEORI TR2 TSPGW V2I VVN W3M WH7 WOQ WOW X3V X3W XOL XXN XYM YFH ZB8 ZFV ZXP ZZMQN AWKKM CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM OLW 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c372t-f5c9d64229120dc0bdd6b7b0f7a25cf428d8b948fd6cf6e60255e4c71f9bb16c3 |
ISSN | 1040-5488 1538-9235 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 16:53:08 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 01:47:24 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:11:21 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:35:52 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c372t-f5c9d64229120dc0bdd6b7b0f7a25cf428d8b948fd6cf6e60255e4c71f9bb16c3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 |
PMID | 19918210 |
PQID | 733831972 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 7 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_733831972 pubmed_primary_19918210 crossref_citationtrail_10_1097_OPX_0b013e3181c1d547 crossref_primary_10_1097_OPX_0b013e3181c1d547 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2010-01-00 2010-Jan 20100101 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2010-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2010 text: 2010-01-00 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Optometry and vision science |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Optom Vis Sci |
PublicationYear | 2010 |
References | Pelli (R14-7-20210202) 1986 Graci (R12-7-20210202) 2009; 30 Johnson (R25-7-20210202) 2008; 56 Patla (R16-7-20210202) 1998; 10 Rietdyk (R9-7-20210202) 2006; 169 Koenderink (R28-7-20210202) 1986; 26 Turano (R6-7-20210202) 2004; 81 Marigold (R11-7-20210202) 2008; 188 Menant (R24-7-20210202) 2009; 57 Armand (R19-7-20210202) 1998; 6 Black (R1-7-20210202) 1997; 80 Rhea (R10-7-20210202) 2007; 418 Johnson (R8-7-20210202) 2007; 48 Patla (R17-7-20210202) 1997; 8 Patla (R26-7-20210202) 1997; 5 Chou (R23-7-20210202) 1998; 31 Heasley (R18-7-20210202) 2004; 45 Gonzalez-Alvarez (R13-7-20210202) 2007; 27 Black (R7-7-20210202) 2005; 88 Warren (R27-7-20210202) 1988 Lovie-Kitchin (R5-7-20210202) 1990; 5 Geruschat (R3-7-20210202) 2007; 48 Atchley (R29-7-20210202) 1998; 13 Freeman (R15-7-20210202) 2007; 48 Vargas-Martin (R4-7-20210202) 2006; 47 Patla (R22-7-20210202) 1993; 1 Chou (R21-7-20210202) 1997; 30 Turano (R2-7-20210202) 1999; 40 |
References_xml | – volume: 48 start-page: 3988 year: 2007 ident: R3-7-20210202 article-title: Estimating the amount of mental effort required for independent mobility: persons with glaucoma. publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-1193 – volume: 5 start-page: 249 year: 1990 ident: R5-7-20210202 article-title: What areas of the visual field are important for mobility in low vision patients? publication-title: Clin Vis Sci – start-page: 339 year: 1988 ident: R27-7-20210202 article-title: Movement Behavior: “The” Motor-Action Controversy. – volume: 8 start-page: 3661 year: 1997 ident: R17-7-20210202 article-title: Where and when do we look as we approach and step over an obstacle in the travel path? publication-title: Neuroreport doi: 10.1097/00001756-199712010-00002 – volume: 30 start-page: 370 year: 2009 ident: R12-7-20210202 article-title: Peripheral visual cues affect minimum-foot-clearance during overground locomotion. publication-title: Gait Posture doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.06.011 – volume: 30 start-page: 331 year: 1997 ident: R21-7-20210202 article-title: Stepping over an obstacle increases the motions and moments of the joints of the trailing limb in young adults. publication-title: J Biomech doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(96)00161-3 – volume: 80 start-page: 1 year: 1997 ident: R1-7-20210202 article-title: Mobility performance with retinitis pigmentosa. publication-title: Clin Exp Opt doi: 10.1111/j.1444-0938.1997.tb04841.x – volume: 27 start-page: 265 year: 2007 ident: R13-7-20210202 article-title: Reaching and grasping with restricted peripheral vision. publication-title: Ophthalmic Physiol Opt doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00476.x – volume: 56 start-page: 178 year: 2008 ident: R25-7-20210202 article-title: Use of single vision spectacles improves stepping precision and safety when elderly habitual multi-focal wearers negotiate a raised surface. publication-title: J Am Geriatr Soc doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01485.x – volume: 48 start-page: 1466 year: 2007 ident: R8-7-20210202 article-title: Multifocal spectacles increase variability in toe clearance and risk of tripping in the elderly. publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-0586 – volume: 6 start-page: 43 year: 1998 ident: R19-7-20210202 article-title: Stepping over obstacles during locomotion: insights from multiobjective optimization on set of input parameters. publication-title: IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng doi: 10.1109/86.662619 – volume: 88 start-page: 212 year: 2005 ident: R7-7-20210202 article-title: Vision and falls. publication-title: Clin Exp Optom doi: 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2005.tb06699.x – volume: 188 start-page: 23 year: 2008 ident: R11-7-20210202 article-title: Visual information from the lower visual field is important for walking across multi-surface terrain. publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1335-7 – volume: 10 start-page: 287 year: 1998 ident: R16-7-20210202 article-title: How is human gait controlled by vision? publication-title: Ecol Psychol doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco103&4_7 – volume: 47 start-page: 5295 year: 2006 ident: R4-7-20210202 article-title: Eye movements of patients with tunnel vision while walking. publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-1043 – volume: 48 start-page: 4445 year: 2007 ident: R15-7-20210202 article-title: Visual field loss increases the risk of falls in older adults: the Salisbury eye evaluation. publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0326 – volume: 81 start-page: 298 year: 2004 ident: R6-7-20210202 article-title: Association of visual field loss and mobility performance in older adults: Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study. publication-title: Optom Vis Sci doi: 10.1097/01.opx.0000134903.13651.8e – volume: 31 start-page: 685 year: 1998 ident: R23-7-20210202 article-title: Placing the trailing foot closer to an obstacle reduces flexion of the hip, knee, and ankle to increase the risk of tripping. publication-title: J Biomech doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(98)00081-5 – volume: 5 start-page: 54 year: 1997 ident: R26-7-20210202 article-title: Understanding the roles of vision in the control of human locomotion. publication-title: Gait Posture doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(96)01109-5 – start-page: 134 year: 1986 ident: R14-7-20210202 article-title: Low Vision: Principles and Applications. – volume: 169 start-page: 272 year: 2006 ident: R9-7-20210202 article-title: Control of adaptive locomotion: effect of visual obstruction and visual cues in the environment. publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0345-y – volume: 57 start-page: 1833 year: 2009 ident: R24-7-20210202 article-title: Older people contact more obstacles when wearing multifocal glasses and performing a secondary visual task. publication-title: J Am Geriatr Soc doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02436.x – volume: 418 start-page: 60 year: 2007 ident: R10-7-20210202 article-title: Visual exteroceptive information provided during obstacle crossing did not modify the lower limb trajectory. publication-title: Neurosci Lett doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.063 – volume: 26 start-page: 161 year: 1986 ident: R28-7-20210202 article-title: Optic flow. publication-title: Vision Res doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90078-7 – volume: 40 start-page: 2803 year: 1999 ident: R2-7-20210202 article-title: Mobility performance in glaucoma. publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci – volume: 45 start-page: 2122 year: 2004 ident: R18-7-20210202 article-title: Stepping up to a new level: effects of blurring vision in the elderly. publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-1199 – volume: 1 start-page: 45 year: 1993 ident: R22-7-20210202 article-title: Visual control of limb trajectory over obstacles during locomotion: effect of obstacle height and width. publication-title: Gait Posture doi: 10.1016/0966-6362(93)90042-Y – volume: 13 start-page: 297 year: 1998 ident: R29-7-20210202 article-title: The effect of age, retinal eccentricity, and speed on the detection of optic flow components. publication-title: Psychol Aging doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.297 |
SSID | ssj0017356 |
Score | 2.1105523 |
Snippet | To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.
Twelve subjects... To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.PURPOSETo determine the... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 21 |
SubjectTerms | Adaptation, Ocular - physiology Adult Cues Female Gait - physiology Humans Male Photic Stimulation Sensory Deprivation Vision Tests - utilization Visual Fields - physiology |
Title | Utility of Peripheral Visual Cues in Planning and Controlling Adaptive Gait |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19918210 https://www.proquest.com/docview/733831972 |
Volume | 87 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1La9tAEF7cFEovIX3GTVr20FtQql1JK-sSCCHGbR7uwQ6-Ce0rGBzZJHIP_fWdfVl2cEjbizCLJYudz7vzzX4zg9BXQoQiDGhJD_buKAWXICq0OYXP4lTSinGSmnznq2s2GKc_Jtmk0zlZUy0tG34sfm_NK_kfq8IY2NVkyf6DZVcPhQH4DPaFK1gYrn9l43EznXlJhSlYbCsEzI5-TR9MTohYKqcR922JfAabVabbHPRKVgsrHLqtphsR-uGimd-p5t6VZnLZ50d-q2z1OpWwQoCbyuxbvgn3IxWIFcy3fZ39OXJQAPu2Xj7kYJVrIeSwtkpSV2fkWG0Z80ur30vXIRTWya3LtysLPPw5aeOzRBCZuaKcm9Wyr4dlf3x5WY7OJ6MX6CUFmmAp9feL1SlSntjuvatXC6mTRf5t229suiZP8A3rd4z20K4nDPjUWf8N6qj6LXp15SUR79CFBwGea9yCADsQYAMCPK1xAAEGi-I1EOAAAmxA8B6N--ejs0HkO2REIslpE-lMFBIYJC0IjaWIuZSM5zzWeUUzoYFayh4v0p6WTGimmCGQKhU50QXnhInkA9qp57XaRzgBR5kwpnKtaSoZ_EkVUE2g60mSgY8ruygJk1MKXz7edDGZlUHGAFNaPp7SLopWdy1c-ZRnvo_DvJewzpnDq6pW8-VDmZtYiumR10UfnT3aBwLH6VESf3r-5gP0ugX0Idpp7pfqM3iVDf9iofMH-8R2Pw |
linkProvider | Flying Publisher |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Utility+of+peripheral+visual+cues+in+planning+and+controlling+adaptive+gait&rft.jtitle=Optometry+and+vision+science&rft.au=Graci%2C+Valentina&rft.au=Elliott%2C+David+B&rft.au=Buckley%2C+John+G&rft.date=2010-01-01&rft.issn=1538-9235&rft.eissn=1538-9235&rft.volume=87&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=21&rft_id=info:doi/10.1097%2FOPX.0b013e3181c1d547&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1040-5488&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1040-5488&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1040-5488&client=summon |