Utility of Peripheral Visual Cues in Planning and Controlling Adaptive Gait

To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field. Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlu...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inOptometry and vision science Vol. 87; no. 1; pp. 21 - 27
Main Authors Graci, Valentina, Elliott, David B., Buckley, John G.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States 01.01.2010
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field. Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe. Given that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body. Our findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion.
AbstractList To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field. Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe. Given that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body. Our findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion.
To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.PURPOSETo determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.Twelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe.METHODSTwelve subjects completed obstacle crossing trials while wearing goggles that provided four visual conditions: upper visual field occlusion, lower visual field occlusion (LO), circumferential peripheral visual field occlusion (CPO), and full vision. The obstacle was either positioned as a lone structure or within a doorframe.Given that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body.RESULTSGiven that subjects completed the task safely without cues from the lower or peripheral visual field, this suggests that subjects used exteroceptive information provided in a feed-forward manner under these conditions. LO and CPO led to increased foot placement distance from the obstacle and to increased toe clearance over the obstacle with a reduced crossing-walking velocity. The increased variability of dependent measures under LO and CPO suggests that exproprioceptive information from the peripheral visual field is generally used to provide online control of lower limbs. The presence of the doorframe facilitated lead-foot placement under LO by providing exproprioceptive cues in the upper visual field. However, under CPO conditions, the doorframe led to a further reduction in crossing velocity and increase in trail-foot horizontal distance and lead-toe clearance, which may have been because of concerns about hitting the doorframe with the head and/or upper body.Our findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion.CONCLUSIONSOur findings suggest that exteroceptive cues are provided by the central visual field and are used in a feed-forward manner to plan the gait adaptations required to safely negotiate an obstacle, whereas exproprioceptive information is provided by the peripheral visual field and used online to "fine tune" adaptive gait. The loss of the upper and lower peripheral visual fields together had a greater effect on adaptive gait compared with the loss of the lower visual field alone, likely because of the absence of lamellar flow visual cues used to control egomotion.
Author Graci, Valentina
Buckley, John G.
Elliott, David B.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Valentina
  surname: Graci
  fullname: Graci, Valentina
– sequence: 2
  givenname: David B.
  surname: Elliott
  fullname: Elliott, David B.
– sequence: 3
  givenname: John G.
  surname: Buckley
  fullname: Buckley, John G.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19918210$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9UDtPwzAYtFARfcA_QCgbU4ofcWyzVRUURKV2oIjNcvwAozQJsYPUf0-qAkMHpvs-6e50d2MwqOrKAnCJ4BRBwW5W69cpLCAiliCONDI0YydghCjhqcCEDvobZjClGedDMA7hA0LECM3PwBAJgThGcASeNtGXPu6S2iVr2_rm3baqTF586HqYdzYkvkrWpaoqX70lqjLJvK5iW5fl_p8Z1UT_ZZOF8vEcnDpVBnvxgxOwub97nj-ky9XicT5bppowHFNHtTB5hrFAGBoNC2PyghXQMYWpdhnmhhci487k2uU2h5hSm2mGnCgKlGsyAdcH36atP_uEUW590LbsQ9q6C5IRwgkSDPfMqx9mV2ytkU3rt6rdyd_-PeH2QNBtHUJrndQ-quj3FZUvJYJyP7bsx5bHY_fi7Ej85_-f7BvFuIOD
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jbiomech_2011_02_010
crossref_primary_10_1080_00140139_2015_1105304
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2023_103063
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_51926_y
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4721_6
crossref_primary_10_1093_milmed_usae317
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2016_11_023
crossref_primary_10_1080_00140139_2022_2141347
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2020_102615
crossref_primary_10_1177_00315125211050322
crossref_primary_10_1177_1477153516659783
crossref_primary_10_18273_saluduis_54_e_22064
crossref_primary_10_34108_eujhs_960187
crossref_primary_10_1177_00315125221077566
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2019_02_033
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep41547
crossref_primary_10_1080_00222895_2010_530307
crossref_primary_10_3310_phr03080
crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_8838
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0019079
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2020_102676
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_57217_w
crossref_primary_10_3390_healthcare12010042
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2011_07_003
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0267476
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_00484
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_017_1417_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2019_03_012
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00169_2016
crossref_primary_10_1123_mc_2023_0028
crossref_primary_10_1093_milmed_usac327
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_13864
crossref_primary_10_3390_s24237789
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2019_04_014
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_archger_2020_104265
crossref_primary_10_11596_asiajot_17_79
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10439_015_1440_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2012_02_008
crossref_primary_10_1093_milmed_usad082
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00931_2017
crossref_primary_10_1097_OPX_0000000000000268
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2014_10_021
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1475_1313_2010_00811_x
Cites_doi 10.1167/iovs.06-1193
10.1097/00001756-199712010-00002
10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.06.011
10.1016/S0021-9290(96)00161-3
10.1111/j.1444-0938.1997.tb04841.x
10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00476.x
10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01485.x
10.1167/iovs.06-0586
10.1109/86.662619
10.1111/j.1444-0938.2005.tb06699.x
10.1007/s00221-008-1335-7
10.1207/s15326969eco103&4_7
10.1167/iovs.05-1043
10.1167/iovs.07-0326
10.1097/01.opx.0000134903.13651.8e
10.1016/S0021-9290(98)00081-5
10.1016/S0966-6362(96)01109-5
10.1007/s00221-005-0345-y
10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02436.x
10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.063
10.1016/0042-6989(86)90078-7
10.1167/iovs.03-1199
10.1016/0966-6362(93)90042-Y
10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.297
ContentType Journal Article
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
DOI 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181c1d547
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
EISSN 1538-9235
EndPage 27
ExternalDocumentID 19918210
10_1097_OPX_0b013e3181c1d547
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
Comparative Study
GroupedDBID ---
-~X
.GJ
.Z2
0R~
123
4Q1
4Q2
4Q3
53G
5VS
71W
AAAAV
AAHPQ
AAIQE
AAMTA
AARTV
AASCR
AAWTL
AAYXX
ABASU
ABBUW
ABDIG
ABDPE
ABJNI
ABPXF
ABVCZ
ABXRP
ABXVJ
ABZAD
ABZZY
ACDDN
ACEWG
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACILI
ACNCT
ACWDW
ACWRI
ACXJB
ACXNZ
ADBBV
ADFPA
ADGGA
ADHPY
ADNKB
AE3
AEETU
AENEX
AFBFQ
AFDTB
AFFNX
AFUWQ
AHQNM
AHRYX
AHVBC
AINUH
AJCLO
AJIOK
AJNWD
AJNYG
AJZMW
AKCTQ
ALKUP
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALMTX
AMJPA
AMKUR
AMNEI
AOHHW
AOQMC
BAWUL
BQLVK
BS7
C45
CITATION
CS3
DIK
DIWNM
DU5
DUNZO
E.X
EBS
EEVPB
EJD
EX3
F5P
FCALG
FL-
GNXGY
GQDEL
H0~
HLJTE
HZ~
IKREB
IN~
JF9
JG8
JK3
JK8
K8S
KD2
L-C
N~M
O9-
OAG
OAH
OCUKA
ODA
OK1
OLG
OPUJH
ORVUJ
OUVQU
OVD
OVDNE
OWU
OWV
OWW
OWX
OWY
OWZ
OXXIT
P-K
P2P
P6G
R58
RLZ
S4R
S4S
T8P
TEORI
TR2
TSPGW
V2I
VVN
W3M
WH7
WOQ
WOW
X3V
X3W
XOL
XXN
XYM
YFH
ZB8
ZFV
ZXP
ZZMQN
AWKKM
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
OLW
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c372t-f5c9d64229120dc0bdd6b7b0f7a25cf428d8b948fd6cf6e60255e4c71f9bb16c3
ISSN 1040-5488
1538-9235
IngestDate Fri Jul 11 16:53:08 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 01:47:24 EST 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:11:21 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:35:52 EDT 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 1
Language English
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c372t-f5c9d64229120dc0bdd6b7b0f7a25cf428d8b948fd6cf6e60255e4c71f9bb16c3
Notes ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 23
PMID 19918210
PQID 733831972
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 7
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_733831972
pubmed_primary_19918210
crossref_citationtrail_10_1097_OPX_0b013e3181c1d547
crossref_primary_10_1097_OPX_0b013e3181c1d547
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2010-01-00
2010-Jan
20100101
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2010-01-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 01
  year: 2010
  text: 2010-01-00
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle Optometry and vision science
PublicationTitleAlternate Optom Vis Sci
PublicationYear 2010
References Pelli (R14-7-20210202) 1986
Graci (R12-7-20210202) 2009; 30
Johnson (R25-7-20210202) 2008; 56
Patla (R16-7-20210202) 1998; 10
Rietdyk (R9-7-20210202) 2006; 169
Koenderink (R28-7-20210202) 1986; 26
Turano (R6-7-20210202) 2004; 81
Marigold (R11-7-20210202) 2008; 188
Menant (R24-7-20210202) 2009; 57
Armand (R19-7-20210202) 1998; 6
Black (R1-7-20210202) 1997; 80
Rhea (R10-7-20210202) 2007; 418
Johnson (R8-7-20210202) 2007; 48
Patla (R17-7-20210202) 1997; 8
Patla (R26-7-20210202) 1997; 5
Chou (R23-7-20210202) 1998; 31
Heasley (R18-7-20210202) 2004; 45
Gonzalez-Alvarez (R13-7-20210202) 2007; 27
Black (R7-7-20210202) 2005; 88
Warren (R27-7-20210202) 1988
Lovie-Kitchin (R5-7-20210202) 1990; 5
Geruschat (R3-7-20210202) 2007; 48
Atchley (R29-7-20210202) 1998; 13
Freeman (R15-7-20210202) 2007; 48
Vargas-Martin (R4-7-20210202) 2006; 47
Patla (R22-7-20210202) 1993; 1
Chou (R21-7-20210202) 1997; 30
Turano (R2-7-20210202) 1999; 40
References_xml – volume: 48
  start-page: 3988
  year: 2007
  ident: R3-7-20210202
  article-title: Estimating the amount of mental effort required for independent mobility: persons with glaucoma.
  publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
  doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-1193
– volume: 5
  start-page: 249
  year: 1990
  ident: R5-7-20210202
  article-title: What areas of the visual field are important for mobility in low vision patients?
  publication-title: Clin Vis Sci
– start-page: 339
  year: 1988
  ident: R27-7-20210202
  article-title: Movement Behavior: “The” Motor-Action Controversy.
– volume: 8
  start-page: 3661
  year: 1997
  ident: R17-7-20210202
  article-title: Where and when do we look as we approach and step over an obstacle in the travel path?
  publication-title: Neuroreport
  doi: 10.1097/00001756-199712010-00002
– volume: 30
  start-page: 370
  year: 2009
  ident: R12-7-20210202
  article-title: Peripheral visual cues affect minimum-foot-clearance during overground locomotion.
  publication-title: Gait Posture
  doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.06.011
– volume: 30
  start-page: 331
  year: 1997
  ident: R21-7-20210202
  article-title: Stepping over an obstacle increases the motions and moments of the joints of the trailing limb in young adults.
  publication-title: J Biomech
  doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(96)00161-3
– volume: 80
  start-page: 1
  year: 1997
  ident: R1-7-20210202
  article-title: Mobility performance with retinitis pigmentosa.
  publication-title: Clin Exp Opt
  doi: 10.1111/j.1444-0938.1997.tb04841.x
– volume: 27
  start-page: 265
  year: 2007
  ident: R13-7-20210202
  article-title: Reaching and grasping with restricted peripheral vision.
  publication-title: Ophthalmic Physiol Opt
  doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00476.x
– volume: 56
  start-page: 178
  year: 2008
  ident: R25-7-20210202
  article-title: Use of single vision spectacles improves stepping precision and safety when elderly habitual multi-focal wearers negotiate a raised surface.
  publication-title: J Am Geriatr Soc
  doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01485.x
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1466
  year: 2007
  ident: R8-7-20210202
  article-title: Multifocal spectacles increase variability in toe clearance and risk of tripping in the elderly.
  publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
  doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-0586
– volume: 6
  start-page: 43
  year: 1998
  ident: R19-7-20210202
  article-title: Stepping over obstacles during locomotion: insights from multiobjective optimization on set of input parameters.
  publication-title: IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng
  doi: 10.1109/86.662619
– volume: 88
  start-page: 212
  year: 2005
  ident: R7-7-20210202
  article-title: Vision and falls.
  publication-title: Clin Exp Optom
  doi: 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2005.tb06699.x
– volume: 188
  start-page: 23
  year: 2008
  ident: R11-7-20210202
  article-title: Visual information from the lower visual field is important for walking across multi-surface terrain.
  publication-title: Exp Brain Res
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1335-7
– volume: 10
  start-page: 287
  year: 1998
  ident: R16-7-20210202
  article-title: How is human gait controlled by vision?
  publication-title: Ecol Psychol
  doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco103&4_7
– volume: 47
  start-page: 5295
  year: 2006
  ident: R4-7-20210202
  article-title: Eye movements of patients with tunnel vision while walking.
  publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
  doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-1043
– volume: 48
  start-page: 4445
  year: 2007
  ident: R15-7-20210202
  article-title: Visual field loss increases the risk of falls in older adults: the Salisbury eye evaluation.
  publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
  doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0326
– volume: 81
  start-page: 298
  year: 2004
  ident: R6-7-20210202
  article-title: Association of visual field loss and mobility performance in older adults: Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study.
  publication-title: Optom Vis Sci
  doi: 10.1097/01.opx.0000134903.13651.8e
– volume: 31
  start-page: 685
  year: 1998
  ident: R23-7-20210202
  article-title: Placing the trailing foot closer to an obstacle reduces flexion of the hip, knee, and ankle to increase the risk of tripping.
  publication-title: J Biomech
  doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(98)00081-5
– volume: 5
  start-page: 54
  year: 1997
  ident: R26-7-20210202
  article-title: Understanding the roles of vision in the control of human locomotion.
  publication-title: Gait Posture
  doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(96)01109-5
– start-page: 134
  year: 1986
  ident: R14-7-20210202
  article-title: Low Vision: Principles and Applications.
– volume: 169
  start-page: 272
  year: 2006
  ident: R9-7-20210202
  article-title: Control of adaptive locomotion: effect of visual obstruction and visual cues in the environment.
  publication-title: Exp Brain Res
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0345-y
– volume: 57
  start-page: 1833
  year: 2009
  ident: R24-7-20210202
  article-title: Older people contact more obstacles when wearing multifocal glasses and performing a secondary visual task.
  publication-title: J Am Geriatr Soc
  doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02436.x
– volume: 418
  start-page: 60
  year: 2007
  ident: R10-7-20210202
  article-title: Visual exteroceptive information provided during obstacle crossing did not modify the lower limb trajectory.
  publication-title: Neurosci Lett
  doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.063
– volume: 26
  start-page: 161
  year: 1986
  ident: R28-7-20210202
  article-title: Optic flow.
  publication-title: Vision Res
  doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90078-7
– volume: 40
  start-page: 2803
  year: 1999
  ident: R2-7-20210202
  article-title: Mobility performance in glaucoma.
  publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
– volume: 45
  start-page: 2122
  year: 2004
  ident: R18-7-20210202
  article-title: Stepping up to a new level: effects of blurring vision in the elderly.
  publication-title: Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
  doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-1199
– volume: 1
  start-page: 45
  year: 1993
  ident: R22-7-20210202
  article-title: Visual control of limb trajectory over obstacles during locomotion: effect of obstacle height and width.
  publication-title: Gait Posture
  doi: 10.1016/0966-6362(93)90042-Y
– volume: 13
  start-page: 297
  year: 1998
  ident: R29-7-20210202
  article-title: The effect of age, retinal eccentricity, and speed on the detection of optic flow components.
  publication-title: Psychol Aging
  doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.297
SSID ssj0017356
Score 2.1105523
Snippet To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field. Twelve subjects...
To determine the relative importance to adaptive locomotion of peripheral visual cues provided by different parts of the visual field.PURPOSETo determine the...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage 21
SubjectTerms Adaptation, Ocular - physiology
Adult
Cues
Female
Gait - physiology
Humans
Male
Photic Stimulation
Sensory Deprivation
Vision Tests - utilization
Visual Fields - physiology
Title Utility of Peripheral Visual Cues in Planning and Controlling Adaptive Gait
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19918210
https://www.proquest.com/docview/733831972
Volume 87
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1La9tAEF7cFEovIX3GTVr20FtQql1JK-sSCCHGbR7uwQ6-Ce0rGBzZJHIP_fWdfVl2cEjbizCLJYudz7vzzX4zg9BXQoQiDGhJD_buKAWXICq0OYXP4lTSinGSmnznq2s2GKc_Jtmk0zlZUy0tG34sfm_NK_kfq8IY2NVkyf6DZVcPhQH4DPaFK1gYrn9l43EznXlJhSlYbCsEzI5-TR9MTohYKqcR922JfAabVabbHPRKVgsrHLqtphsR-uGimd-p5t6VZnLZ50d-q2z1OpWwQoCbyuxbvgn3IxWIFcy3fZ39OXJQAPu2Xj7kYJVrIeSwtkpSV2fkWG0Z80ur30vXIRTWya3LtysLPPw5aeOzRBCZuaKcm9Wyr4dlf3x5WY7OJ6MX6CUFmmAp9feL1SlSntjuvatXC6mTRf5t229suiZP8A3rd4z20K4nDPjUWf8N6qj6LXp15SUR79CFBwGea9yCADsQYAMCPK1xAAEGi-I1EOAAAmxA8B6N--ejs0HkO2REIslpE-lMFBIYJC0IjaWIuZSM5zzWeUUzoYFayh4v0p6WTGimmCGQKhU50QXnhInkA9qp57XaRzgBR5kwpnKtaSoZ_EkVUE2g60mSgY8ruygJk1MKXz7edDGZlUHGAFNaPp7SLopWdy1c-ZRnvo_DvJewzpnDq6pW8-VDmZtYiumR10UfnT3aBwLH6VESf3r-5gP0ugX0Idpp7pfqM3iVDf9iofMH-8R2Pw
linkProvider Flying Publisher
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Utility+of+peripheral+visual+cues+in+planning+and+controlling+adaptive+gait&rft.jtitle=Optometry+and+vision+science&rft.au=Graci%2C+Valentina&rft.au=Elliott%2C+David+B&rft.au=Buckley%2C+John+G&rft.date=2010-01-01&rft.issn=1538-9235&rft.eissn=1538-9235&rft.volume=87&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=21&rft_id=info:doi/10.1097%2FOPX.0b013e3181c1d547&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1040-5488&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1040-5488&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1040-5488&client=summon