Ontogeny of escape-hatching decisions vibrational cue use changes as predicted from costs of sampling and false alarms

As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis c...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBehavioral ecology and sociobiology Vol. 73; no. 4; pp. 1 - 14
Main Authors Warkentin, Karen M., Jung, Julie, Solano, L. Alberto Rueda, McDaniel, J. Gregory
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Berlin/Heidelberg Springer Science + Business Media 01.04.2019
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior.
AbstractList As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: Animal behavior changes ontogenetically for many reasons, including developing capacities to sense and assess cues and perform actions, adaptive responses to changing costs and benefits, and learning. Hatching is a critical behavior, subject to selection from pre- and post-hatching environments, that is performed during a period of rapid development. Environmentally cued hatching is widespread, and many embryos use vibrations to inform behavior. The egg-tray system we developed enables vibration playbacks to red-eyed treefrog embryos across a broader range of stages than previously possible, facilitating tests of how and why development changes behavior. Playbacks revealed that embryos at different stages assess cues and make decisions differently, with stage-specific responses matching adaptive predictions. This reveals a new level of complexity in embryo information use, motivating and enabling further research assessing the role of adaptive ontogeny and developmental constraint in early behavioral development.
As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior.
As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior.Significance statementAnimal behavior changes ontogenetically for many reasons, including developing capacities to sense and assess cues and perform actions, adaptive responses to changing costs and benefits, and learning. Hatching is a critical behavior, subject to selection from pre- and post-hatching environments, that is performed during a period of rapid development. Environmentally cued hatching is widespread, and many embryos use vibrations to inform behavior. The egg-tray system we developed enables vibration playbacks to red-eyed treefrog embryos across a broader range of stages than previously possible, facilitating tests of how and why development changes behavior. Playbacks revealed that embryos at different stages assess cues and make decisions differently, with stage-specific responses matching adaptive predictions. This reveals a new level of complexity in embryo information use, motivating and enabling further research assessing the role of adaptive ontogeny and developmental constraint in early behavioral development.
As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas . These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior. Significance statement Animal behavior changes ontogenetically for many reasons, including developing capacities to sense and assess cues and perform actions, adaptive responses to changing costs and benefits, and learning. Hatching is a critical behavior, subject to selection from pre- and post-hatching environments, that is performed during a period of rapid development. Environmentally cued hatching is widespread, and many embryos use vibrations to inform behavior. The egg-tray system we developed enables vibration playbacks to red-eyed treefrog embryos across a broader range of stages than previously possible, facilitating tests of how and why development changes behavior. Playbacks revealed that embryos at different stages assess cues and make decisions differently, with stage-specific responses matching adaptive predictions. This reveals a new level of complexity in embryo information use, motivating and enabling further research assessing the role of adaptive ontogeny and developmental constraint in early behavioral development.
ArticleNumber 51
Author Solano, L. Alberto Rueda
Warkentin, Karen M.
McDaniel, J. Gregory
Jung, Julie
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Karen M.
  surname: Warkentin
  fullname: Warkentin, Karen M.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Julie
  surname: Jung
  fullname: Jung, Julie
– sequence: 3
  givenname: L. Alberto Rueda
  surname: Solano
  fullname: Solano, L. Alberto Rueda
– sequence: 4
  givenname: J. Gregory
  surname: McDaniel
  fullname: McDaniel, J. Gregory
BookMark eNp9kEtLAzEURoMo2Ko_wIVQEMFN9OY1SZYivkBwo-uQSe-0U9qkJtNF_71TRhS66Cqbc24-zpgcxxSRkEsGdwxA3xcAXikKzFJeVYLyIzJiUnAKuuLHZARCAlVSilMyLmUBABUzZkRuPmKXZhi3k9RMsAS_Rjr3XZi3cTaZYmhLm2I5JyeNXxa8-H3PyNfz0-fjK33_eHl7fHinQWjW0RqV0WgRghFc-GkdOKAWUjS1R5jWutHc1CCCAltLI5U26DmGKVpjeSPEGbkd7q5z-t5g6dyqLQGXSx8xbYrjXGhpLSjbo9d76CJtcuzXOc6stlorsaPYQIWcSsnYuHVuVz5vHQO3C-eGcK4P53bhHO8dveeEtvNd36HLvl0eNPlglv6XOMP8v-mQdDVIi9Kl_LdPGg1KaS1-AHdui78
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_023_03324_8
crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_206052
crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_236141
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_022_01634_4
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2022_1466
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10682_024_10316_1
crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_12932
crossref_primary_10_1643_h2020085
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_021_03070_9
crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_244533
Cites_doi 10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.003.0010
10.1007/BF00164184
10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.005
10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025
10.1242/jeb.026518
10.1890/12-0194.1
10.1093/icb/icr009
10.1093/icb/icr017
10.1242/jeb.078295
10.1016/0020-7519(88)90127-0
10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010
10.1007/s00265-016-2159-2
10.1242/jeb.02150
10.1242/jeb.001362
10.1890/07-0529.1
10.1002/dev.20521
10.1073/pnas.92.8.3507
10.1007/s00442-007-0708-0
10.1242/jeb.139519
10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019
10.1007/s10682-013-9661-z
10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.006
10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641
10.1670/14-111
10.1643/CE-12-111
10.1093/icb/icr046
10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.012
10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.024
10.1006/anbe.2000.1508
10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.000515
10.1038/nmeth.2089
10.1093/beheco/10.3.251
10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00931.x
10.1093/icb/icr003
10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.035
10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00212.x
10.1371/journal.pone.0052551
10.2307/1565364
10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.006
10.1093/icb/icr037
10.1371/journal.pone.0087932
10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00031-8
10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.007
10.1007/s002270000329
10.1098/rspb.2008.0754
10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.008
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Springer-Verlag GmbH, part of Springer Nature 2019
Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology is a copyright of Springer, (2019). All Rights Reserved.
Copyright_xml – notice: Springer-Verlag GmbH, part of Springer Nature 2019
– notice: Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019
– notice: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology is a copyright of Springer, (2019). All Rights Reserved.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
0-V
3V.
7QG
7QR
7SN
7SS
7X7
7XB
88G
88I
88J
8FD
8FE
8FH
8FI
8FJ
8FK
ABUWG
AEUYN
AFKRA
ALSLI
ATCPS
AZQEC
BBNVY
BENPR
BHPHI
C1K
CCPQU
DWQXO
FR3
FYUFA
GHDGH
GNUQQ
HCIFZ
HEHIP
K9.
LK8
M0S
M2M
M2P
M2R
M2S
M7P
P64
PATMY
PHGZM
PHGZT
PKEHL
POGQB
PQEST
PQGLB
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
PRQQA
PSYQQ
PYCSY
Q9U
RC3
7S9
L.6
DOI 10.1007/s00265-019-2663-2
DatabaseName CrossRef
ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection
ProQuest Central (Corporate)
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)
Health & Medical Collection
ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)
Psychology Database (Alumni)
Science Database (Alumni Edition)
Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)
Technology Research Database
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
Hospital Premium Collection
Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest One Sustainability
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
Social Science Premium Collection
Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest SciTech Premium Collection Natural Science Collection Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central
Natural Science Collection
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
ProQuest One
ProQuest Central Korea
Engineering Research Database
Health Research Premium Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Central Student
SciTech Premium Collection
Sociology Collection
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Health & Medical Collection
Psychology Database
Science Database
Social Science Database
Sociology Database
Biological Science Database
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Science Database
ProQuest Central Premium
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest One Social Sciences
ProQuest One Psychology
Environmental Science Collection
ProQuest Central Basic
Genetics Abstracts
AGRICOLA
AGRICOLA - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ProQuest One Psychology
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Central Essentials
SciTech Premium Collection
Sociology & Social Sciences Collection
ProQuest Central China
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences
ProQuest One Sustainability
Health Research Premium Collection
Natural Science Collection
Biological Science Collection
Chemoreception Abstracts
ProQuest Central (New)
ProQuest Sociology
Social Science Premium Collection
ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
ProQuest Hospital Collection
Sociology Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
Biological Science Database
Ecology Abstracts
ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Science Collection
Entomology Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete
ProQuest Social Science Journals
ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
Environmental Science Database
Engineering Research Database
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection
Technology Research Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central
Genetics Abstracts
Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central Korea
Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection
ProQuest Sociology Collection
ProQuest One Social Sciences
ProQuest Central Basic
ProQuest Science Journals
ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni)
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Psychology Journals
Animal Behavior Abstracts
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
AGRICOLA
AGRICOLA - Academic
DatabaseTitleList AGRICOLA

ProQuest One Psychology

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest Central
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Biology
Zoology
Psychology
Environmental Sciences
EISSN 1432-0762
EndPage 14
ExternalDocumentID 10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2
48705577
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Universidad del Magdalena
  grantid: Academic Arrangement 014–2015
– fundername: National Science Foundation (US)
  grantid: IOS–1354072
– fundername: Boston University (US)
  grantid: N/A
– fundername: Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute
  grantid: N/A
  funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100009201
GroupedDBID -DZ
-~C
.86
0-V
06C
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
203
23N
29~
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2~H
30V
4.4
406
408
409
40D
40E
4P2
5GY
5RE
5VS
67N
67Z
6NX
78A
7X7
7XC
88I
8CJ
8FE
8FH
8FI
8FJ
8UJ
95-
95.
95~
96X
AABHQ
AACDK
AAGAY
AAHBH
AAHKG
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANZL
AAPKM
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYZH
ABAKF
ABBBX
ABBHK
ABBRH
ABBXA
ABDBE
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFSG
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABHQN
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABKCH
ABKTR
ABLJU
ABMNI
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPLY
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTLG
ABTMW
ABUWG
ABWNU
ABXPI
ABXSQ
ACAOD
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACGOD
ACHIC
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIWK
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACOMO
ACPIV
ACPRK
ACSTC
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADIMF
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEMSY
AENEX
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETLH
AEUPB
AEUYN
AEVLU
AEXYK
AEZWR
AFAZZ
AFBBN
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFOHR
AFQWF
AFRAH
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGUYK
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHMBA
AHPBZ
AHSBF
AHWEU
AHXOZ
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AIXLP
AJRNO
AJZVZ
AKMHD
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
ALWAN
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
AQVQM
ARALO
ARMRJ
ASOEW
ASPBG
ATCPS
ATHPR
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYFIA
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
BA0
BBNVY
BDATZ
BENPR
BGNMA
BHPHI
BPHCQ
BSONS
BVXVI
CBGCD
CCPQU
CS3
CSCUP
D0L
D1J
DDRTE
DL5
DNIVK
DPUIP
DU5
DWQXO
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EDH
EIOEI
EJD
EPAXT
ESBYG
EV9
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FWDCC
FYUFA
G-Y
G-Z
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ7
GQ8
GXS
HCIFZ
HEHIP
HF~
HG5
HG6
HMCUK
HMJXF
HQYDN
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
I09
IHE
IJ-
IKXTQ
IPSME
ITM
IWAJR
IXC
IZIGR
IZQ
I~X
I~Z
J-C
J0Z
JAAYA
JBMMH
JBS
JBSCW
JCJTX
JENOY
JHFFW
JKQEH
JLS
JLXEF
JPM
JST
JZLTJ
KDC
KOV
KPH
LAS
LK8
LLZTM
M2M
M2P
M2R
M2S
M4Y
M7P
MA-
MQGED
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O93
O9G
O9I
O9J
OAM
P19
P2P
PATMY
PF-
PHGZM
PHGZT
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSYQQ
PT4
PT5
PYCSY
Q2X
QOK
QOR
QOS
R89
R9I
RHV
RNS
ROL
RPX
RSV
S16
S27
S3A
S3B
SA0
SAP
SBL
SBY
SDH
SDM
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SZN
T13
TN5
TSG
TSK
TSV
TUC
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UOJIU
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
W23
W48
WH7
WJK
WK8
YLTOR
Z45
ZCA
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
~02
~EX
~KM
-4W
-56
-5G
-BR
-EM
-Y2
1SB
2.D
28-
2P1
2VQ
3SX
3V.
53G
5QI
AAAVM
AANXM
AARHV
AAYTO
ABTAH
ABULA
ACBXY
ADINQ
ADULT
ADYPR
AEBTG
AEFIE
AEKMD
AFEXP
AFGCZ
AGGDS
AGJBK
AI.
AJBLW
BBWZM
CAG
COF
DOOOF
EN4
GQ6
GTFYD
H13
HTVGU
JSODD
KOW
M0O
MVM
N2Q
NDZJH
O9-
OVD
R4E
RIG
RNI
RZK
S1Z
S26
S28
SCLPG
T16
TEORI
VH1
WK6
XIH
Y6R
YIF
Z7Y
Z7Z
Z83
Z8S
Z8T
Z8W
ZXP
ZY4
AAYXX
ADHKG
ADXHL
AGQPQ
CITATION
7QG
7QR
7SN
7SS
7XB
8FD
8FK
ABRTQ
C1K
FR3
K9.
P64
PKEHL
POGQB
PQEST
PQGLB
PQUKI
PRINS
PRQQA
Q9U
RC3
7S9
L.6
PUEGO
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c371t-be587e9e0c8323adbc20e7343fbae0db7f728b03c509b484578ea2ecde9892f33
IEDL.DBID U2A
ISSN 0340-5443
IngestDate Sun Aug 24 04:06:32 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 10:13:14 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 02:15:15 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:12:05 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:33:13 EST 2025
Thu Jun 19 22:09:38 EDT 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Keywords Biotremology
Ontogenetic adaptation
Risk assessment
Vibration playback
Environmentally cued hatching
Embryo behavior
Language English
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c371t-be587e9e0c8323adbc20e7343fbae0db7f728b03c509b484578ea2ecde9892f33
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0002-7804-800X
PQID 2197977539
PQPubID 54055
PageCount 14
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_2237499059
proquest_journals_2197977539
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2
crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2
springer_journals_10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2
jstor_primary_48705577
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 20190401
20190400
2019-4-00
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2019-04-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 4
  year: 2019
  text: 20190401
  day: 1
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace Berlin/Heidelberg
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Berlin/Heidelberg
– name: Heidelberg
PublicationTitle Behavioral ecology and sociobiology
PublicationTitleAbbrev Behav Ecol Sociobiol
PublicationYear 2019
Publisher Springer Science + Business Media
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer Science + Business Media
– name: Springer Berlin Heidelberg
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Dugatkin, Godin (CR13) 1992; 29
Darmaillacq, Lesimple, Dickel (CR10) 2008; 76
Warkentin (CR39) 2005; 70
Buckley, Michael, Irschick (CR4) 2005; 19
Doody, Paull (CR11) 2013; 2013
Kempster, Hart, Collin (CR23) 2013; 8
Touchon, Vonesh (CR34) 2016; 50
Brown, Iskandar (CR3) 2000; 34
Caldwell, McDaniel, Warkentin (CR5) 2009; 212
Griem, Martin (CR19) 2000; 137
Wiedenmayer (CR49) 2009; 33
FitzGibbon (CR16) 1994; 34
Hall, Oppenheim (CR21) 1987; 38
Gomez-Mestre, Wiens, Warkentin (CR18) 2008; 78
Oyarzun, Strathmann (CR29) 2011; 51
Ferrari, Chivers (CR15) 2009; 5
Warkentin (CR36) 1995; 92
Savage (CR31) 2002
Warkentin, Caldwell, Siok, D’Amato, McDaniel (CR45) 2007; 210
Warkentin, Caldwell, Dukas, Ratcliffe (CR42) 2009
Gomez-Mestre, Warkentin (CR17) 2007; 153
Whittington, Kearn (CR47) 1988; 18
Schneider, Rasband, Elicieri (CR32) 2012; 9
Warkentin (CR38) 2000; 60
Bradbury, Vehrencamp (CR2) 1998
Romagny, Darmaillacq, Guibe, Bellanger, Dickel (CR30) 2012; 215
Colombelli-Negrel, Hauber, Robertson, Sulloway, Hoi, Griggio, Kleindorfer (CR8) 2012; 22
Warkentin, Caldwell, McDaniel (CR44) 2006; 209
Mukai, Hironaka, Tojo, Nomakuchi (CR27) 2014; 9
Martin, Bailey, Moravek, Carlson (CR24) 2011; 51
Touchon, McCoy, Vonesh, Warkentin (CR35) 2013; 94
Endo, Takanashi, Mukai, Numata (CR14) 2019; 29
Warkentin (CR37) 1999; 10
Cohen, Seid, Warkentin (CR7) 2016; 219
Guibe, Poirel, Houde, Dickel (CR20) 2012; 84
Bate (CR1) 1999; 9
Dall, Giraldeau, Olsson, McNamara, Stephens (CR9) 2005; 20
Warkentin (CR40) 2011; 51
Mukai, Hironaka, Tojo, Nomakuchi (CR26) 2012; 84
Willink, Palmer, Landberg, Vonesh, Warkentin (CR50) 2014; 28
Caldwell, McDaniel, Warkentin (CR6) 2010; 79
Warkentin, Cuccaro Diaz, Güell, Jung, Kim, Cohen (CR46) 2017; 129
Mathis, Ferrari, Windel, Messier, Chivers (CR25) 2008; 275
Nishide, Tanaka (CR28) 2016; 70
Doody, Stewart, Camacho, Christian (CR12) 2012; 83
Warkentin, Buckley, Metcalf (CR43) 2006; 71
Harshaw, Lickliter (CR22) 2011; 53
Whittington, Kearn (CR48) 2011; 51
Warkentin (CR41) 2011; 51
Strathmann, Strathmann, Ruiz-Jones, Hadfield (CR33) 2010; 129
AS Darmaillacq (2663_CR10) 2008; 76
JS Doody (2663_CR12) 2012; 83
KM Warkentin (2663_CR41) 2011; 51
JW Bradbury (2663_CR2) 1998
CD FitzGibbon (2663_CR16) 1994; 34
RR Strathmann (2663_CR33) 2010; 129
J Endo (2663_CR14) 2019; 29
CP Wiedenmayer (2663_CR49) 2009; 33
H Mukai (2663_CR27) 2014; 9
H Mukai (2663_CR26) 2012; 84
JN Griem (2663_CR19) 2000; 137
JS Doody (2663_CR11) 2013; 2013
I Gomez-Mestre (2663_CR18) 2008; 78
KM Warkentin (2663_CR39) 2005; 70
MCO Ferrari (2663_CR15) 2009; 5
KL Cohen (2663_CR7) 2016; 219
JC Touchon (2663_CR34) 2016; 50
KM Warkentin (2663_CR36) 1995; 92
JC Touchon (2663_CR35) 2013; 94
K Martin (2663_CR24) 2011; 51
KM Warkentin (2663_CR46) 2017; 129
KM Warkentin (2663_CR38) 2000; 60
LA Dugatkin (2663_CR13) 1992; 29
MS Caldwell (2663_CR5) 2009; 212
KM Warkentin (2663_CR42) 2009
JM Savage (2663_CR31) 2002
A Mathis (2663_CR25) 2008; 275
FX Oyarzun (2663_CR29) 2011; 51
SRX Dall (2663_CR9) 2005; 20
I Gomez-Mestre (2663_CR17) 2007; 153
ID Whittington (2663_CR48) 2011; 51
Y Nishide (2663_CR28) 2016; 70
WG Hall (2663_CR21) 1987; 38
KM Warkentin (2663_CR40) 2011; 51
CR Buckley (2663_CR4) 2005; 19
ID Whittington (2663_CR47) 1988; 18
D Colombelli-Negrel (2663_CR8) 2012; 22
KM Warkentin (2663_CR44) 2006; 209
S Romagny (2663_CR30) 2012; 215
B Willink (2663_CR50) 2014; 28
KM Warkentin (2663_CR45) 2007; 210
KM Warkentin (2663_CR37) 1999; 10
RM Kempster (2663_CR23) 2013; 8
M Guibe (2663_CR20) 2012; 84
MS Caldwell (2663_CR6) 2010; 79
CA Schneider (2663_CR32) 2012; 9
KM Warkentin (2663_CR43) 2006; 71
M Bate (2663_CR1) 1999; 9
RM Brown (2663_CR3) 2000; 34
C Harshaw (2663_CR22) 2011; 53
References_xml – start-page: 177
  year: 2009
  end-page: 200
  ident: CR42
  article-title: Assessing risk: embryos, information, and escape hatching
  publication-title: Cognitive ecology II
  doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.003.0010
– volume: 34
  start-page: 139
  year: 1994
  end-page: 148
  ident: CR16
  article-title: The costs and benefits of predator inspection behaviour in Thompson’s gazelles
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/BF00164184
– year: 2002
  ident: CR31
  publication-title: The amphibians and reptiles of Costa Rica
– volume: 79
  start-page: 255
  year: 2010
  end-page: 260
  ident: CR6
  article-title: Is it safe? Red-eyed treefrog embryos assessing predation risk use two features of rain vibrations to avoid false alarms
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.005
– volume: 22
  start-page: 2155
  year: 2012
  end-page: 2160
  ident: CR8
  article-title: Embryonic learning of vocal passwords in superb fairy-wrens reveals intruder cuckoo nestlings
  publication-title: Curr Biol
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025
– volume: 212
  start-page: 566
  year: 2009
  end-page: 575
  ident: CR5
  article-title: Frequency information in the vibration-cued escape hatching of red-eyed treefrogs
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.026518
– volume: 94
  start-page: 850
  year: 2013
  end-page: 860
  ident: CR35
  article-title: Effects of plastic hatching timing carry over through metamorphosis in red-eyed treefrogs
  publication-title: Ecology
  doi: 10.1890/12-0194.1
– year: 1998
  ident: CR2
  publication-title: Principles of animal communication
– volume: 51
  start-page: 81
  year: 2011
  end-page: 90
  ident: CR29
  article-title: Plasticity of hatching and duration of planktonic development in marine invertebrates
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr009
– volume: 51
  start-page: 14
  year: 2011
  end-page: 25
  ident: CR40
  article-title: Environmentally cued hatching across taxa: embryos respond to risk and opportunity
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr017
– volume: 215
  start-page: 4125
  year: 2012
  end-page: 4130
  ident: CR30
  article-title: Feel, smell and see in an egg: emergence of perception and learning in an immature invertebrate, the cuttlefish embryo
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.078295
– volume: 18
  start-page: 847
  year: 1988
  end-page: 852
  ident: CR47
  article-title: Rapid hatching of mechanically-disturbed eggs of the monogenean gill parasite , with observations on sedimentation of egg bundles
  publication-title: Int J Parasitol
  doi: 10.1016/0020-7519(88)90127-0
– volume: 20
  start-page: 187
  year: 2005
  end-page: 193
  ident: CR9
  article-title: Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology
  publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1507
  year: 2016
  end-page: 1515
  ident: CR28
  article-title: Desert locust, , eggs hatch in synchrony in a mass but not when separated
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2159-2
– volume: 209
  start-page: 1376
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1384
  ident: CR44
  article-title: Temporal pattern cues in vibrational risk assessment by red-eyed treefrog embryos,
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.02150
– volume: 210
  start-page: 614
  year: 2007
  end-page: 619
  ident: CR45
  article-title: Flexible information sampling in vibrational assessment of predation risk by red-eyed treefrog embryos
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.001362
– volume: 78
  start-page: 205
  year: 2008
  end-page: 224
  ident: CR18
  article-title: Evolution of adaptive plasticity: risk-sensitive hatching in Neotropical leaf-breeding treefrogs
  publication-title: Ecol Monogr
  doi: 10.1890/07-0529.1
– volume: 53
  start-page: 291
  year: 2011
  end-page: 302
  ident: CR22
  article-title: Biased embryos: prenatal experience alters the postnatal malleability of auditory preferences in bobwhite quail
  publication-title: Dev Psychobiol
  doi: 10.1002/dev.20521
– volume: 92
  start-page: 3507
  year: 1995
  end-page: 3510
  ident: CR36
  article-title: Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs
  publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3507
– volume: 29
  start-page: 233
  year: 1992
  end-page: 252
  ident: CR13
  article-title: Prey approaching predators: a cost-benefit perspective
  publication-title: Ann Zool Fenn
– volume: 153
  start-page: 197
  year: 2007
  end-page: 206
  ident: CR17
  article-title: To hatch and hatch not: similar selective trade-offs but different responses to egg predators in two closely related, syntopic treefrogs
  publication-title: Oecologia
  doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-0708-0
– volume: 219
  start-page: 1875
  year: 2016
  end-page: 1883
  ident: CR7
  article-title: How embryos escape from danger: the mechanism of rapid, plastic hatching in red-eyed treefrogs
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.139519
– volume: 70
  start-page: 59
  year: 2005
  end-page: 71
  ident: CR39
  article-title: How do embryos assess risk? Vibrational cues in predator-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrogs
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019
– volume: 28
  start-page: 103
  year: 2014
  end-page: 116
  ident: CR50
  article-title: Environmental context shapes immediate and cumulative costs of risk-induced early hatching
  publication-title: Evol Ecol
  doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9661-z
– volume: 76
  start-page: 131
  year: 2008
  end-page: 134
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Embryonic visual learning in the cuttlefish,
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.006
– volume: 5
  start-page: 160
  year: 2009
  end-page: 162
  ident: CR15
  article-title: Latent inhibition of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians
  publication-title: Biol Lett
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641
– volume: 50
  start-page: 113
  year: 2016
  end-page: 119
  ident: CR34
  article-title: Variation in abundance and efficacy of tadpole predators in a Neotropical pond community
  publication-title: J Herpetol
  doi: 10.1670/14-111
– volume: 2013
  start-page: 159
  year: 2013
  end-page: 164
  ident: CR11
  article-title: Hitting the ground running: environmentally cued hatching in a lizard
  publication-title: Copeia
  doi: 10.1643/CE-12-111
– volume: 51
  start-page: 111
  year: 2011
  end-page: 127
  ident: CR41
  article-title: Plasticity of hatching in amphibians: evolution, trade-offs, cues and mechanisms
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr046
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1443
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1448
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Maternal vibration induces synchronous hatching in a subsocial burrower bug
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.012
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1
  year: 2019
  end-page: 6
  ident: CR14
  article-title: Egg-cracking vibration as a cue for stink bug siblings to synchronize hatching
  publication-title: Curr Biol
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.024
– volume: 60
  start-page: 503
  year: 2000
  end-page: 510
  ident: CR38
  article-title: Wasp predation and wasp-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrog eggs
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1508
– volume: 38
  start-page: 91
  year: 1987
  end-page: 128
  ident: CR21
  article-title: Developmental psychobiology: prenatal, perinatal, and early postnatal aspects of behavioral development
  publication-title: Annu Rev Psychol
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.000515
– volume: 9
  start-page: 671
  year: 2012
  end-page: 675
  ident: CR32
  article-title: NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis
  publication-title: Nat Methods
  doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089
– volume: 10
  start-page: 251
  year: 1999
  end-page: 262
  ident: CR37
  article-title: The development of behavioral defenses: a mechanistic analysis of vulnerability in red-eyed tree frog hatchlings
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.3.251
– volume: 19
  start-page: 67
  year: 2005
  end-page: 72
  ident: CR4
  article-title: Early hatching decreases jumping performance in a direct-developing frog,
  publication-title: Funct Ecol
  doi: 10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00931.x
– volume: 51
  start-page: 91
  year: 2011
  end-page: 99
  ident: CR48
  article-title: Hatching strategies in monogenean (platyhelminth) parasites that facilitate host infection
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr003
– volume: 84
  start-page: 213
  year: 2012
  end-page: 217
  ident: CR20
  article-title: Food imprinting and visual generalization in embryos and newly hatched cuttlefish,
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.035
– volume: 129
  start-page: 309
  year: 2010
  end-page: 318
  ident: CR33
  article-title: Effect of plasticity in hatching on duration as a precompetent swimming larva in the nudibranch
  publication-title: Invertebr Biol
  doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00212.x
– volume: 8
  start-page: e52551
  year: 2013
  ident: CR23
  article-title: Survival of the stillest: predator avoidance in shark embryos
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052551
– volume: 34
  start-page: 404
  year: 2000
  end-page: 413
  ident: CR3
  article-title: Nest site selection, larval hatching, and advertisement calls of from southwestern Sulawesi (Celebes) Island, Indonesia
  publication-title: J Herpetol
  doi: 10.2307/1565364
– volume: 83
  start-page: 645
  year: 2012
  end-page: 651
  ident: CR12
  article-title: Good vibrations? Sibling embryos expedite hatching in a turtle
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.006
– volume: 51
  start-page: 26
  year: 2011
  end-page: 37
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Taking the plunge: California grunion embryos emerge rapidly with environmentally cued hatching (ECH)
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr037
– volume: 9
  start-page: e87932
  year: 2014
  ident: CR27
  article-title: Maternal vibration: an important cue for embryo hatching in a subsocial shield bug
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087932
– volume: 9
  start-page: 670
  year: 1999
  end-page: 675
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Development of motor behaviour
  publication-title: Curr Opin Neurobiol
  doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00031-8
– volume: 71
  start-page: 417
  year: 2006
  end-page: 425
  ident: CR43
  article-title: Development of red-eyed treefrog eggs affects efficiency and choices of egg-foraging wasps
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.007
– volume: 137
  start-page: 177
  year: 2000
  end-page: 181
  ident: CR19
  article-title: Wave action: the environmental trigger for hatching in the California grunion (Teleostei: Atherinopsidae)
  publication-title: Mar Biol
  doi: 10.1007/s002270000329
– volume: 275
  start-page: 2603
  year: 2008
  end-page: 2607
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Learning by embryos and the ghost of predation future
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0754
– volume: 129
  start-page: 103
  year: 2017
  end-page: 112
  ident: CR46
  article-title: Developmental onset of escape-hatching responses in red-eyed treefrogs depends on cue type
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.008
– volume: 33
  start-page: 432
  year: 2009
  end-page: 441
  ident: CR49
  article-title: Plasticity of defensive behavior and fear in early development
  publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004
– volume: 34
  start-page: 404
  year: 2000
  ident: 2663_CR3
  publication-title: J Herpetol
  doi: 10.2307/1565364
– volume: 209
  start-page: 1376
  year: 2006
  ident: 2663_CR44
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.02150
– volume: 212
  start-page: 566
  year: 2009
  ident: 2663_CR5
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.026518
– volume-title: The amphibians and reptiles of Costa Rica
  year: 2002
  ident: 2663_CR31
– volume: 210
  start-page: 614
  year: 2007
  ident: 2663_CR45
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.001362
– volume: 76
  start-page: 131
  year: 2008
  ident: 2663_CR10
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.006
– volume: 8
  start-page: e52551
  year: 2013
  ident: 2663_CR23
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052551
– volume: 129
  start-page: 103
  year: 2017
  ident: 2663_CR46
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.008
– volume: 9
  start-page: 670
  year: 1999
  ident: 2663_CR1
  publication-title: Curr Opin Neurobiol
  doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00031-8
– volume: 9
  start-page: 671
  year: 2012
  ident: 2663_CR32
  publication-title: Nat Methods
  doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089
– volume: 22
  start-page: 2155
  year: 2012
  ident: 2663_CR8
  publication-title: Curr Biol
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025
– volume: 28
  start-page: 103
  year: 2014
  ident: 2663_CR50
  publication-title: Evol Ecol
  doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9661-z
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1443
  year: 2012
  ident: 2663_CR26
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.012
– volume: 29
  start-page: 1
  year: 2019
  ident: 2663_CR14
  publication-title: Curr Biol
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.024
– volume: 60
  start-page: 503
  year: 2000
  ident: 2663_CR38
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1508
– volume: 2013
  start-page: 159
  year: 2013
  ident: 2663_CR11
  publication-title: Copeia
  doi: 10.1643/CE-12-111
– volume: 153
  start-page: 197
  year: 2007
  ident: 2663_CR17
  publication-title: Oecologia
  doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-0708-0
– start-page: 177
  volume-title: Cognitive ecology II
  year: 2009
  ident: 2663_CR42
  doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.003.0010
– volume-title: Principles of animal communication
  year: 1998
  ident: 2663_CR2
– volume: 10
  start-page: 251
  year: 1999
  ident: 2663_CR37
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.3.251
– volume: 51
  start-page: 26
  year: 2011
  ident: 2663_CR24
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr037
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1507
  year: 2016
  ident: 2663_CR28
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2159-2
– volume: 92
  start-page: 3507
  year: 1995
  ident: 2663_CR36
  publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3507
– volume: 51
  start-page: 14
  year: 2011
  ident: 2663_CR40
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr017
– volume: 50
  start-page: 113
  year: 2016
  ident: 2663_CR34
  publication-title: J Herpetol
  doi: 10.1670/14-111
– volume: 51
  start-page: 91
  year: 2011
  ident: 2663_CR48
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr003
– volume: 5
  start-page: 160
  year: 2009
  ident: 2663_CR15
  publication-title: Biol Lett
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641
– volume: 20
  start-page: 187
  year: 2005
  ident: 2663_CR9
  publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010
– volume: 38
  start-page: 91
  year: 1987
  ident: 2663_CR21
  publication-title: Annu Rev Psychol
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.000515
– volume: 275
  start-page: 2603
  year: 2008
  ident: 2663_CR25
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0754
– volume: 94
  start-page: 850
  year: 2013
  ident: 2663_CR35
  publication-title: Ecology
  doi: 10.1890/12-0194.1
– volume: 53
  start-page: 291
  year: 2011
  ident: 2663_CR22
  publication-title: Dev Psychobiol
  doi: 10.1002/dev.20521
– volume: 33
  start-page: 432
  year: 2009
  ident: 2663_CR49
  publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004
– volume: 34
  start-page: 139
  year: 1994
  ident: 2663_CR16
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/BF00164184
– volume: 219
  start-page: 1875
  year: 2016
  ident: 2663_CR7
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.139519
– volume: 78
  start-page: 205
  year: 2008
  ident: 2663_CR18
  publication-title: Ecol Monogr
  doi: 10.1890/07-0529.1
– volume: 51
  start-page: 81
  year: 2011
  ident: 2663_CR29
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr009
– volume: 18
  start-page: 847
  year: 1988
  ident: 2663_CR47
  publication-title: Int J Parasitol
  doi: 10.1016/0020-7519(88)90127-0
– volume: 83
  start-page: 645
  year: 2012
  ident: 2663_CR12
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.006
– volume: 129
  start-page: 309
  year: 2010
  ident: 2663_CR33
  publication-title: Invertebr Biol
  doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00212.x
– volume: 79
  start-page: 255
  year: 2010
  ident: 2663_CR6
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.005
– volume: 70
  start-page: 59
  year: 2005
  ident: 2663_CR39
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019
– volume: 29
  start-page: 233
  year: 1992
  ident: 2663_CR13
  publication-title: Ann Zool Fenn
– volume: 215
  start-page: 4125
  year: 2012
  ident: 2663_CR30
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.078295
– volume: 137
  start-page: 177
  year: 2000
  ident: 2663_CR19
  publication-title: Mar Biol
  doi: 10.1007/s002270000329
– volume: 84
  start-page: 213
  year: 2012
  ident: 2663_CR20
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.035
– volume: 71
  start-page: 417
  year: 2006
  ident: 2663_CR43
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.007
– volume: 51
  start-page: 111
  year: 2011
  ident: 2663_CR41
  publication-title: Integr Comp Biol
  doi: 10.1093/icb/icr046
– volume: 19
  start-page: 67
  year: 2005
  ident: 2663_CR4
  publication-title: Funct Ecol
  doi: 10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00931.x
– volume: 9
  start-page: e87932
  year: 2014
  ident: 2663_CR27
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087932
SSID ssj0006188
Score 2.320623
Snippet As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes...
SourceID proquest
crossref
springer
jstor
SourceType Aggregation Database
Enrichment Source
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 1
SubjectTerms Agalychnis callidryas
Alarms
Animal behavior
Animal Ecology
Animal populations
animals
behavior change
Behavioral Sciences
Biomedical and Life Sciences
Cues
Decision making
Developmental stages
eggs
Embryos
Frequency spectrum
Frogs
Hatching
learning
Life Sciences
mortality
Ontogeny
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Playback
Playbacks
Predation
Predators
prediction
rain
risk
Risk acceptance
Risk assessment
Sampling
tadpoles
Tradeoffs
Vibration
Vibration control
Vibrations
Zoology
Subtitle vibrational cue use changes as predicted from costs of sampling and false alarms
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: Health & Medical Collection
  dbid: 7X7
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3di9QwEB_05OBeRFcXV0-J4JMS7CbtpvVFRO44fNAXDxZfSpJO8EHb9dJV7r93Jv1YTvDeCplOC_OR32QmMwCv8qLYWM327UKQ3L9EOkIFslwXRuUeg8NUbfF5c3GZf9oW2_HALY5llZNPTI666Tyfkb8lyzKEVQpdvd_9kjw1irOr4wiNu3CPW5dxSZfZzgEXbVVp7mSm80xyn7cpq5mlJqJqw2VrlaQtSkt1Y18aShNvgM5_8qRp-zl_APdH3Cg-DIJ-CHewXcDxMEnyegHLs8OFNSIbLTYu4GT2cER1_K1LT4_gz5e270h3rkUXBEaugpLfySvzeZRoxrk78Z34zdH0cFwo_B7FPqIY7gpHYaPYXXGeh0Cr4Gsqwnexj8wxWi5UJ1a2pSXScBSWQuif8TFcnp99_XghxxkM0muz7qXDojRYYebJ9LVtnFcZGp3r4CxmjTPBqNJl2hPwcHmZkwNAq9A3WJWVClov4ajtWnwCIhCU0uu8sJtC57YpK1v6gIUmPtY2oVlBNkmg9mODcp6T8aOeWysnodUktJqFVqsVvJ5f2Q3dOW4jXiaxzpQUq3EHMrOC00nO9WjBsT7o2wpezstke5xQsS12e6JR2lDESAh1BW8m_Tiw-O-fPL39g8_gRLFipvqgUzjqr_b4nKBP714k_f4Lk6f_kA
  priority: 102
  providerName: ProQuest
Title Ontogeny of escape-hatching decisions
URI https://www.jstor.org/stable/48705577
https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00265-019-2663-2
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2197977539
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2237499059
Volume 73
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3di9NAEB_0joN7Ea0Wo2dZwSdlIc1mu4lvrbQeClXEQvUl7G52uQdNjttUuf_emXwdJyr4lEAmQ2A-9jeZL4AXqZQLLci-jfec5pdwg6iAZ3OpktQ6b1xbbbFdnO_Sd3u57_u4w1DtPqQkW089NrtRuECFZjnHQ0Vw9LvHkkJ3VOJdshzd72LeLpuMRRpzGu42pDL_xOLWYdTVI95Cmr8lR9szZ3Mf7vVgkS076T6AO66awEm3PvJ6AtP1TZcakvVmGiZwOro1pDr5Wrd3D-Hnh6qpUWGuWe2ZC1T6xC_QFdNPKFb2y3bCa_aDQujuHyGzB8cOwbGuQTgwHdjlFSV3EKky6k1htg5NII5BU3U6stIVPkK1dkxj3Pw9PILdZv35zTnvFy9wK9S84cbJTLncxRbtXejS2CR2SqTCG-3i0iivkszEwiLaMGmWotU7nThbujzLEy_EFI6qunKPgXnET2KeSr2QItVlluvMeicF8tG69GUE8SCBwvZTyWk5xrdinKfcCq1AoRUktCKJ4OX4ymU3kuNfxNNWrCMlBmg0dkxFcDbIuejNNhTovhUCYinyCJ6Pj9HgKIuiK1cfkCYRCsNEhKURvBr044bFX7_kyX9RP4XThPS0rRE6g6Pm6uCeIfxpzAzuqr2awfFys1pt6fr2y_s1Xlfr7cdPs9YYfgF5pQGN
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3da9RAEB_KlWJfRE8Po1VX0BdlMbebT0HEjytXW0-RFoov6e5mgw-anN2c5f4p_0Zn8nVUsG99C-xkE5jffO3MzgA8DcIwUpLkWxcFp_4lXKNXwJNpGIvA2ELbptpiEc1Pgo-n4ekW_OnvwlBZZa8TG0WdV4bOyF-iZMXoq4QyfbP8xWlqFGVX-xEaLSwO7foCQzb3-uAD8veZEPuz4_dz3k0V4EbG05prGyaxTa1vEMxS5doI38YykIVW1s91XMQi0b40aEp1kAQIaauENblNk1QUdACKKn87kBjKjGD73Wzx5eug-6NpM-nSl4HPqbNcn0f1m7alIqJCuZSjUZRcXLKEbTHkJTf3n8xsY_D2b8HNzlNlb1to3YYtW45hp51duR7DZLa5IodknY5wY9gddCpS7Xyrmqc7cPG5rCtE65pVBbOO6q74d7QDdALG8m7Sj3vFflP83h5QMrOybOUsa28nO6YcW55TZgndZEYXY5ipXO1oR6eoNB63UiUuoUxZpjBo_-nuwsm18GcCo7Iq7T1gBTpvchqEKgploPIkVYkpbChxH6XyIvfA7zmQma4lOk3m-JENzZwbpmXItIyYlgkPng-vLNt-IFcRTxq2DpQYHVLPs9iDvZ7PWaczXLZBuAdPhmWUdkrhqNJWK6QRMsYYFX1iD170-Nhs8d8_uX_1Bx_Djfnxp6Ps6GBx-AB2BYG0qU7ag1F9vrIP0fGq9aMO7QzOrlvA_gJOoT7W
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3da9RAEB9KpdIX0dPDaNUV9EVZmttNbhNBRGyP1kr1wcLhS9xsdumDJmf3znL_mn-dM5uPo4J961tgJ5vA_OZrZ3YG4EWSplMtSb5L5zj1L-ElegU8m6RKJMa60oZqi9Pp0VnycZ7Ot-BPfxeGyip7nRgUddUYOiPfR8lS6KukMt93XVnEl4PZu8UvThOkKNPaj9NoIXJi15cYvvm3xwfI65dCzA6_fjji3YQBbqSaLHlp00zZ3MYGgS11VRoRWyUT6Upt46pUTomsjKVBs1omWYLwtlpYU9k8y4Wjw1BU_7eUTCckY2o-BHtoJsPMy1gmMacec31GNQ4NTMWUSuZyjuZRcnHFJrZlkVcc3n9ytMH0ze7Cnc5nZe9bkN2DLVuPYKedYrkewfhwc1kOyTpt4UewO2hXpNr51oSn-3D5uV42iNs1axyzniqw-DlaBDoLY1U388e_Yb8pkm-PKplZWbbylrX3lD3Tni0uKMeEDjOjKzLMNH7paUevqUget9I1LqF0WaYxfP_pH8DZjXBnDNt1U9uHwBy6cXKSpHqaykRXWa4z42wqcR-tK1dFEPccKEzXHJ1mdPwohrbOgWkFMq0gphUiglfDK4u2M8h1xOPA1oES40TqfqYi2Ov5XHTawxcbrEfwfFhGuadkjq5ts0IaIRVGq-gdR_C6x8dmi__-yaPrP_gMbqNYFZ-OT08ew64gjIYypT3YXl6s7BP0wJbl0wB1Bt9vWrb-ArLlQaY
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Ontogeny+of+escape-hatching+decisions&rft.jtitle=Behavioral+ecology+and+sociobiology&rft.au=Warkentin%2C+Karen+M.&rft.au=Jung%2C+Julie&rft.au=Solano%2C+L.+Alberto+Rueda&rft.au=McDaniel%2C+J.+Gregory&rft.date=2019-04-01&rft.pub=Springer+Science+%2B+Business+Media&rft.issn=0340-5443&rft.eissn=1432-0762&rft.volume=73&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=1&rft.epage=14&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs00265-019-2663-2&rft.externalDocID=48705577
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0340-5443&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0340-5443&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0340-5443&client=summon