Ontogeny of escape-hatching decisions vibrational cue use changes as predicted from costs of sampling and false alarms
As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis c...
Saved in:
Published in | Behavioral ecology and sociobiology Vol. 73; no. 4; pp. 1 - 14 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Berlin/Heidelberg
Springer Science + Business Media
01.04.2019
Springer Berlin Heidelberg Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior. |
---|---|
AbstractList | As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: Animal behavior changes ontogenetically for many reasons, including developing capacities to sense and assess cues and perform actions, adaptive responses to changing costs and benefits, and learning. Hatching is a critical behavior, subject to selection from pre- and post-hatching environments, that is performed during a period of rapid development. Environmentally cued hatching is widespread, and many embryos use vibrations to inform behavior. The egg-tray system we developed enables vibration playbacks to red-eyed treefrog embryos across a broader range of stages than previously possible, facilitating tests of how and why development changes behavior. Playbacks revealed that embryos at different stages assess cues and make decisions differently, with stage-specific responses matching adaptive predictions. This reveals a new level of complexity in embryo information use, motivating and enabling further research assessing the role of adaptive ontogeny and developmental constraint in early behavioral development. As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior. As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas. These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior.Significance statementAnimal behavior changes ontogenetically for many reasons, including developing capacities to sense and assess cues and perform actions, adaptive responses to changing costs and benefits, and learning. Hatching is a critical behavior, subject to selection from pre- and post-hatching environments, that is performed during a period of rapid development. Environmentally cued hatching is widespread, and many embryos use vibrations to inform behavior. The egg-tray system we developed enables vibration playbacks to red-eyed treefrog embryos across a broader range of stages than previously possible, facilitating tests of how and why development changes behavior. Playbacks revealed that embryos at different stages assess cues and make decisions differently, with stage-specific responses matching adaptive predictions. This reveals a new level of complexity in embryo information use, motivating and enabling further research assessing the role of adaptive ontogeny and developmental constraint in early behavioral development. As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes in information use and environmentally cued hatching decisions of embryos match adaptive predictions, using red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas . These arboreal embryos hatch rapidly and prematurely to escape egg predators, cued by physical disturbance in attacks. Hatching early increases tadpole mortality, and premature embryos use multiple frequency and temporal properties of vibrations to assess risk, avoiding false alarms in benign disturbances such as rain. Because development increases hatchling survival, we hypothesized embryos approaching spontaneous hatching would reduce vibration sampling to reduce egg-predation risk, accepting more false alarms if cues are time-consuming to assess. We designed stimuli to elicit high or low hatching of younger embryos, with the property that reduces hatching either rapidly evident (frequency spectrum or fast temporal pattern) or time-consuming to assess (slow temporal pattern). We developed a new egg-tray vibration playback system to present motion-only cues with minimal disturbance in setup, then used it to compare responses of younger and previously untestable older embryos. The playback duration embryos experienced before or without hatching decreased with age, suggesting reduced vibration sampling. At both stages, hatching responses differed between frequencies, and among temporal patterns; nearly full-term embryos still showed selective hatching responses to cues. Hatching increased developmentally in a stimulus-dependent manner, with the greatest change in response to the slow pattern, as predicted. Developmental changes in discrimination appear well-matched to changing trade-offs, consistent with ontogenetic adaptation of embryo behavior. Significance statement Animal behavior changes ontogenetically for many reasons, including developing capacities to sense and assess cues and perform actions, adaptive responses to changing costs and benefits, and learning. Hatching is a critical behavior, subject to selection from pre- and post-hatching environments, that is performed during a period of rapid development. Environmentally cued hatching is widespread, and many embryos use vibrations to inform behavior. The egg-tray system we developed enables vibration playbacks to red-eyed treefrog embryos across a broader range of stages than previously possible, facilitating tests of how and why development changes behavior. Playbacks revealed that embryos at different stages assess cues and make decisions differently, with stage-specific responses matching adaptive predictions. This reveals a new level of complexity in embryo information use, motivating and enabling further research assessing the role of adaptive ontogeny and developmental constraint in early behavioral development. |
ArticleNumber | 51 |
Author | Solano, L. Alberto Rueda Warkentin, Karen M. McDaniel, J. Gregory Jung, Julie |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Karen M. surname: Warkentin fullname: Warkentin, Karen M. – sequence: 2 givenname: Julie surname: Jung fullname: Jung, Julie – sequence: 3 givenname: L. Alberto Rueda surname: Solano fullname: Solano, L. Alberto Rueda – sequence: 4 givenname: J. Gregory surname: McDaniel fullname: McDaniel, J. Gregory |
BookMark | eNp9kEtLAzEURoMo2Ko_wIVQEMFN9OY1SZYivkBwo-uQSe-0U9qkJtNF_71TRhS66Cqbc24-zpgcxxSRkEsGdwxA3xcAXikKzFJeVYLyIzJiUnAKuuLHZARCAlVSilMyLmUBABUzZkRuPmKXZhi3k9RMsAS_Rjr3XZi3cTaZYmhLm2I5JyeNXxa8-H3PyNfz0-fjK33_eHl7fHinQWjW0RqV0WgRghFc-GkdOKAWUjS1R5jWutHc1CCCAltLI5U26DmGKVpjeSPEGbkd7q5z-t5g6dyqLQGXSx8xbYrjXGhpLSjbo9d76CJtcuzXOc6stlorsaPYQIWcSsnYuHVuVz5vHQO3C-eGcK4P53bhHO8dveeEtvNd36HLvl0eNPlglv6XOMP8v-mQdDVIi9Kl_LdPGg1KaS1-AHdui78 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_023_03324_8 crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_206052 crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_236141 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_022_01634_4 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2022_1466 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10682_024_10316_1 crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_12932 crossref_primary_10_1643_h2020085 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_021_03070_9 crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_244533 |
Cites_doi | 10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.003.0010 10.1007/BF00164184 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.005 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025 10.1242/jeb.026518 10.1890/12-0194.1 10.1093/icb/icr009 10.1093/icb/icr017 10.1242/jeb.078295 10.1016/0020-7519(88)90127-0 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010 10.1007/s00265-016-2159-2 10.1242/jeb.02150 10.1242/jeb.001362 10.1890/07-0529.1 10.1002/dev.20521 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3507 10.1007/s00442-007-0708-0 10.1242/jeb.139519 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019 10.1007/s10682-013-9661-z 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.006 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641 10.1670/14-111 10.1643/CE-12-111 10.1093/icb/icr046 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.012 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.024 10.1006/anbe.2000.1508 10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.000515 10.1038/nmeth.2089 10.1093/beheco/10.3.251 10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00931.x 10.1093/icb/icr003 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.035 10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00212.x 10.1371/journal.pone.0052551 10.2307/1565364 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.006 10.1093/icb/icr037 10.1371/journal.pone.0087932 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00031-8 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.007 10.1007/s002270000329 10.1098/rspb.2008.0754 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.008 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Springer-Verlag GmbH, part of Springer Nature 2019 Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology is a copyright of Springer, (2019). All Rights Reserved. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Springer-Verlag GmbH, part of Springer Nature 2019 – notice: Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019 – notice: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology is a copyright of Springer, (2019). All Rights Reserved. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 0-V 3V. 7QG 7QR 7SN 7SS 7X7 7XB 88G 88I 88J 8FD 8FE 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AEUYN AFKRA ALSLI ATCPS AZQEC BBNVY BENPR BHPHI C1K CCPQU DWQXO FR3 FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ HCIFZ HEHIP K9. LK8 M0S M2M M2P M2R M2S M7P P64 PATMY PHGZM PHGZT PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQGLB PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PRQQA PSYQQ PYCSY Q9U RC3 7S9 L.6 |
DOI | 10.1007/s00265-019-2663-2 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest Central (Corporate) Animal Behavior Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Psychology Database (Alumni) Science Database (Alumni Edition) Social Science Database (Alumni Edition) Technology Research Database ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest One Sustainability ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Social Science Premium Collection Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest SciTech Premium Collection Natural Science Collection Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Natural Science Collection Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One ProQuest Central Korea Engineering Research Database Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student SciTech Premium Collection Sociology Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Collection Psychology Database Science Database Social Science Database Sociology Database Biological Science Database Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Science Database ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest One Psychology Environmental Science Collection ProQuest Central Basic Genetics Abstracts AGRICOLA AGRICOLA - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Central Essentials SciTech Premium Collection Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central China Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Sustainability Health Research Premium Collection Natural Science Collection Biological Science Collection Chemoreception Abstracts ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Sociology Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Sociology Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Biological Science Database Ecology Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Science Collection Entomology Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Social Science Journals ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition Environmental Science Database Engineering Research Database ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central Genetics Abstracts Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection ProQuest Sociology Collection ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Science Journals ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni) ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Psychology Journals Animal Behavior Abstracts ProQuest Central (Alumni) AGRICOLA AGRICOLA - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | AGRICOLA ProQuest One Psychology |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Biology Zoology Psychology Environmental Sciences |
EISSN | 1432-0762 |
EndPage | 14 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2 48705577 |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Universidad del Magdalena grantid: Academic Arrangement 014–2015 – fundername: National Science Foundation (US) grantid: IOS–1354072 – fundername: Boston University (US) grantid: N/A – fundername: Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute grantid: N/A funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100009201 |
GroupedDBID | -DZ -~C .86 0-V 06C 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 203 23N 29~ 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2~H 30V 4.4 406 408 409 40D 40E 4P2 5GY 5RE 5VS 67N 67Z 6NX 78A 7X7 7XC 88I 8CJ 8FE 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8UJ 95- 95. 95~ 96X AABHQ AACDK AAGAY AAHBH AAHKG AAHNG AAIAL AAJBT AAJKR AANZL AAPKM AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYQN AAYZH ABAKF ABBBX ABBHK ABBRH ABBXA ABDBE ABDZT ABECU ABFSG ABFTV ABHLI ABHQN ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABKCH ABKTR ABLJU ABMNI ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABPLY ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTLG ABTMW ABUWG ABWNU ABXPI ABXSQ ACAOD ACDTI ACGFS ACGOD ACHIC ACHSB ACHXU ACIWK ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACNCT ACOKC ACOMO ACPIV ACPRK ACSTC ACZOJ ADBBV ADHHG ADHIR ADIMF ADKNI ADKPE ADRFC ADTPH ADURQ ADYFF ADZKW AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEMSY AENEX AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETLH AEUPB AEUYN AEVLU AEXYK AEZWR AFAZZ AFBBN AFDZB AFHIU AFKRA AFLOW AFOHR AFQWF AFRAH AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGRTI AGUYK AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHMBA AHPBZ AHSBF AHWEU AHXOZ AHYZX AIAKS AIGIU AIIXL AILAN AITGF AIXLP AJRNO AJZVZ AKMHD ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ALWAN AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG AQVQM ARALO ARMRJ ASOEW ASPBG ATCPS ATHPR AVWKF AXYYD AYFIA AZFZN AZQEC B-. BA0 BBNVY BDATZ BENPR BGNMA BHPHI BPHCQ BSONS BVXVI CBGCD CCPQU CS3 CSCUP D0L D1J DDRTE DL5 DNIVK DPUIP DU5 DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS EDH EIOEI EJD EPAXT ESBYG EV9 F5P FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FWDCC FYUFA G-Y G-Z GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ7 GQ8 GXS HCIFZ HEHIP HF~ HG5 HG6 HMCUK HMJXF HQYDN HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ I09 IHE IJ- IKXTQ IPSME ITM IWAJR IXC IZIGR IZQ I~X I~Z J-C J0Z JAAYA JBMMH JBS JBSCW JCJTX JENOY JHFFW JKQEH JLS JLXEF JPM JST JZLTJ KDC KOV KPH LAS LK8 LLZTM M2M M2P M2R M2S M4Y M7P MA- MQGED NB0 NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O93 O9G O9I O9J OAM P19 P2P PATMY PF- PHGZM PHGZT PQQKQ PROAC PSYQQ PT4 PT5 PYCSY Q2X QOK QOR QOS R89 R9I RHV RNS ROL RPX RSV S16 S27 S3A S3B SA0 SAP SBL SBY SDH SDM SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SZN T13 TN5 TSG TSK TSV TUC U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UTJUX UZXMN VC2 VFIZW W23 W48 WH7 WJK WK8 YLTOR Z45 ZCA ZMTXR ZOVNA ~02 ~EX ~KM -4W -56 -5G -BR -EM -Y2 1SB 2.D 28- 2P1 2VQ 3SX 3V. 53G 5QI AAAVM AANXM AARHV AAYTO ABTAH ABULA ACBXY ADINQ ADULT ADYPR AEBTG AEFIE AEKMD AFEXP AFGCZ AGGDS AGJBK AI. AJBLW BBWZM CAG COF DOOOF EN4 GQ6 GTFYD H13 HTVGU JSODD KOW M0O MVM N2Q NDZJH O9- OVD R4E RIG RNI RZK S1Z S26 S28 SCLPG T16 TEORI VH1 WK6 XIH Y6R YIF Z7Y Z7Z Z83 Z8S Z8T Z8W ZXP ZY4 AAYXX ADHKG ADXHL AGQPQ CITATION 7QG 7QR 7SN 7SS 7XB 8FD 8FK ABRTQ C1K FR3 K9. P64 PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQGLB PQUKI PRINS PRQQA Q9U RC3 7S9 L.6 PUEGO |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c371t-be587e9e0c8323adbc20e7343fbae0db7f728b03c509b484578ea2ecde9892f33 |
IEDL.DBID | U2A |
ISSN | 0340-5443 |
IngestDate | Sun Aug 24 04:06:32 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 10:13:14 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:15:15 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:12:05 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:33:13 EST 2025 Thu Jun 19 22:09:38 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 4 |
Keywords | Biotremology Ontogenetic adaptation Risk assessment Vibration playback Environmentally cued hatching Embryo behavior |
Language | English |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c371t-be587e9e0c8323adbc20e7343fbae0db7f728b03c509b484578ea2ecde9892f33 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-7804-800X |
PQID | 2197977539 |
PQPubID | 54055 |
PageCount | 14 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2237499059 proquest_journals_2197977539 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2 crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2 springer_journals_10_1007_s00265_019_2663_2 jstor_primary_48705577 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20190401 20190400 2019-4-00 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-04-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 4 year: 2019 text: 20190401 day: 1 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Berlin/Heidelberg |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Berlin/Heidelberg – name: Heidelberg |
PublicationTitle | Behavioral ecology and sociobiology |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Behav Ecol Sociobiol |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
Publisher | Springer Science + Business Media Springer Berlin Heidelberg Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer Science + Business Media – name: Springer Berlin Heidelberg – name: Springer Nature B.V |
References | Dugatkin, Godin (CR13) 1992; 29 Darmaillacq, Lesimple, Dickel (CR10) 2008; 76 Warkentin (CR39) 2005; 70 Buckley, Michael, Irschick (CR4) 2005; 19 Doody, Paull (CR11) 2013; 2013 Kempster, Hart, Collin (CR23) 2013; 8 Touchon, Vonesh (CR34) 2016; 50 Brown, Iskandar (CR3) 2000; 34 Caldwell, McDaniel, Warkentin (CR5) 2009; 212 Griem, Martin (CR19) 2000; 137 Wiedenmayer (CR49) 2009; 33 FitzGibbon (CR16) 1994; 34 Hall, Oppenheim (CR21) 1987; 38 Gomez-Mestre, Wiens, Warkentin (CR18) 2008; 78 Oyarzun, Strathmann (CR29) 2011; 51 Ferrari, Chivers (CR15) 2009; 5 Warkentin (CR36) 1995; 92 Savage (CR31) 2002 Warkentin, Caldwell, Siok, D’Amato, McDaniel (CR45) 2007; 210 Warkentin, Caldwell, Dukas, Ratcliffe (CR42) 2009 Gomez-Mestre, Warkentin (CR17) 2007; 153 Whittington, Kearn (CR47) 1988; 18 Schneider, Rasband, Elicieri (CR32) 2012; 9 Warkentin (CR38) 2000; 60 Bradbury, Vehrencamp (CR2) 1998 Romagny, Darmaillacq, Guibe, Bellanger, Dickel (CR30) 2012; 215 Colombelli-Negrel, Hauber, Robertson, Sulloway, Hoi, Griggio, Kleindorfer (CR8) 2012; 22 Warkentin, Caldwell, McDaniel (CR44) 2006; 209 Mukai, Hironaka, Tojo, Nomakuchi (CR27) 2014; 9 Martin, Bailey, Moravek, Carlson (CR24) 2011; 51 Touchon, McCoy, Vonesh, Warkentin (CR35) 2013; 94 Endo, Takanashi, Mukai, Numata (CR14) 2019; 29 Warkentin (CR37) 1999; 10 Cohen, Seid, Warkentin (CR7) 2016; 219 Guibe, Poirel, Houde, Dickel (CR20) 2012; 84 Bate (CR1) 1999; 9 Dall, Giraldeau, Olsson, McNamara, Stephens (CR9) 2005; 20 Warkentin (CR40) 2011; 51 Mukai, Hironaka, Tojo, Nomakuchi (CR26) 2012; 84 Willink, Palmer, Landberg, Vonesh, Warkentin (CR50) 2014; 28 Caldwell, McDaniel, Warkentin (CR6) 2010; 79 Warkentin, Cuccaro Diaz, Güell, Jung, Kim, Cohen (CR46) 2017; 129 Mathis, Ferrari, Windel, Messier, Chivers (CR25) 2008; 275 Nishide, Tanaka (CR28) 2016; 70 Doody, Stewart, Camacho, Christian (CR12) 2012; 83 Warkentin, Buckley, Metcalf (CR43) 2006; 71 Harshaw, Lickliter (CR22) 2011; 53 Whittington, Kearn (CR48) 2011; 51 Warkentin (CR41) 2011; 51 Strathmann, Strathmann, Ruiz-Jones, Hadfield (CR33) 2010; 129 AS Darmaillacq (2663_CR10) 2008; 76 JS Doody (2663_CR12) 2012; 83 KM Warkentin (2663_CR41) 2011; 51 JW Bradbury (2663_CR2) 1998 CD FitzGibbon (2663_CR16) 1994; 34 RR Strathmann (2663_CR33) 2010; 129 J Endo (2663_CR14) 2019; 29 CP Wiedenmayer (2663_CR49) 2009; 33 H Mukai (2663_CR27) 2014; 9 H Mukai (2663_CR26) 2012; 84 JN Griem (2663_CR19) 2000; 137 JS Doody (2663_CR11) 2013; 2013 I Gomez-Mestre (2663_CR18) 2008; 78 KM Warkentin (2663_CR39) 2005; 70 MCO Ferrari (2663_CR15) 2009; 5 KL Cohen (2663_CR7) 2016; 219 JC Touchon (2663_CR34) 2016; 50 KM Warkentin (2663_CR36) 1995; 92 JC Touchon (2663_CR35) 2013; 94 K Martin (2663_CR24) 2011; 51 KM Warkentin (2663_CR46) 2017; 129 KM Warkentin (2663_CR38) 2000; 60 LA Dugatkin (2663_CR13) 1992; 29 MS Caldwell (2663_CR5) 2009; 212 KM Warkentin (2663_CR42) 2009 JM Savage (2663_CR31) 2002 A Mathis (2663_CR25) 2008; 275 FX Oyarzun (2663_CR29) 2011; 51 SRX Dall (2663_CR9) 2005; 20 I Gomez-Mestre (2663_CR17) 2007; 153 ID Whittington (2663_CR48) 2011; 51 Y Nishide (2663_CR28) 2016; 70 WG Hall (2663_CR21) 1987; 38 KM Warkentin (2663_CR40) 2011; 51 CR Buckley (2663_CR4) 2005; 19 ID Whittington (2663_CR47) 1988; 18 D Colombelli-Negrel (2663_CR8) 2012; 22 KM Warkentin (2663_CR44) 2006; 209 S Romagny (2663_CR30) 2012; 215 B Willink (2663_CR50) 2014; 28 KM Warkentin (2663_CR45) 2007; 210 KM Warkentin (2663_CR37) 1999; 10 RM Kempster (2663_CR23) 2013; 8 M Guibe (2663_CR20) 2012; 84 MS Caldwell (2663_CR6) 2010; 79 CA Schneider (2663_CR32) 2012; 9 KM Warkentin (2663_CR43) 2006; 71 M Bate (2663_CR1) 1999; 9 RM Brown (2663_CR3) 2000; 34 C Harshaw (2663_CR22) 2011; 53 |
References_xml | – start-page: 177 year: 2009 end-page: 200 ident: CR42 article-title: Assessing risk: embryos, information, and escape hatching publication-title: Cognitive ecology II doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.003.0010 – volume: 34 start-page: 139 year: 1994 end-page: 148 ident: CR16 article-title: The costs and benefits of predator inspection behaviour in Thompson’s gazelles publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/BF00164184 – year: 2002 ident: CR31 publication-title: The amphibians and reptiles of Costa Rica – volume: 79 start-page: 255 year: 2010 end-page: 260 ident: CR6 article-title: Is it safe? Red-eyed treefrog embryos assessing predation risk use two features of rain vibrations to avoid false alarms publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.005 – volume: 22 start-page: 2155 year: 2012 end-page: 2160 ident: CR8 article-title: Embryonic learning of vocal passwords in superb fairy-wrens reveals intruder cuckoo nestlings publication-title: Curr Biol doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025 – volume: 212 start-page: 566 year: 2009 end-page: 575 ident: CR5 article-title: Frequency information in the vibration-cued escape hatching of red-eyed treefrogs publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.026518 – volume: 94 start-page: 850 year: 2013 end-page: 860 ident: CR35 article-title: Effects of plastic hatching timing carry over through metamorphosis in red-eyed treefrogs publication-title: Ecology doi: 10.1890/12-0194.1 – year: 1998 ident: CR2 publication-title: Principles of animal communication – volume: 51 start-page: 81 year: 2011 end-page: 90 ident: CR29 article-title: Plasticity of hatching and duration of planktonic development in marine invertebrates publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr009 – volume: 51 start-page: 14 year: 2011 end-page: 25 ident: CR40 article-title: Environmentally cued hatching across taxa: embryos respond to risk and opportunity publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr017 – volume: 215 start-page: 4125 year: 2012 end-page: 4130 ident: CR30 article-title: Feel, smell and see in an egg: emergence of perception and learning in an immature invertebrate, the cuttlefish embryo publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.078295 – volume: 18 start-page: 847 year: 1988 end-page: 852 ident: CR47 article-title: Rapid hatching of mechanically-disturbed eggs of the monogenean gill parasite , with observations on sedimentation of egg bundles publication-title: Int J Parasitol doi: 10.1016/0020-7519(88)90127-0 – volume: 20 start-page: 187 year: 2005 end-page: 193 ident: CR9 article-title: Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010 – volume: 70 start-page: 1507 year: 2016 end-page: 1515 ident: CR28 article-title: Desert locust, , eggs hatch in synchrony in a mass but not when separated publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2159-2 – volume: 209 start-page: 1376 year: 2006 end-page: 1384 ident: CR44 article-title: Temporal pattern cues in vibrational risk assessment by red-eyed treefrog embryos, publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.02150 – volume: 210 start-page: 614 year: 2007 end-page: 619 ident: CR45 article-title: Flexible information sampling in vibrational assessment of predation risk by red-eyed treefrog embryos publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.001362 – volume: 78 start-page: 205 year: 2008 end-page: 224 ident: CR18 article-title: Evolution of adaptive plasticity: risk-sensitive hatching in Neotropical leaf-breeding treefrogs publication-title: Ecol Monogr doi: 10.1890/07-0529.1 – volume: 53 start-page: 291 year: 2011 end-page: 302 ident: CR22 article-title: Biased embryos: prenatal experience alters the postnatal malleability of auditory preferences in bobwhite quail publication-title: Dev Psychobiol doi: 10.1002/dev.20521 – volume: 92 start-page: 3507 year: 1995 end-page: 3510 ident: CR36 article-title: Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3507 – volume: 29 start-page: 233 year: 1992 end-page: 252 ident: CR13 article-title: Prey approaching predators: a cost-benefit perspective publication-title: Ann Zool Fenn – volume: 153 start-page: 197 year: 2007 end-page: 206 ident: CR17 article-title: To hatch and hatch not: similar selective trade-offs but different responses to egg predators in two closely related, syntopic treefrogs publication-title: Oecologia doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-0708-0 – volume: 219 start-page: 1875 year: 2016 end-page: 1883 ident: CR7 article-title: How embryos escape from danger: the mechanism of rapid, plastic hatching in red-eyed treefrogs publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.139519 – volume: 70 start-page: 59 year: 2005 end-page: 71 ident: CR39 article-title: How do embryos assess risk? Vibrational cues in predator-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrogs publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019 – volume: 28 start-page: 103 year: 2014 end-page: 116 ident: CR50 article-title: Environmental context shapes immediate and cumulative costs of risk-induced early hatching publication-title: Evol Ecol doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9661-z – volume: 76 start-page: 131 year: 2008 end-page: 134 ident: CR10 article-title: Embryonic visual learning in the cuttlefish, publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.006 – volume: 5 start-page: 160 year: 2009 end-page: 162 ident: CR15 article-title: Latent inhibition of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians publication-title: Biol Lett doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641 – volume: 50 start-page: 113 year: 2016 end-page: 119 ident: CR34 article-title: Variation in abundance and efficacy of tadpole predators in a Neotropical pond community publication-title: J Herpetol doi: 10.1670/14-111 – volume: 2013 start-page: 159 year: 2013 end-page: 164 ident: CR11 article-title: Hitting the ground running: environmentally cued hatching in a lizard publication-title: Copeia doi: 10.1643/CE-12-111 – volume: 51 start-page: 111 year: 2011 end-page: 127 ident: CR41 article-title: Plasticity of hatching in amphibians: evolution, trade-offs, cues and mechanisms publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr046 – volume: 84 start-page: 1443 year: 2012 end-page: 1448 ident: CR26 article-title: Maternal vibration induces synchronous hatching in a subsocial burrower bug publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.012 – volume: 29 start-page: 1 year: 2019 end-page: 6 ident: CR14 article-title: Egg-cracking vibration as a cue for stink bug siblings to synchronize hatching publication-title: Curr Biol doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.024 – volume: 60 start-page: 503 year: 2000 end-page: 510 ident: CR38 article-title: Wasp predation and wasp-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrog eggs publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1508 – volume: 38 start-page: 91 year: 1987 end-page: 128 ident: CR21 article-title: Developmental psychobiology: prenatal, perinatal, and early postnatal aspects of behavioral development publication-title: Annu Rev Psychol doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.000515 – volume: 9 start-page: 671 year: 2012 end-page: 675 ident: CR32 article-title: NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis publication-title: Nat Methods doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089 – volume: 10 start-page: 251 year: 1999 end-page: 262 ident: CR37 article-title: The development of behavioral defenses: a mechanistic analysis of vulnerability in red-eyed tree frog hatchlings publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.3.251 – volume: 19 start-page: 67 year: 2005 end-page: 72 ident: CR4 article-title: Early hatching decreases jumping performance in a direct-developing frog, publication-title: Funct Ecol doi: 10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00931.x – volume: 51 start-page: 91 year: 2011 end-page: 99 ident: CR48 article-title: Hatching strategies in monogenean (platyhelminth) parasites that facilitate host infection publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr003 – volume: 84 start-page: 213 year: 2012 end-page: 217 ident: CR20 article-title: Food imprinting and visual generalization in embryos and newly hatched cuttlefish, publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.035 – volume: 129 start-page: 309 year: 2010 end-page: 318 ident: CR33 article-title: Effect of plasticity in hatching on duration as a precompetent swimming larva in the nudibranch publication-title: Invertebr Biol doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00212.x – volume: 8 start-page: e52551 year: 2013 ident: CR23 article-title: Survival of the stillest: predator avoidance in shark embryos publication-title: PLoS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052551 – volume: 34 start-page: 404 year: 2000 end-page: 413 ident: CR3 article-title: Nest site selection, larval hatching, and advertisement calls of from southwestern Sulawesi (Celebes) Island, Indonesia publication-title: J Herpetol doi: 10.2307/1565364 – volume: 83 start-page: 645 year: 2012 end-page: 651 ident: CR12 article-title: Good vibrations? Sibling embryos expedite hatching in a turtle publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.006 – volume: 51 start-page: 26 year: 2011 end-page: 37 ident: CR24 article-title: Taking the plunge: California grunion embryos emerge rapidly with environmentally cued hatching (ECH) publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr037 – volume: 9 start-page: e87932 year: 2014 ident: CR27 article-title: Maternal vibration: an important cue for embryo hatching in a subsocial shield bug publication-title: PLoS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087932 – volume: 9 start-page: 670 year: 1999 end-page: 675 ident: CR1 article-title: Development of motor behaviour publication-title: Curr Opin Neurobiol doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00031-8 – volume: 71 start-page: 417 year: 2006 end-page: 425 ident: CR43 article-title: Development of red-eyed treefrog eggs affects efficiency and choices of egg-foraging wasps publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.007 – volume: 137 start-page: 177 year: 2000 end-page: 181 ident: CR19 article-title: Wave action: the environmental trigger for hatching in the California grunion (Teleostei: Atherinopsidae) publication-title: Mar Biol doi: 10.1007/s002270000329 – volume: 275 start-page: 2603 year: 2008 end-page: 2607 ident: CR25 article-title: Learning by embryos and the ghost of predation future publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0754 – volume: 129 start-page: 103 year: 2017 end-page: 112 ident: CR46 article-title: Developmental onset of escape-hatching responses in red-eyed treefrogs depends on cue type publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.008 – volume: 33 start-page: 432 year: 2009 end-page: 441 ident: CR49 article-title: Plasticity of defensive behavior and fear in early development publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004 – volume: 34 start-page: 404 year: 2000 ident: 2663_CR3 publication-title: J Herpetol doi: 10.2307/1565364 – volume: 209 start-page: 1376 year: 2006 ident: 2663_CR44 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.02150 – volume: 212 start-page: 566 year: 2009 ident: 2663_CR5 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.026518 – volume-title: The amphibians and reptiles of Costa Rica year: 2002 ident: 2663_CR31 – volume: 210 start-page: 614 year: 2007 ident: 2663_CR45 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.001362 – volume: 76 start-page: 131 year: 2008 ident: 2663_CR10 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.006 – volume: 8 start-page: e52551 year: 2013 ident: 2663_CR23 publication-title: PLoS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052551 – volume: 129 start-page: 103 year: 2017 ident: 2663_CR46 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.008 – volume: 9 start-page: 670 year: 1999 ident: 2663_CR1 publication-title: Curr Opin Neurobiol doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00031-8 – volume: 9 start-page: 671 year: 2012 ident: 2663_CR32 publication-title: Nat Methods doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089 – volume: 22 start-page: 2155 year: 2012 ident: 2663_CR8 publication-title: Curr Biol doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025 – volume: 28 start-page: 103 year: 2014 ident: 2663_CR50 publication-title: Evol Ecol doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9661-z – volume: 84 start-page: 1443 year: 2012 ident: 2663_CR26 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.012 – volume: 29 start-page: 1 year: 2019 ident: 2663_CR14 publication-title: Curr Biol doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.024 – volume: 60 start-page: 503 year: 2000 ident: 2663_CR38 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1508 – volume: 2013 start-page: 159 year: 2013 ident: 2663_CR11 publication-title: Copeia doi: 10.1643/CE-12-111 – volume: 153 start-page: 197 year: 2007 ident: 2663_CR17 publication-title: Oecologia doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-0708-0 – start-page: 177 volume-title: Cognitive ecology II year: 2009 ident: 2663_CR42 doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226169378.003.0010 – volume-title: Principles of animal communication year: 1998 ident: 2663_CR2 – volume: 10 start-page: 251 year: 1999 ident: 2663_CR37 publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.3.251 – volume: 51 start-page: 26 year: 2011 ident: 2663_CR24 publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr037 – volume: 70 start-page: 1507 year: 2016 ident: 2663_CR28 publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2159-2 – volume: 92 start-page: 3507 year: 1995 ident: 2663_CR36 publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3507 – volume: 51 start-page: 14 year: 2011 ident: 2663_CR40 publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr017 – volume: 50 start-page: 113 year: 2016 ident: 2663_CR34 publication-title: J Herpetol doi: 10.1670/14-111 – volume: 51 start-page: 91 year: 2011 ident: 2663_CR48 publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr003 – volume: 5 start-page: 160 year: 2009 ident: 2663_CR15 publication-title: Biol Lett doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641 – volume: 20 start-page: 187 year: 2005 ident: 2663_CR9 publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010 – volume: 38 start-page: 91 year: 1987 ident: 2663_CR21 publication-title: Annu Rev Psychol doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.000515 – volume: 275 start-page: 2603 year: 2008 ident: 2663_CR25 publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0754 – volume: 94 start-page: 850 year: 2013 ident: 2663_CR35 publication-title: Ecology doi: 10.1890/12-0194.1 – volume: 53 start-page: 291 year: 2011 ident: 2663_CR22 publication-title: Dev Psychobiol doi: 10.1002/dev.20521 – volume: 33 start-page: 432 year: 2009 ident: 2663_CR49 publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004 – volume: 34 start-page: 139 year: 1994 ident: 2663_CR16 publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/BF00164184 – volume: 219 start-page: 1875 year: 2016 ident: 2663_CR7 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.139519 – volume: 78 start-page: 205 year: 2008 ident: 2663_CR18 publication-title: Ecol Monogr doi: 10.1890/07-0529.1 – volume: 51 start-page: 81 year: 2011 ident: 2663_CR29 publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr009 – volume: 18 start-page: 847 year: 1988 ident: 2663_CR47 publication-title: Int J Parasitol doi: 10.1016/0020-7519(88)90127-0 – volume: 83 start-page: 645 year: 2012 ident: 2663_CR12 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.006 – volume: 129 start-page: 309 year: 2010 ident: 2663_CR33 publication-title: Invertebr Biol doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7410.2010.00212.x – volume: 79 start-page: 255 year: 2010 ident: 2663_CR6 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.005 – volume: 70 start-page: 59 year: 2005 ident: 2663_CR39 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019 – volume: 29 start-page: 233 year: 1992 ident: 2663_CR13 publication-title: Ann Zool Fenn – volume: 215 start-page: 4125 year: 2012 ident: 2663_CR30 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.078295 – volume: 137 start-page: 177 year: 2000 ident: 2663_CR19 publication-title: Mar Biol doi: 10.1007/s002270000329 – volume: 84 start-page: 213 year: 2012 ident: 2663_CR20 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.035 – volume: 71 start-page: 417 year: 2006 ident: 2663_CR43 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.06.007 – volume: 51 start-page: 111 year: 2011 ident: 2663_CR41 publication-title: Integr Comp Biol doi: 10.1093/icb/icr046 – volume: 19 start-page: 67 year: 2005 ident: 2663_CR4 publication-title: Funct Ecol doi: 10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00931.x – volume: 9 start-page: e87932 year: 2014 ident: 2663_CR27 publication-title: PLoS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087932 |
SSID | ssj0006188 |
Score | 2.320623 |
Snippet | As animals develop, their abilities and needs change, altering cost/benefit trade-offs and optimal behavior responses to cues. We tested if ontogenetic changes... |
SourceID | proquest crossref springer jstor |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Enrichment Source Index Database Publisher |
StartPage | 1 |
SubjectTerms | Agalychnis callidryas Alarms Animal behavior Animal Ecology Animal populations animals behavior change Behavioral Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences Cues Decision making Developmental stages eggs Embryos Frequency spectrum Frogs Hatching learning Life Sciences mortality Ontogeny ORIGINAL ARTICLE Playback Playbacks Predation Predators prediction rain risk Risk acceptance Risk assessment Sampling tadpoles Tradeoffs Vibration Vibration control Vibrations Zoology |
Subtitle | vibrational cue use changes as predicted from costs of sampling and false alarms |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: Health & Medical Collection dbid: 7X7 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3di9QwEB_05OBeRFcXV0-J4JMS7CbtpvVFRO44fNAXDxZfSpJO8EHb9dJV7r93Jv1YTvDeCplOC_OR32QmMwCv8qLYWM327UKQ3L9EOkIFslwXRuUeg8NUbfF5c3GZf9oW2_HALY5llZNPTI666Tyfkb8lyzKEVQpdvd_9kjw1irOr4wiNu3CPW5dxSZfZzgEXbVVp7mSm80xyn7cpq5mlJqJqw2VrlaQtSkt1Y18aShNvgM5_8qRp-zl_APdH3Cg-DIJ-CHewXcDxMEnyegHLs8OFNSIbLTYu4GT2cER1_K1LT4_gz5e270h3rkUXBEaugpLfySvzeZRoxrk78Z34zdH0cFwo_B7FPqIY7gpHYaPYXXGeh0Cr4Gsqwnexj8wxWi5UJ1a2pSXScBSWQuif8TFcnp99_XghxxkM0muz7qXDojRYYebJ9LVtnFcZGp3r4CxmjTPBqNJl2hPwcHmZkwNAq9A3WJWVClov4ajtWnwCIhCU0uu8sJtC57YpK1v6gIUmPtY2oVlBNkmg9mODcp6T8aOeWysnodUktJqFVqsVvJ5f2Q3dOW4jXiaxzpQUq3EHMrOC00nO9WjBsT7o2wpezstke5xQsS12e6JR2lDESAh1BW8m_Tiw-O-fPL39g8_gRLFipvqgUzjqr_b4nKBP714k_f4Lk6f_kA priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest |
Title | Ontogeny of escape-hatching decisions |
URI | https://www.jstor.org/stable/48705577 https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00265-019-2663-2 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2197977539 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2237499059 |
Volume | 73 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3di9NAEB_0joN7Ea0Wo2dZwSdlIc1mu4lvrbQeClXEQvUl7G52uQdNjttUuf_emXwdJyr4lEAmQ2A-9jeZL4AXqZQLLci-jfec5pdwg6iAZ3OpktQ6b1xbbbFdnO_Sd3u57_u4w1DtPqQkW089NrtRuECFZjnHQ0Vw9LvHkkJ3VOJdshzd72LeLpuMRRpzGu42pDL_xOLWYdTVI95Cmr8lR9szZ3Mf7vVgkS076T6AO66awEm3PvJ6AtP1TZcakvVmGiZwOro1pDr5Wrd3D-Hnh6qpUWGuWe2ZC1T6xC_QFdNPKFb2y3bCa_aDQujuHyGzB8cOwbGuQTgwHdjlFSV3EKky6k1htg5NII5BU3U6stIVPkK1dkxj3Pw9PILdZv35zTnvFy9wK9S84cbJTLncxRbtXejS2CR2SqTCG-3i0iivkszEwiLaMGmWotU7nThbujzLEy_EFI6qunKPgXnET2KeSr2QItVlluvMeicF8tG69GUE8SCBwvZTyWk5xrdinKfcCq1AoRUktCKJ4OX4ymU3kuNfxNNWrCMlBmg0dkxFcDbIuejNNhTovhUCYinyCJ6Pj9HgKIuiK1cfkCYRCsNEhKURvBr044bFX7_kyX9RP4XThPS0rRE6g6Pm6uCeIfxpzAzuqr2awfFys1pt6fr2y_s1Xlfr7cdPs9YYfgF5pQGN |
linkProvider | Springer Nature |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3da9RAEB_KlWJfRE8Po1VX0BdlMbebT0HEjytXW0-RFoov6e5mgw-anN2c5f4p_0Zn8nVUsG99C-xkE5jffO3MzgA8DcIwUpLkWxcFp_4lXKNXwJNpGIvA2ELbptpiEc1Pgo-n4ekW_OnvwlBZZa8TG0WdV4bOyF-iZMXoq4QyfbP8xWlqFGVX-xEaLSwO7foCQzb3-uAD8veZEPuz4_dz3k0V4EbG05prGyaxTa1vEMxS5doI38YykIVW1s91XMQi0b40aEp1kAQIaauENblNk1QUdACKKn87kBjKjGD73Wzx5eug-6NpM-nSl4HPqbNcn0f1m7alIqJCuZSjUZRcXLKEbTHkJTf3n8xsY_D2b8HNzlNlb1to3YYtW45hp51duR7DZLa5IodknY5wY9gddCpS7Xyrmqc7cPG5rCtE65pVBbOO6q74d7QDdALG8m7Sj3vFflP83h5QMrOybOUsa28nO6YcW55TZgndZEYXY5ipXO1oR6eoNB63UiUuoUxZpjBo_-nuwsm18GcCo7Iq7T1gBTpvchqEKgploPIkVYkpbChxH6XyIvfA7zmQma4lOk3m-JENzZwbpmXItIyYlgkPng-vLNt-IFcRTxq2DpQYHVLPs9iDvZ7PWaczXLZBuAdPhmWUdkrhqNJWK6QRMsYYFX1iD170-Nhs8d8_uX_1Bx_Djfnxp6Ps6GBx-AB2BYG0qU7ag1F9vrIP0fGq9aMO7QzOrlvA_gJOoT7W |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3da9RAEB9KpdIX0dPDaNUV9EVZmttNbhNBRGyP1kr1wcLhS9xsdumDJmf3znL_mn-dM5uPo4J961tgJ5vA_OZrZ3YG4EWSplMtSb5L5zj1L-ElegU8m6RKJMa60oZqi9Pp0VnycZ7Ot-BPfxeGyip7nRgUddUYOiPfR8lS6KukMt93XVnEl4PZu8UvThOkKNPaj9NoIXJi15cYvvm3xwfI65dCzA6_fjji3YQBbqSaLHlp00zZ3MYGgS11VRoRWyUT6Upt46pUTomsjKVBs1omWYLwtlpYU9k8y4Wjw1BU_7eUTCckY2o-BHtoJsPMy1gmMacec31GNQ4NTMWUSuZyjuZRcnHFJrZlkVcc3n9ytMH0ze7Cnc5nZe9bkN2DLVuPYKedYrkewfhwc1kOyTpt4UewO2hXpNr51oSn-3D5uV42iNs1axyzniqw-DlaBDoLY1U388e_Yb8pkm-PKplZWbbylrX3lD3Tni0uKMeEDjOjKzLMNH7paUevqUget9I1LqF0WaYxfP_pH8DZjXBnDNt1U9uHwBy6cXKSpHqaykRXWa4z42wqcR-tK1dFEPccKEzXHJ1mdPwohrbOgWkFMq0gphUiglfDK4u2M8h1xOPA1oES40TqfqYi2Ov5XHTawxcbrEfwfFhGuadkjq5ts0IaIRVGq-gdR_C6x8dmi__-yaPrP_gMbqNYFZ-OT08ew64gjIYypT3YXl6s7BP0wJbl0wB1Bt9vWrb-ArLlQaY |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Ontogeny+of+escape-hatching+decisions&rft.jtitle=Behavioral+ecology+and+sociobiology&rft.au=Warkentin%2C+Karen+M.&rft.au=Jung%2C+Julie&rft.au=Solano%2C+L.+Alberto+Rueda&rft.au=McDaniel%2C+J.+Gregory&rft.date=2019-04-01&rft.pub=Springer+Science+%2B+Business+Media&rft.issn=0340-5443&rft.eissn=1432-0762&rft.volume=73&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=1&rft.epage=14&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs00265-019-2663-2&rft.externalDocID=48705577 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0340-5443&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0340-5443&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0340-5443&client=summon |