Examining the Effect of Interruptions at Different Breakpoints and Frequencies Within a Task
Objective The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries. Background Research on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be e...
Saved in:
Published in | Human factors Vol. 65; no. 1; pp. 22 - 36 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Los Angeles, CA
SAGE Publications
01.02.2023
Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Objective
The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries.
Background
Research on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be either disruptive or beneficial, depending on which aspects of an interruption are manipulated. Two important aspects that contribute to these conflicting results concern when and how often interruptions occur.
Method
Undergraduates completed a trip planning task divided into three subtasks. The within-subjects factor was interruption timing with three levels: none, coarse breakpoints, and fine breakpoints. The between-subjects factor was interruption frequency with two levels: one and three. The dependent measures included resumption lag, number of errors, mental workload, and frustration.
Results
Participants took longer to resume the primary task and reported higher mental workload when interruptions occurred at fine breakpoints. The effect of interruptions at coarse breakpoints was similar to completing the task without interruption. Interruption frequency had no effect on performance; however, participants spent significantly longer attending to interruptions in the initial task, and within a task, the first and second interruptions were attended to significantly longer than the third interruption.
Conclusion
The disruptiveness of an interruption is tied to the point within the task hierarchy where it occurs.
Application
The performance cost associated with interruptions must be considered within the task structure. Interruptions occurring at coarse breakpoints may not be disruptive or have a negative effect on mental workload. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Objective
The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries.
Background
Research on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be either disruptive or beneficial, depending on which aspects of an interruption are manipulated. Two important aspects that contribute to these conflicting results concern when and how often interruptions occur.
Method
Undergraduates completed a trip planning task divided into three subtasks. The within-subjects factor was interruption timing with three levels: none, coarse breakpoints, and fine breakpoints. The between-subjects factor was interruption frequency with two levels: one and three. The dependent measures included resumption lag, number of errors, mental workload, and frustration.
Results
Participants took longer to resume the primary task and reported higher mental workload when interruptions occurred at fine breakpoints. The effect of interruptions at coarse breakpoints was similar to completing the task without interruption. Interruption frequency had no effect on performance; however, participants spent significantly longer attending to interruptions in the initial task, and within a task, the first and second interruptions were attended to significantly longer than the third interruption.
Conclusion
The disruptiveness of an interruption is tied to the point within the task hierarchy where it occurs.
Application
The performance cost associated with interruptions must be considered within the task structure. Interruptions occurring at coarse breakpoints may not be disruptive or have a negative effect on mental workload. Objective The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries. Background Research on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be either disruptive or beneficial, depending on which aspects of an interruption are manipulated. Two important aspects that contribute to these conflicting results concern when and how often interruptions occur. Method Undergraduates completed a trip planning task divided into three subtasks. The within-subjects factor was interruption timing with three levels: none, coarse breakpoints, and fine breakpoints. The between-subjects factor was interruption frequency with two levels: one and three. The dependent measures included resumption lag, number of errors, mental workload, and frustration. Results Participants took longer to resume the primary task and reported higher mental workload when interruptions occurred at fine breakpoints. The effect of interruptions at coarse breakpoints was similar to completing the task without interruption. Interruption frequency had no effect on performance; however, participants spent significantly longer attending to interruptions in the initial task, and within a task, the first and second interruptions were attended to significantly longer than the third interruption. Conclusion The disruptiveness of an interruption is tied to the point within the task hierarchy where it occurs. Application The performance cost associated with interruptions must be considered within the task structure. Interruptions occurring at coarse breakpoints may not be disruptive or have a negative effect on mental workload. The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries. Research on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be either disruptive or beneficial, depending on which aspects of an interruption are manipulated. Two important aspects that contribute to these conflicting results concern when and how often interruptions occur. Undergraduates completed a trip planning task divided into three subtasks. The within-subjects factor was interruption timing with three levels: none, coarse breakpoints, and fine breakpoints. The between-subjects factor was interruption frequency with two levels: one and three. The dependent measures included resumption lag, number of errors, mental workload, and frustration. Participants took longer to resume the primary task and reported higher mental workload when interruptions occurred at fine breakpoints. The effect of interruptions at coarse breakpoints was similar to completing the task without interruption. Interruption frequency had no effect on performance; however, participants spent significantly longer attending to interruptions in the initial task, and within a task, the first and second interruptions were attended to significantly longer than the third interruption. The disruptiveness of an interruption is tied to the point within the task hierarchy where it occurs. The performance cost associated with interruptions must be considered within the task structure. Interruptions occurring at coarse breakpoints may not be disruptive or have a negative effect on mental workload. The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries.OBJECTIVEThe purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task analysis (HTA) to identify coarse and fine event boundaries.Research on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be either disruptive or beneficial, depending on which aspects of an interruption are manipulated. Two important aspects that contribute to these conflicting results concern when and how often interruptions occur.BACKGROUNDResearch on the effects of interruptions shows that they can be either disruptive or beneficial, depending on which aspects of an interruption are manipulated. Two important aspects that contribute to these conflicting results concern when and how often interruptions occur.Undergraduates completed a trip planning task divided into three subtasks. The within-subjects factor was interruption timing with three levels: none, coarse breakpoints, and fine breakpoints. The between-subjects factor was interruption frequency with two levels: one and three. The dependent measures included resumption lag, number of errors, mental workload, and frustration.METHODUndergraduates completed a trip planning task divided into three subtasks. The within-subjects factor was interruption timing with three levels: none, coarse breakpoints, and fine breakpoints. The between-subjects factor was interruption frequency with two levels: one and three. The dependent measures included resumption lag, number of errors, mental workload, and frustration.Participants took longer to resume the primary task and reported higher mental workload when interruptions occurred at fine breakpoints. The effect of interruptions at coarse breakpoints was similar to completing the task without interruption. Interruption frequency had no effect on performance; however, participants spent significantly longer attending to interruptions in the initial task, and within a task, the first and second interruptions were attended to significantly longer than the third interruption.RESULTSParticipants took longer to resume the primary task and reported higher mental workload when interruptions occurred at fine breakpoints. The effect of interruptions at coarse breakpoints was similar to completing the task without interruption. Interruption frequency had no effect on performance; however, participants spent significantly longer attending to interruptions in the initial task, and within a task, the first and second interruptions were attended to significantly longer than the third interruption.The disruptiveness of an interruption is tied to the point within the task hierarchy where it occurs.CONCLUSIONThe disruptiveness of an interruption is tied to the point within the task hierarchy where it occurs.The performance cost associated with interruptions must be considered within the task structure. Interruptions occurring at coarse breakpoints may not be disruptive or have a negative effect on mental workload.APPLICATIONThe performance cost associated with interruptions must be considered within the task structure. Interruptions occurring at coarse breakpoints may not be disruptive or have a negative effect on mental workload. |
Author | Scerbo, Mark W. Powers, Sarah A. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Sarah A. orcidid: 0000-0003-2976-5384 surname: Powers fullname: Powers, Sarah A. – sequence: 2 givenname: Mark W. surname: Scerbo fullname: Scerbo, Mark W. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33861143$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kctKBDEQRYMoOj4-wI0E3LhpTXXSnWTpY3yA4EZxIzSZTEWjM-kxSYP-vT2OD1B0lcU9p7ipWifLoQ1IyDawfQApDxgDJUumSgDGNAO2RAZQCVkoULBMBvO8mANrZD2lR8ZYrXm1StY4VzWA4ANyN3wxUx98uKf5AenQObSZto5ehIwxdrPs25CoyfTE91nEkOlRRPM0a33IfRDG9DTic4fBekz01ucHH6ih1yY9bZIVZyYJtz7eDXJzOrw-Pi8ur84ujg8vC8trlQvHuHFcOqVZKUutRlaCEVK-f248cq5SUow0ByuVcyVqFFAzUwllxwbA8g2yt5g7i23fJOVm6pPFycQEbLvUlBWISnPOdI_u_kAf2y6Gvl1TyrrSQpRS9NTOB9WNpjhuZtFPTXxtPvfWA7AAbGxTiui-EGDN_DbNr9v0jvzhWJ_NfL85Gj_519xfmMnc43fhv4U3D0ecdQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2024_3383142 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2024_2439621 crossref_primary_10_1097_NNE_0000000000001583 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_74244_4 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0314183 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ergon_2024_103649 crossref_primary_10_1177_00187208221150353 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1465323 crossref_primary_10_1177_00222429241304322 crossref_primary_10_1080_24721840_2024_2311706 |
Cites_doi | 10.1111/j.1540-5915.1999.tb01613.x 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2004.00161.x 10.1037/a0021912 10.1037/0096-3445.130.1.29 10.1136/amiajnl-2010-000024 10.1016/B978-044451020-4/50031-1 10.1518/155723408X299852 10.1038/88486 10.1016/j.intcom.2007.01.004 10.1002/hfm.20603 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.273 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3 10.1007/978-3-540-74800-7_39 10.1016/j.apergo.2005.06.003 10.1145/1054972.1055016 10.1207/S15327663JCP1203_08 10.1145/1314683.1314689 10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00023-5 10.1145/1753326.1753341 10.1201/9781410607775.ch2 10.1080/10615806.2015.1011141 10.1093/ajhp/56.13.1319 10.1007/BF00309260 10.1177/0018720817732605 10.3758/BF03196473 10.1201/b10420-17 10.1016/j.accinf.2009.10.003 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62386-9 10.1145/1056808.1056948 10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1120 10.1037/a0014402 10.5898/JHRI.2.1.Trafton 10.1080/713827180 10.1136/qshc.2006.019711 10.1177/154193120404800304 10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.568 10.1145/985692.985727 10.1037/a0023497 10.1177/001872089303500205 10.1348/096317999166581 10.1207/s15516709cog2601_2 10.3758/BF03193094 10.1177/1555343412441001 10.1201/b15703-13 10.1016/j.chb.2005.12.009 10.1145/1518701.1518981 10.1201/9781420063363 10.1145/223355.223665 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2021, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2021, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QF 7QQ 7SC 7SE 7SP 7SR 7T2 7TA 7TB 7TK 7U5 8BQ 8FD C1K F28 FR3 H8D H8G JG9 JQ2 K9. KR7 L7M L~C L~D 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1177/00187208211009010 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Aluminium Industry Abstracts Ceramic Abstracts Computer and Information Systems Abstracts Corrosion Abstracts Electronics & Communications Abstracts Engineered Materials Abstracts Health and Safety Science Abstracts (Full archive) Materials Business File Mechanical & Transportation Engineering Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Solid State and Superconductivity Abstracts METADEX Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ANTE: Abstracts in New Technology & Engineering Engineering Research Database Aerospace Database Copper Technical Reference Library Materials Research Database ProQuest Computer Science Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Civil Engineering Abstracts Advanced Technologies Database with Aerospace Computer and Information Systems Abstracts Academic Computer and Information Systems Abstracts Professional MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Materials Research Database Technology Research Database Computer and Information Systems Abstracts – Academic Mechanical & Transportation Engineering Abstracts ProQuest Computer Science Collection Computer and Information Systems Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Materials Business File Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Aerospace Database Copper Technical Reference Library Engineered Materials Abstracts Health & Safety Science Abstracts Advanced Technologies Database with Aerospace ANTE: Abstracts in New Technology & Engineering Civil Engineering Abstracts Aluminium Industry Abstracts Electronics & Communications Abstracts Ceramic Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts METADEX Computer and Information Systems Abstracts Professional Solid State and Superconductivity Abstracts Engineering Research Database Corrosion Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Materials Research Database MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Engineering Psychology |
EISSN | 1547-8181 |
EndPage | 36 |
ExternalDocumentID | 33861143 10_1177_00187208211009010 10.1177_00187208211009010 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -TM -TN -~X .2G .2L .2N .55 .DC .GJ 01A 09Z 0R~ 1~K 29I 2FS 31W 31X 31Z 36B 3EH 4.4 53G 54M 5GY 5VS 7X2 7X7 7XC 85S 88E 88I 8AF 8CJ 8FE 8FG 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8G5 8R4 8R5 AABOD AACKU AACTG AADIR AADUE AAGGD AAGLT AAJOX AAJPV AAKTJ AAMFR AANSI AAPEO AAQDB AAQXI AARIX AATAA AATBZ AAWLO AAWTL AAYJJ AAYTG ABAWP ABCCA ABCJG ABDMP ABDPE ABDWY ABEIX ABFWQ ABFXH ABGFU ABHKI ABIDT ABIVO ABJCF ABKRH ABLUO ABPNF ABQKF ABQPY ABQXT ABRHV ABUJY ABUWG ABYTW ACABN ACAEP ACDXX ACFUR ACFZE ACGBL ACGFS ACGOD ACHQT ACIWK ACJER ACLZU ACOFE ACOXC ACPRK ACROE ACRPL ACSIQ ACUAV ACUFS ACUIR ACXKE ADBBV ADDLC ADEBD ADEIA ADMHG ADNMO ADNON ADNWM ADPEE ADRRZ ADSTG ADTBJ ADTOS ADUKL ADVBO ADXHL ADYCS AEDFJ AEDXQ AEILP AENEX AEOBU AEONT AEPTA AEQLS AESMA AESZF AEUHG AEUYN AEVPJ AEWDL AEWHI AEXNY AFEET AFFNX AFKBI AFKRA AFKRG AFMOU AFQAA AFRAH AFUIA AFWMB AGDVU AGKLV AGNHF AGNWV AGQPQ AGWFA AHDMH AHHFK AHMBA AHOJL AHWHD AI. AIZZC AJEFB AJUXI AJUZI ALEEW ALFTD ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ANDLU AQUVI ARTOV ASPBG ATCPS AUTPY AUVAJ AVQMV AVWKF AYAKG AYPQM AZFZN AZQEC B8T B8Z B94 BBNVY BBRGL BCR BCU BDDNI BDZRT BEC BENPR BES BGLVJ BHPHI BKOMP BKSAR BLC BMVBW BPACV BPHCQ BVXVI BYIEH CAG CBRKF CCGJY CCPQU CEADM CFDXU COF CORYS D1J D1K DD0 DD~ DE- DG~ DH. DO- DOPDO DU5 DV7 DV8 DWQXO D~Y EBS EJD F5P FAC FAS FEDTE FHBDP FJW FYUFA GNUQQ GROUPED_SAGE_PREMIER_JOURNAL_COLLECTION GUQSH H13 HCIFZ HF~ HMCUK HVGLF HZ~ IAO ICD ICJ IEA IGS IHR IOF IPO IPY ITC J8X K.F K50 K6- KQ4 L6V LK5 LK8 LPU M0K M0T M1D M1P M2M M2O M2P M2Q M7P M7R M7S N9A O9- OHT P.B P2P PADUT PATMY PCBAR PEA PHGZM PHGZT PMKZF PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ PTHSS PYCSY Q1R Q2X Q7O Q7P Q7V Q7X Q82 Q83 ROL S01 S0X SAUOL SCNPE SFC SGU SGV SGZ SJFOW SPV SSDHQ TN5 U5U UAP UBX UHB UKHRP UPT VH1 WH7 X7M XH6 XOL XZL YNY ZGI ZPLXX ZPPRI ZRKOI ZXP ~02 ~32 AAYXX ACCVC AJGYC AMNSR CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF M4V NPM 7QF 7QQ 7SC 7SE 7SP 7SR 7T2 7TA 7TB 7TK 7U5 8BQ 8FD AAPII AJHME AJVBE C1K F28 FR3 H8D H8G JG9 JQ2 K9. KR7 L7M L~C L~D 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c368t-f03af37f89027298bc71a47718720dbff5874b931c78ff2e9e4160a548cda11c3 |
ISSN | 0018-7208 1547-8181 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 15:29:04 EDT 2025 Sat Aug 16 17:21:03 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:00:16 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 05:28:11 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:05:25 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 22:30:22 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | memory for goals theory performance hierarchical task analysis event segmentation theory mental workload |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c368t-f03af37f89027298bc71a47718720dbff5874b931c78ff2e9e4160a548cda11c3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-2976-5384 |
PMID | 33861143 |
PQID | 2765944274 |
PQPubID | 48481 |
PageCount | 15 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2514593309 proquest_journals_2765944274 pubmed_primary_33861143 crossref_primary_10_1177_00187208211009010 crossref_citationtrail_10_1177_00187208211009010 sage_journals_10_1177_00187208211009010 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20230200 2023-02-00 20230201 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2023-02-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 2 year: 2023 text: 20230200 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Los Angeles, CA |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Los Angeles, CA – name: United States – name: Santa Monica |
PublicationTitle | Human factors |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Hum Factors |
PublicationYear | 2023 |
Publisher | SAGE Publications Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
Publisher_xml | – name: SAGE Publications – name: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
References | Zacks, Speer, Swallow, Braver, Reynolds 2007; 133 Campoe, Giuliano 2017; 59 Bailey, Konstan, Carlis 2001 Hodgetts, Jones 2006; 32 Speier, Valacich, Vessey 1999; 30 Wierwille, Eggemeier 1993; 35 Graziano, Moore, Collins 1988; 24 Zhang, Zhang, Yang 2003; 17 Crystal, Ellington 2004 Rubio, Díaz, Martín, Puente 2004; 53 Altmann, Trafton 2002; 26 Trafton, Hiatt, Harrison, Tanborello, Khemlani, Schultz 2013; 2 Dorneich, Ververs, Mathan, Whitlow, Hayes 2012; 6 Zijlstra, Roe, Leonora, Krediet 1999; 72 Bailey, Iqbal 2008; 14 Zacks, Tversky, Iyer 2001; 130 Bailey, Busbey, Iqbal 2007; 19 Tams, Thatcher, Grover, Pak 2015; 28 Lee, Duffy 2015; 25 Trafton, Altmann, Brock, Mintz 2003; 58 Cades, Boehm-Davis, Trafton, Monk 2011; 17 Bailey, Konstan 2006; 22 Basoglu, Fuller, Sweeney 2009; 10 Monk, Trafton, Boehm-Davis 2008; 14 Healey, Primus, Koutantji 2007; 16 Altmann, Trafton 2007; 14 Grundgeiger, Sanderson, MacDougall, Venkatesh 2010; 16 Gillie, Broadbent 1989; 50 Horton, Rapp 2003; 10 Monk 2004; 48 Xiao, Wang, Wang, Lan 2005; 23 Altmann, Trafton 2004; 26 Li, Magrabi, Coiera 2012; 19 Stanton 2006; 37 Xia, Sudharshan 2002; 12 Zacks, Braver, Sheridan, Donaldson, Snyder, Ollinger, Buckner, Raichle 2001; 4 Flynn, Barker, Gibson, Pearson, Berger, Smith 1999; 56 Zacks, Tversky 2001; 127 bibr1-00187208211009010 bibr37-00187208211009010 bibr24-00187208211009010 bibr6-00187208211009010 Crystal A. (bibr17-00187208211009010) 2004 bibr51-00187208211009010 Xiao Y. M. (bibr54-00187208211009010) 2005; 23 bibr7-00187208211009010 bibr31-00187208211009010 Monk C. A. (bibr38-00187208211009010) 2004 bibr57-00187208211009010 bibr44-00187208211009010 Anderson J. R. (bibr5-00187208211009010) 1998 bibr43-00187208211009010 Altmann E. M. (bibr3-00187208211009010) 2004; 26 bibr49-00187208211009010 bibr23-00187208211009010 bibr13-00187208211009010 bibr59-00187208211009010 bibr33-00187208211009010 bibr39-00187208211009010 bibr29-00187208211009010 bibr53-00187208211009010 bibr22-00187208211009010 bibr48-00187208211009010 bibr58-00187208211009010 Bailey B. P. (bibr11-00187208211009010) 2001 bibr28-00187208211009010 bibr18-00187208211009010 bibr42-00187208211009010 bibr52-00187208211009010 bibr12-00187208211009010 bibr2-00187208211009010 bibr14-00187208211009010 bibr27-00187208211009010 Kahneman D (bibr32-00187208211009010) 1973 bibr4-00187208211009010 bibr34-00187208211009010 bibr21-00187208211009010 bibr9-00187208211009010 bibr19-00187208211009010 Bailey B. P. (bibr8-00187208211009010) 2005 bibr61-00187208211009010 Maxwell S. E. (bibr35-00187208211009010) 2004 bibr41-00187208211009010 bibr47-00187208211009010 Hayes C. (bibr25-00187208211009010) 2002 Spira J. B. (bibr45-00187208211009010) 2005 bibr20-00187208211009010 bibr30-00187208211009010 bibr40-00187208211009010 bibr10-00187208211009010 bibr60-00187208211009010 bibr50-00187208211009010 bibr46-00187208211009010 bibr16-00187208211009010 bibr26-00187208211009010 bibr56-00187208211009010 Card S. K. (bibr15-00187208211009010) 1983 bibr36-00187208211009010 bibr55-00187208211009010 |
References_xml | – volume: 17 start-page: 375 year: 2003 end-page: 381 article-title: Data preparation for data mining publication-title: Applied Artificial Intelligence – volume: 19 start-page: 314 year: 2007 end-page: 329 article-title: TAPRAV: An interactive analysis tool for exploring workload aligned to models of task execution publication-title: Interacting with Computers – volume: 48 start-page: 295 year: 2004 end-page: 299 article-title: The effect of frequent versus infrequent interruptions on primary task resumption publication-title: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting – volume: 127 start-page: 3 year: 2001 end-page: 21 article-title: Event structure in perception and conception publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 28 start-page: 663 year: 2015 end-page: 686 article-title: Selective attention as a protagonist in contemporary workplace stress: Implications for the interruption age publication-title: Anxiety, Stress, & Coping – volume: 72 start-page: 163 year: 1999 end-page: 185 article-title: Temporal factors in mental work: Effects of interrupted activities publication-title: Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology – volume: 56 start-page: 1319 year: 1999 end-page: 1325 article-title: Impact of interruptions and distractions on dispensing errors in an ambulatory care pharmacy publication-title: American Journal of Health-System Pharmacy – volume: 32 start-page: 1120 year: 2006 end-page: 1132 article-title: Contextual cues aid recovery from interruption: The role of associative activation publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 23 start-page: 178 year: 2005 end-page: 181 article-title: The appraisal of reliability and validity of subjective workload assessment technique and NASA-task load index (English translation) publication-title: Chinese Journal of Industrial Hygiene and Occupational Diseases – volume: 58 start-page: 583 year: 2003 end-page: 603 article-title: Preparing to resume an interrupted task: Effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal publication-title: International Journal of Human-Computer Studies – volume: 14 start-page: 1 year: 2008 end-page: 28 article-title: Understanding changes in mental workload during execution of goal-directed tasks and its application for interruption management publication-title: ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction – volume: 24 start-page: 568 year: 1988 end-page: 573 article-title: Social cognition as segmentation of the stream of behavior publication-title: Developmental Psychology – start-page: 3202 year: 2004 end-page: 3210 article-title: Task analysis and human-computer interaction: Approaches, techniques, and levels of analysis publication-title: AMCIS 2004 Proceedings – volume: 53 start-page: 61 year: 2004 end-page: 86 article-title: Evaluation of subjective mental workload: A comparison of SWAT, NASA‐TLX, and workload profile methods publication-title: Applied Psychology – volume: 14 start-page: 1079 year: 2007 end-page: 1084 article-title: Timecourse of recovery from task interruption: Data and a model publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 10 start-page: 177 year: 2009 end-page: 189 article-title: Investigating the effects of computer mediated interruptions: An analysis of task characteristics and interruption frequency on financial performance publication-title: International Journal of Accounting Information Systems – volume: 6 start-page: 243 year: 2012 end-page: 265 article-title: Considering etiquette in the design of an adaptive system publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making – volume: 37 start-page: 55 year: 2006 end-page: 79 article-title: Hierarchical task analysis: Developments, applications, and extensions publication-title: Applied Ergonomics – volume: 35 start-page: 263 year: 1993 end-page: 281 article-title: Recommendations for mental workload measurement in a test and evaluation environment publication-title: Human Factors – volume: 59 start-page: 1204 year: 2017 end-page: 1213 article-title: Impact of frequent interruption on nurses’ patient-controlled analgesia programming performance publication-title: Human Factors – volume: 12 start-page: 265 year: 2002 end-page: 280 article-title: Effects of interruptions on consumer online decision processes publication-title: Journal of Consumer Psychology – volume: 16 start-page: 317 year: 2010 end-page: 334 article-title: Interruption management in the intensive care unit: Predicting resumption times and assessing distributed support publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology, Applied – volume: 10 start-page: 104 year: 2003 end-page: 110 article-title: Out of sight, out of mind: Occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 130 start-page: 29 year: 2001 end-page: 58 article-title: Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 26 start-page: 39 year: 2002 end-page: 83 article-title: Memory for goals: An activation-based model publication-title: Cognitive Science – volume: 133 start-page: 273 year: 2007 end-page: 293 article-title: Event perception: A mind-brain perspective publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 25 start-page: 137 year: 2015 end-page: 152 article-title: The effects of task interruption on human performance: A study of the systematic classification of human behavior and interruption frequency publication-title: Human Factors and Ergonomics in Manufacturing & Service Industries – volume: 30 start-page: 337 year: 1999 end-page: 360 article-title: The influence of task interruption on individual decision making: An information overload perspective publication-title: Decision Sciences – volume: 50 start-page: 243 year: 1989 end-page: 250 article-title: What makes interruptions disruptive? A study of length, similarity, and complexity publication-title: Psychological Research – volume: 19 start-page: 6 year: 2012 end-page: 12 article-title: A systematic review of the psychological literature on interruption and its patient safety implications publication-title: Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association – volume: 26 year: 2004 article-title: Task interruption: Resumption lag and the role of cues publication-title: Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society – volume: 17 start-page: 97 year: 2011 end-page: 109 article-title: Mitigating disruptive effects of interruptions through training: What needs to be practiced? publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied – volume: 16 start-page: 135 year: 2007 end-page: 139 article-title: Quantifying distraction and interruption in urological surgery publication-title: Quality and Safety in Health Care – volume: 4 start-page: 651 year: 2001 end-page: 655 article-title: Human brain activity time-locked to perceptual event boundaries publication-title: Nature Neuroscience – volume: 14 start-page: 299 year: 2008 end-page: 313 article-title: The effect of interruption duration and demand on resuming suspended goals publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology, Applied – volume: 22 start-page: 685 year: 2006 end-page: 708 article-title: On the need for attention-aware systems: Measuring effects of interruption on task performance, error rate, and affective state publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior – volume: 2 start-page: 30 year: 2013 end-page: 55 article-title: Act-r/e: An embodied cognitive architecture for human-robot interaction publication-title: Journal of Human-Robot Interaction – start-page: 593 year: 2001 end-page: 601 article-title: The effects of interruptions on task performance, annoyance, and anxiety in the user interface publication-title: Interact – ident: bibr44-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5915.1999.tb01613.x – volume-title: The atomic components of thought year: 1998 ident: bibr5-00187208211009010 – volume: 23 start-page: 178 year: 2005 ident: bibr54-00187208211009010 publication-title: Chinese Journal of Industrial Hygiene and Occupational Diseases – ident: bibr41-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2004.00161.x – ident: bibr22-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/a0021912 – ident: bibr58-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.1.29 – ident: bibr34-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1136/amiajnl-2010-000024 – ident: bibr31-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/B978-044451020-4/50031-1 – ident: bibr50-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1518/155723408X299852 – ident: bibr55-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1038/88486 – ident: bibr7-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/j.intcom.2007.01.004 – ident: bibr33-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1002/hfm.20603 – ident: bibr56-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.273 – ident: bibr57-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3 – ident: bibr16-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-74800-7_39 – ident: bibr46-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2005.06.003 – ident: bibr29-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/1054972.1055016 – ident: bibr53-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1207/S15327663JCP1203_08 – start-page: 593 year: 2001 ident: bibr11-00187208211009010 publication-title: Interact – ident: bibr9-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/1314683.1314689 – ident: bibr48-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00023-5 – ident: bibr42-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/1753326.1753341 – ident: bibr6-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1201/9781410607775.ch2 – ident: bibr61-00187208211009010 – ident: bibr47-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1080/10615806.2015.1011141 – ident: bibr19-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1093/ajhp/56.13.1319 – ident: bibr20-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1007/BF00309260 – volume: 26 year: 2004 ident: bibr3-00187208211009010 publication-title: Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society – volume-title: The cost of not paying attention: How interruptions impact knowledge worker productivity year: 2005 ident: bibr45-00187208211009010 – ident: bibr14-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1177/0018720817732605 – ident: bibr28-00187208211009010 doi: 10.3758/BF03196473 – ident: bibr24-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1201/b10420-17 – ident: bibr12-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/j.accinf.2009.10.003 – ident: bibr23-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62386-9 – ident: bibr30-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/1056808.1056948 – start-page: 3202 year: 2004 ident: bibr17-00187208211009010 publication-title: AMCIS 2004 Proceedings – ident: bibr27-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1120 – ident: bibr39-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/a0014402 – volume-title: Attention and effort (Prentice-Hall series in experimental psychology) year: 1973 ident: bibr32-00187208211009010 – volume-title: The psychology of human-computer interaction year: 1983 ident: bibr15-00187208211009010 – ident: bibr49-00187208211009010 doi: 10.5898/JHRI.2.1.Trafton – ident: bibr59-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1080/713827180 – ident: bibr26-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1136/qshc.2006.019711 – ident: bibr37-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1177/154193120404800304 – ident: bibr21-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.568 – ident: bibr1-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/985692.985727 – ident: bibr13-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1037/a0023497 – volume-title: Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective year: 2004 ident: bibr35-00187208211009010 – ident: bibr51-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1177/001872089303500205 – ident: bibr60-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1348/096317999166581 – volume-title: Leveraging changes in mental workload during task execution to mitigate effects of interruption year: 2005 ident: bibr8-00187208211009010 – start-page: 1606 volume-title: Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society year: 2004 ident: bibr38-00187208211009010 – ident: bibr2-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2601_2 – ident: bibr4-00187208211009010 doi: 10.3758/BF03193094 – volume-title: Etiquette in human computer interactions: What does it mean for a computer to be polite? Or who needs polite computers anyway? year: 2002 ident: bibr25-00187208211009010 – ident: bibr18-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1177/1555343412441001 – ident: bibr36-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1201/b15703-13 – ident: bibr10-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.12.009 – ident: bibr43-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/1518701.1518981 – ident: bibr52-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1201/9781420063363 – ident: bibr40-00187208211009010 doi: 10.1145/223355.223665 |
SSID | ssj0006935 |
Score | 2.421439 |
Snippet | Objective
The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical... The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical task... Objective The purpose was to explore how event segmentation theory (EST) can be used to determine optimal moments for an interruption relying on hierarchical... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref sage |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 22 |
SubjectTerms | Attention Between-subjects design Breakpoints Frustration Humans Segmentation Task analysis Task Performance and Analysis Workload Workloads |
Title | Examining the Effect of Interruptions at Different Breakpoints and Frequencies Within a Task |
URI | https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/00187208211009010 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33861143 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2765944274 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2514593309 |
Volume | 65 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3ta9QwGA-6fZkfROdbdUoEQfDo0TRtk348dGMIE4UbThBK2iZsTNtx64H61_vkpWm7mzL9Uo5eLr3m-eXJ8_4g9EqxKkpjwcMyKykoKEyFQpe7q3lJpBQ8llznDh99yA6Pk_cn6ckQ1myyS7pyXv26Nq_kf6gK94CuOkv2HyjrJ4Ub8BnoC1egMFxvROP9H-K7afBgxEdXiNjlhqxWaxuvotMV37k2KNoYIMX5RXumo1-0zfxgZWOpQWEGDtGdnjUzMVuKy_Ox1Got_a41j2emvr-asSrPFvPBqQO0avtMoNnn-di0ENM-Gnni07reehgRHrI4sqxTOt6ZwIFHbAeWnrnaRhATEDlOGY_OXFsDZZObG3-y6RsIj9KqaqSjSYajywcUutHFxtjbaDsGBQI44Pbiy8dPR_6UznLqulvYF3Eeb1OM6-okU5llQxGZBAEauWR5D911CgVeWHTcR7dks4vujMpM7qIdf8r9fIC-etBgAA22oMGtwhPQYNFhDxo8Ag0G0OARaLAFDRZYg-YhOj7YX749DF2LjbCiGe9CFVGhKFPa2wxqFi8rRkTCmFmAulQq5Swpc0oqxpWKZS5BgI8EqLlVLQip6CO01bSNfIJwWrEaNqcgSQkyOAfBNiF1SphWSVLJWICifhGLytWf121QvhWkLzl_dd0D9Mb_5MIWX_nb4L2eMoXbo5dFzLI0T5KYJQF66b8GDqrdYqKR7RrGgM6QarteHqDHlqL-aZTyjMDrBOi1JvEw8R__xtMbj3yGdoZNt4e2utVaPgcJtytfOLD-Bn7SmEs |
linkProvider | SAGE Publications |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3fT9swED5N5WHwABuDEeiGJ01CQgqq4yR2HstG1TGKhlQEaJMiJ7G1qpBWJZXY_nrOiZONn5r2bMc5-3z2d_b5O4CPmqedwJPCTcKEoYPCtSsN3V0mEqqUFJ4S5u3w4Djsn_qH58G5jao0b2HsCF7vmbAqlKhcrBvrNkxJJouchxuX4TqLytdVC8KwYrZgoXvx7WTQrMNhxGz-AuGaL-yd5qON3N2VHkDNO2Fe5c7TW4HvtcxVwMl4b16guL_v0Tn-X6dewbIFpKRbzaDX8ELlq7D0F03hKiw2q-SvN_Dj4EZelVklCGJHUrEfk4km5dnibF4FyRBZkM8290pB9hGZjqeTUV5gQZ6R3qwK4EYvnZyNip-jnEgylNfjNTjtHQw_9V2bosFNWSgKV3eY1Ixrc1uJMF0kKafS57zsTpZoHQjuJxGjKRdaeypSCAA7Et2kNJOUpmwdWvkkVxtAgpRnaLKS-olJyY7AyKdZQLmBtIHi3IFOraI4tfzlJo3GZUxryvL7o-jAbvPJtCLveK5yu9Z7XCss9ngYRL6PrrsDH5pitEBzrSJzNZljHcScgTkXihx4W82X5m-MiRA9TubAjtH9n4afFGPzn2tuw8v-cHAUH305_roFix6iryqcvA2tYjZX7xAtFcl7axa3VIcB-Q |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rS-QwEB9EQfSDz1Pr6yIIBwfVTdM26cf1sXh3KgqKHhyUtE1wWe0uu13w7q930qY9H3dy3Oek6SSTyfwmM5kB2NU8bQWeFG4SJgwNFK5dadLdZSKhSknhKWHeDp-dhyfX_tfb4NZeuJm3MHYFR3smrAopKg9rI92DTO9bH-N-WUnOQ-Vl8p1F5QurKVRUBiBNtb9fXJ41Z3EYMVvDQLjmC-vX_OMgLzXTG7j5ItSr1D6deYhruqugk97euECSf71K6fj_E1uAOQtMSbvaSYswofIlmH2WrnAJZprT8ucy_Dh-lA9ldQmCGJJUWZBJX5PyjnE4roJliCzIka3BUpADRKi9Qb-bF9iQZ6QzrAK50VonN93irpsTSa7kqPcBrjvHV4cnri3V4KYsFIWrW0xqxrXxWiJcF0nKqfQ5L6eTJVoHgvtJxGjKhdaeihQCwZZEcynNJKUpW4HJvJ-rNSBByjMUXUn9xJRmR4Dk0yyg3EDbQHHuQKtmU5zaPOamnMZ9TOvU5a9X0YHPzSeDKonHe503a97HNdNij4dB5Ptowjuw0zSjJBr3isxVf4x9EHsG5n4ocmC12jPN3xgTIVqezIFPhv-_B_4rGev_3PMjTF8cdeLTL-ffNmDGQxBWRZVvwmQxHKstBE1Fsm0l4wn3AgRu |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Examining+the+Effect+of+Interruptions+at+Different+Breakpoints+and+Frequencies+Within+a+Task&rft.jtitle=Human+factors&rft.au=Powers%2C+Sarah+A.&rft.au=Scerbo%2C+Mark+W.&rft.date=2023-02-01&rft.pub=SAGE+Publications&rft.issn=0018-7208&rft.eissn=1547-8181&rft.volume=65&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=22&rft.epage=36&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177%2F00187208211009010&rft.externalDocID=10.1177_00187208211009010 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0018-7208&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0018-7208&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0018-7208&client=summon |