Post-exposure cortisol administration does not augment the success of exposure therapy: A randomized placebo-controlled study

•Studied the effects of post-exposure cortisol administration on exposure therapy outcome.•Employed contextual changes to examine cortisol effects on fear renewal.•Post-exposure cortisol administration did not promote exposure therapy outcome.•Post-exposure cortisol administration led to a stronger...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inPsychoneuroendocrinology Vol. 99; pp. 174 - 182
Main Authors Raeder, Friederike, Merz, Christian J., Tegenthoff, Martin, Wolf, Oliver T., Margraf, Jürgen, Zlomuzica, Armin
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Elsevier Ltd 01.01.2019
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
More Information
Summary:•Studied the effects of post-exposure cortisol administration on exposure therapy outcome.•Employed contextual changes to examine cortisol effects on fear renewal.•Post-exposure cortisol administration did not promote exposure therapy outcome.•Post-exposure cortisol administration led to a stronger fear renewal at long-term. Cortisol administration prior to treatment can promote the efficacy of exposure-based treatments in specific phobia: cortisol has been proposed to reduce fear retrieval at the beginning of exposure and to enhance the acquisition and consolidation of corrective information learned during exposure. Whether cortisol exerts a beneficial therapeutic effect when given after exposure, e.g., by targeting the consolidation of new corrective information, has not been addressed so far to date. Here, we examined whether post-exposure cortisol administration promotes fear reduction and reduces return of fear following contextual change in specific phobia. Furthermore, the effect of cortisol on return of fear following contextual change (i.e., contextual renewal) was assessed. Patients with spider phobia (N = 43) were treated with a single session of in-vivo exposure, followed by cortisol administration (20 mg hydrocortisone) in a double-blind, placebo-controlled study design. Return of fear was assessed with behavioral approach tests (BATs) in the familiar therapy context (versus a novel unfamiliar context) at one-month and seven-month follow-up assessment. Exposure was effective in reducing fear from pre-treatment to post-treatment (i.e., 24 h after exposure) on fear-related behavioral (approach behavior during the BAT), psychophysiological (heart rate during the BAT) and subjective (fear during the BAT, spider-fear related questionnaires) measures of therapeutic outcome, with no add-on benefit of cortisol administration. Cortisol had no effect on contextual renewal at one-month follow-up. However, in a subsample (N = 21) that returned to the seven-month follow-up, an adverse effect of cortisol on fear renewal was found, with cortisol-treated patients showing an increase in subjective fear at the final approach distance of the BAT from post-treatment to seven-month follow-up. These and previous findings underline the importance of considering the exact timing of cortisol application when used as an add-on treatment for extinction-based psychotherapy: post-exposure cortisol administration does not seem to be effective, but might promote fear renewal at the subjective level.
Bibliography:ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-News-1
ObjectType-Feature-3
content type line 23
ISSN:0306-4530
1873-3360
DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.09.015