Signaling the unconditioned stimulus during the preexposure phase does not attenuate the unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect in preweanling rats
The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US‐PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase....
Saved in:
Published in | Developmental psychobiology Vol. 54; no. 8; pp. 808 - 817 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Hoboken
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company
01.12.2012
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0012-1630 1098-2302 1098-2302 |
DOI | 10.1002/dev.21001 |
Cover
Abstract | The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US‐PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase. This evidence has been found consistently across a variety of procedures in adult rats. The aim of the present study was to evaluate whether, in infant rats, signaling the US (LiCl) during preexposure with a salient cue (almond odor) attenuates the US‐PE. During the preexposure phase, preweanling rats received three (Experiment 1) or one (Experiment 2) preexposures to LiCl, preceded by exposure to almond odor. Appropriate control groups were also included in these experiments. After preexposure, two conditioning trials were carried out in which subjects were given LiCl after saccharin consumption. During preexposure, three (Experiment 1a), although not one (Experiment 2a), contingent exposures to almond odor and LiCl resulted in a strong odor aversion. Extinction of the learned taste aversion was facilitated by prior experience with LiCl (Experiments 1b and 2b). This effect was observed regardless of whether or not LiCl was signaled by the almond odor. These results do not coincide with the associative hypotheses proposed to explain the US‐PE, nor are they concurrent with alternative explanations based on the learned helplessness phenomenon. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals,Inc. Dev Psychobiol 54: 808–817, 2012 |
---|---|
AbstractList | The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US‐PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase. This evidence has been found consistently across a variety of procedures in adult rats. The aim of the present study was to evaluate whether, in infant rats, signaling the US (LiCl) during preexposure with a salient cue (almond odor) attenuates the US‐PE. During the preexposure phase, preweanling rats received three (Experiment 1) or one (Experiment 2) preexposures to LiCl, preceded by exposure to almond odor. Appropriate control groups were also included in these experiments. After preexposure, two conditioning trials were carried out in which subjects were given LiCl after saccharin consumption. During preexposure, three (Experiment 1a), although not one (Experiment 2a), contingent exposures to almond odor and LiCl resulted in a strong odor aversion. Extinction of the learned taste aversion was facilitated by prior experience with LiCl (Experiments 1b and 2b). This effect was observed regardless of whether or not LiCl was signaled by the almond odor. These results do not coincide with the associative hypotheses proposed to explain the US‐PE, nor are they concurrent with alternative explanations based on the learned helplessness phenomenon. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals,Inc. Dev Psychobiol 54: 808–817, 2012 The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US-PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase. This evidence has been found consistently across a variety of procedures in adult rats. The aim of the present study was to evaluate whether, in infant rats, signaling the US (LiCl) during preexposure with a salient cue (almond odor) attenuates the US-PE. During the preexposure phase, preweanling rats received three (Experiment 1) or one (Experiment 2) preexposures to LiCl, preceded by exposure to almond odor. Appropriate control groups were also included in these experiments. After preexposure, two conditioning trials were carried out in which subjects were given LiCl after saccharin consumption. During preexposure, three (Experiment 1a), although not one (Experiment 2a), contingent exposures to almond odor and LiCl resulted in a strong odor aversion. Extinction of the learned taste aversion was facilitated by prior experience with LiCl (Experiments 1b and 2b). This effect was observed regardless of whether or not LiCl was signaled by the almond odor. These results do not coincide with the associative hypotheses proposed to explain the US-PE, nor are they concurrent with alternative explanations based on the learned helplessness phenomenon. The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US-PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase. This evidence has been found consistently across a variety of procedures in adult rats. The aim of the present study was to evaluate whether, in infant rats, signaling the US (LiCl) during preexposure with a salient cue (almond odor) attenuates the US-PE. During the preexposure phase, preweanling rats received three (Experiment 1) or one (Experiment 2) preexposures to LiCl, preceded by exposure to almond odor. Appropriate control groups were also included in these experiments. After preexposure, two conditioning trials were carried out in which subjects were given LiCl after saccharin consumption. During preexposure, three (Experiment 1a), although not one (Experiment 2a), contingent exposures to almond odor and LiCl resulted in a strong odor aversion. Extinction of the learned taste aversion was facilitated by prior experience with LiCl (Experiments 1b and 2b). This effect was observed regardless of whether or not LiCl was signaled by the almond odor. These results do not coincide with the associative hypotheses proposed to explain the US-PE, nor are they concurrent with alternative explanations based on the learned helplessness phenomenon.The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US-PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase. This evidence has been found consistently across a variety of procedures in adult rats. The aim of the present study was to evaluate whether, in infant rats, signaling the US (LiCl) during preexposure with a salient cue (almond odor) attenuates the US-PE. During the preexposure phase, preweanling rats received three (Experiment 1) or one (Experiment 2) preexposures to LiCl, preceded by exposure to almond odor. Appropriate control groups were also included in these experiments. After preexposure, two conditioning trials were carried out in which subjects were given LiCl after saccharin consumption. During preexposure, three (Experiment 1a), although not one (Experiment 2a), contingent exposures to almond odor and LiCl resulted in a strong odor aversion. Extinction of the learned taste aversion was facilitated by prior experience with LiCl (Experiments 1b and 2b). This effect was observed regardless of whether or not LiCl was signaled by the almond odor. These results do not coincide with the associative hypotheses proposed to explain the US-PE, nor are they concurrent with alternative explanations based on the learned helplessness phenomenon. |
Author | Arias, Carlos Castello, Stefania Orellana, Estefania Bobbio, Antonella |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Stefania surname: Castello fullname: Castello, Stefania organization: Psychology Department, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, CP 5000, Argentina – sequence: 2 givenname: Antonella surname: Bobbio fullname: Bobbio, Antonella organization: Psychology Department, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, CP 5000, Argentina – sequence: 3 givenname: Estefania surname: Orellana fullname: Orellana, Estefania organization: Psychology Department, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, CP 5000, Argentina – sequence: 4 givenname: Carlos surname: Arias fullname: Arias, Carlos email: afelicidade@yahoo.es organization: Psychology Department, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Córdoba, CP 5000, Argentina |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22213476$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kclOHDEQhi1EBAPhkBeI-pgcGrzM9HJEhCUCESkkgZtVbZfBocc98RLgRfK8eJiZCEUQqSSXS9__l121Rdbd4JCQd4zuMkr5nsbfuzxnbI2MGG2bkgvK18koV3jJKkE3yVYIP_OVjZt6g2xyzpkY19WI_Lmw1w56666LeINFcmpw2kabG-giRDtNfQqFTn5FzDzi_WwIyef8BgIWesBQuCEWECO6BBH_Z_Vcj8agioV18-odgnt6h4cY3pI3BvqAO8tzm3w_Ovx2cFKefTn-fLB_VipRCVZ2XFf1pBHYtLzLQY0CRVtDWzDQwVhU1AhtJgBKszFTjZiIlvNWsbpuOk7FNvmw8J354VfCEOXUBoV9Dw6HFGQemGjqHFVG3y_R1E1Ry5m3U_APcjXLDOwtAOWHEDwaqWyE-f-jB9tLRuV8WzJvSz5tKys-_qNYmb7ELt3vbI8Pr4Py0-GPlaJcKGyIeP9XAf5WVrWoJ_Ly_Fh-PT0_urqomDwRj2VZtv4 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21140 |
Cites_doi | 10.1006/lmot.1996.0021 10.1002/dev.420220407 10.1016/j.alcohol.2006.10.002 10.1037/0097-7403.30.1.58 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.523 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.573 10.1002/dev.20460 10.1101/lm.1755810 10.1002/dev.20418 10.1093/chemse/bjq075 10.3758/BF03337815 10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.126 10.1037/0735-7044.117.1.113 10.1037/0735-7044.112.5.1142 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.710 10.1037/0735-7044.108.2.227 10.3758/BF03192864 10.1037/0097-7403.3.2.189 10.1002/dev.20074 10.1016/0023-9690(81)90012-6 10.1002/dev.20244 10.1002/dev.420170407 10.1037/0097-7403.1.3.270 10.1093/chemse/bjr026 10.1002/dev.420160103 10.1016/0892-0362(92)90020-B 10.3758/BF03199972 10.3758/BF03209785 10.1080/02724990244000115 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
DBID | BSCLL AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1002/dev.21001 |
DatabaseName | Istex CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Engineering Biology Zoology Psychology |
EISSN | 1098-2302 |
EndPage | 817 |
ExternalDocumentID | 22213476 10_1002_dev_21001 DEV21001 ark_67375_WNG_RKNFXS61_H |
Genre | article Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --Z -ET -~X .3N .GA .GJ .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1L6 1OB 1OC 1ZS 31~ 33P 36B 3SF 3WU 4.4 4ZD 50Y 50Z 51W 51X 52M 52N 52O 52P 52S 52T 52U 52W 52X 53G 5GY 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A03 AAESR AAEVG AAHHS AANLZ AAONW AASGY AAXRX AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABEML ABIJN ABIVO ABJNI ABPPZ ABPVW ABTAH ACAHQ ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACGFS ACKIV ACNCT ACPOU ACSCC ACXBN ACXQS ADBBV ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADOZA ADRHT ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFFPM AFGKR AFPWT AFZJQ AHBTC AITYG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE AJXKR ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ATUGU AUFTA AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB B-7 BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BHBCM BMNLL BMXJE BNHUX BROTX BRXPI BSCLL BY8 C45 CS3 D-E D-F DCZOG DPXWK DR1 DR2 DRFUL DRSTM DU5 EBS EJD EMB EMOBN F00 F01 F04 F5P FEDTE G-S G.N GAKWD GNP GODZA H.T H.X HBH HF~ HGLYW HHY HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IX1 J0M JPC KQQ LATKE LAW LC2 LC3 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES M6M MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRSTM MSFUL MSSTM MVM MXFUL MXSTM N04 N05 N9A NF~ NNB O66 O9- OHT OIG OVD P2P P2W P2X P4D PALCI Q.N Q11 QB0 QRW R.K RIWAO RJQFR ROL RWD RWI RX1 RYL S10 SAMSI SUPJJ SV3 TEORI TN5 TWZ UB1 UPT V2E W8V W99 WBKPD WGLLI WH7 WHG WIB WIH WIK WOHZO WQJ WRC WSUWO WUP WXSBR XG1 XJT XOL XPP XSW XV2 YCJ YQI YQJ ZGI ZXP ZY4 ZZTAW ~IA ~WT AAHQN AAMNL AANHP AAYCA ACRPL ACYXJ ADNMO AFWVQ ALVPJ AAYXX ADXHL AETEA AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ AGYGG CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 AAMMB AEFGJ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c3631-b2d67583e892b92b0fcac09f09afaba4360f3df5aacd141c83539229c1778b203 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0012-1630 1098-2302 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 12:34:48 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:01:49 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:58:50 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:42:23 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:48:44 EST 2025 Wed Oct 30 09:53:25 EDT 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 8 |
Language | English |
License | http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor Copyright © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c3631-b2d67583e892b92b0fcac09f09afaba4360f3df5aacd141c83539229c1778b203 |
Notes | ark:/67375/WNG-RKNFXS61-H Stefania Castello and Antonella Bobbio contributed equally to the study. ArticleID:DEV21001 istex:402D60BB7B51B0829B0F497FA00957692BB2901D ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
PMID | 22213476 |
PQID | 1143873876 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 10 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_1143873876 pubmed_primary_22213476 crossref_citationtrail_10_1002_dev_21001 crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21001 wiley_primary_10_1002_dev_21001_DEV21001 istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_RKNFXS61_H |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2012-12 December 2012 2012-12-00 2012-Dec 20121201 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2012-12-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 12 year: 2012 text: 2012-12 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Hoboken |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Hoboken – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Developmental psychobiology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Dev. Psychobiol |
PublicationYear | 2012 |
Publisher | Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company |
References | Aguado, L., Hall, G., Harrington, N., & Symonds, M. (1998). Illness-induced context aversion learning in rats with lesions of the dorsal hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 1142-1151. Arias, C., Pautassi, R. M., Molina, J. C., & Spear, N. E. (2010). A comparison between taste avoidance and conditioned disgust reactions induced by ethanol and lithium chloride in preweanling rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 545-557. Mikulka, P. J., Leard, B., & Klein, S. B. (1977). Illness-alone exposure as a source of interference with the acquisition and retention of a taste aversion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 189-201. Kraemer, P. J., Kraemer, E. L., Smoller, D. E., & Spear, N. E. (1989). Enhancement of flavor aversion conditioning in weanling but not adult rats by prior conditioning to an odor. Psychobiology, 17, 34-42. Baker, A. G., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1979). Pre-exposure to the Cs alone, Us alone, or Cs and Us uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context, or learned irrelevance? Learning and Motivation, 15, 709-720. Chotro, M. G., & Alonso, G. (2003). Stimulus preexposure reduces generalization of conditioned taste aversions between alcohol and non-alcohol flavors in infant rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 113-122. Revillo, D. A., Spear, N. E., & Arias, C. (2011). Ontogenetic differences in sensitivity to LiCl- and amphetamine-induced taste avoidance in preweanling rats. Chemical Senses, 36, 565-577. de Brugada, I., Hall, G., & Symonds, M. (2004). The US-preexposure effect in lithium-induced flavor-aversion conditioning is a consequence of blocking by injection cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 30, 58-66. Parker, L. A., Hills, K., & Jensen, K. (1984). Behavioral CRs elicited by a lithium or an amphetamine-paired contextual test chamber. Animal Learning and Behavior, 12, 307-315. Foster, J. A., & Burman, M. A. (2010). Evidence for hippocampus-dependent contextual learning at postnatal day 17 in the rat. Learning & Memory, 17, 259-266. Rudy, J. W., & Morledge, P. (1994). Ontogeny of contextual fear conditioning in rats: Implications for consolidation, infantile amnesia, and hippocampal system function. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 227-234. Holson, R. R., & Pearce, B. (1992). Principles and pitfalls in the analysis of prenatal treatment effects in multiparous species. Neurotoxicology & Teratology, 14, 221-228. Spear, L. P., Specht, S. M., Kirstein, C. L., & Kuhn, C. M. (1989). Anterior and posterior, but not cheek, intraoral cannulation procedures elevate serum corticosterone levels in neonatal rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 401-411. Caspy, T., & Lubow, R. E. (1981). Generality of US preexposure effects: Transfer from food to shock or shock to food with and without the same response requirements. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9, 524-532. Brasser, S. M., & Spear, N. E. (1998). A sensory-enhanced context facilitates learning and multiple measures of unconditioned stimulus processing in the preweanling rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 126-140. de Brugada, I., Gonzalez, F., & Candido, A. (2003). The role of injection cues in the associative control of the US pre-exposure effect in flavour aversion learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 241-252. Arias, C., & Gabriela Chotro, M. (2006). Interactions between prenatal ethanol exposure and postnatal learning about ethanol in rat pups. Alcohol, 40, 51-59. Gautam, S. H., & Verhagen, J. V. (2010). Evidence that the sweetness of odors depends on experience in rats. Chemical Senses, 35, 767-776. Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources. (1996). National Research Council. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. Washington, DC: National Academic Press. Yap, C. S., & Richardson, R. (2007). Extinction in the developing rat: An examination of renewal effects. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 565-575. Yap, C. S., & Richardson, R. (2005). Latent inhibition in the developing rat: An examination of context-specific effects. Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 55-65. Cannon, D. S., Berman, R. F., Baker, T. B., & Atkins, C. A. (1975). Effect of preconditioning unconditioned stimulus experience on learned taste aversions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 270-284. de Brugada, I., Gonzalez, F., Gil, M., & Hall, G. (2005). The role of habituation of the response to LiCl in the US-preexposure effect. Learning & Behavior, 33, 363-370. Wasserman, E. A., & Miller, R. R. (1997). What's elementary about associative learning? Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 573-607. Cole, K. C., VanTilburg, D., Burch-Vernon, A., & Riccio, D. C. (1996). The importance of context in the US preexposure effect in CTA: Novel versus latently inhibited contextual stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 27, 362-374. Randich, A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1979). Associative and nonassociative theories of the UCS preexposure phenomenon: Implications for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 523-548. Rudy, J. W., & Cheatle, M. D. (1983). Odor-aversion learning by rats following LiCl exposure: Ontogenetic influences. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 13-22. Randich, A. (1981). The US preexposure phenomenon in the conditioned suppression paradigm: A role for conditioned situational stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 12, 321-341. Diaz-Cenzano, E., & Chotro, M. G. (2010). The effect of taste familiarity on intake and taste reactivity in infant rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 109-120. Arias, C., & Chotro, M. G. (2006). Ethanol-induced preferences or aversions as a function of age in preweanling rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 710-718. Caza, P. A., & Spear, N. E. (1984). Short-term exposure to an odor increases its subsequent preference in preweanling rats: A descriptive profile of the phenomenon. Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 407-422. 1989; 22 1979; 15 2003; 117 2010; 35 2010; 17 1997; 48 2009 1981; 9 1996 1972 1992; 14 2011; 36 1998; 112 1983; 16 2003; 56 2005; 47 2004; 30 2006; 40 2001 1984; 17 1984; 12 1977; 3 2006; 120 1975; 1 1979; 86 1996; 27 1994; 108 2010; 52 2005; 33 1969 1989; 17 1981; 12 2007; 49 e_1_2_7_5_1 e_1_2_7_4_1 e_1_2_7_3_1 e_1_2_7_9_1 e_1_2_7_8_1 e_1_2_7_7_1 Rescorla R. A. (e_1_2_7_29_1) 1972 e_1_2_7_18_1 e_1_2_7_17_1 e_1_2_7_16_1 Riley A. L. (e_1_2_7_30_1) 2001 e_1_2_7_2_1 e_1_2_7_15_1 e_1_2_7_14_1 e_1_2_7_13_1 e_1_2_7_12_1 Kraemer P. J. (e_1_2_7_23_1) 1989; 17 e_1_2_7_11_1 e_1_2_7_10_1 e_1_2_7_26_1 e_1_2_7_27_1 e_1_2_7_28_1 Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources (e_1_2_7_21_1) 1996 e_1_2_7_25_1 e_1_2_7_31_1 e_1_2_7_24_1 Rudy J. W. (e_1_2_7_32_1) 1994; 108 e_1_2_7_33_1 e_1_2_7_34_1 e_1_2_7_35_1 e_1_2_7_20_1 e_1_2_7_36_1 Baker A. G. (e_1_2_7_6_1) 1979; 15 Kamin L. J. (e_1_2_7_22_1) 1969 Hall G. (e_1_2_7_19_1) 2009 |
References_xml | – reference: Parker, L. A., Hills, K., & Jensen, K. (1984). Behavioral CRs elicited by a lithium or an amphetamine-paired contextual test chamber. Animal Learning and Behavior, 12, 307-315. – reference: Foster, J. A., & Burman, M. A. (2010). Evidence for hippocampus-dependent contextual learning at postnatal day 17 in the rat. Learning & Memory, 17, 259-266. – reference: de Brugada, I., Gonzalez, F., Gil, M., & Hall, G. (2005). The role of habituation of the response to LiCl in the US-preexposure effect. Learning & Behavior, 33, 363-370. – reference: Caza, P. A., & Spear, N. E. (1984). Short-term exposure to an odor increases its subsequent preference in preweanling rats: A descriptive profile of the phenomenon. Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 407-422. – reference: Rudy, J. W., & Cheatle, M. D. (1983). Odor-aversion learning by rats following LiCl exposure: Ontogenetic influences. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 13-22. – reference: Yap, C. S., & Richardson, R. (2007). Extinction in the developing rat: An examination of renewal effects. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 565-575. – reference: Kraemer, P. J., Kraemer, E. L., Smoller, D. E., & Spear, N. E. (1989). Enhancement of flavor aversion conditioning in weanling but not adult rats by prior conditioning to an odor. Psychobiology, 17, 34-42. – reference: Randich, A. (1981). The US preexposure phenomenon in the conditioned suppression paradigm: A role for conditioned situational stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 12, 321-341. – reference: Caspy, T., & Lubow, R. E. (1981). Generality of US preexposure effects: Transfer from food to shock or shock to food with and without the same response requirements. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9, 524-532. – reference: de Brugada, I., Hall, G., & Symonds, M. (2004). The US-preexposure effect in lithium-induced flavor-aversion conditioning is a consequence of blocking by injection cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 30, 58-66. – reference: Spear, L. P., Specht, S. M., Kirstein, C. L., & Kuhn, C. M. (1989). Anterior and posterior, but not cheek, intraoral cannulation procedures elevate serum corticosterone levels in neonatal rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 401-411. – reference: Holson, R. R., & Pearce, B. (1992). Principles and pitfalls in the analysis of prenatal treatment effects in multiparous species. Neurotoxicology & Teratology, 14, 221-228. – reference: Aguado, L., Hall, G., Harrington, N., & Symonds, M. (1998). Illness-induced context aversion learning in rats with lesions of the dorsal hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 1142-1151. – reference: Rudy, J. W., & Morledge, P. (1994). Ontogeny of contextual fear conditioning in rats: Implications for consolidation, infantile amnesia, and hippocampal system function. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 227-234. – reference: Diaz-Cenzano, E., & Chotro, M. G. (2010). The effect of taste familiarity on intake and taste reactivity in infant rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 109-120. – reference: Yap, C. S., & Richardson, R. (2005). Latent inhibition in the developing rat: An examination of context-specific effects. Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 55-65. – reference: Randich, A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1979). Associative and nonassociative theories of the UCS preexposure phenomenon: Implications for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 523-548. – reference: Mikulka, P. J., Leard, B., & Klein, S. B. (1977). Illness-alone exposure as a source of interference with the acquisition and retention of a taste aversion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 189-201. – reference: Baker, A. G., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1979). Pre-exposure to the Cs alone, Us alone, or Cs and Us uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context, or learned irrelevance? Learning and Motivation, 15, 709-720. – reference: Brasser, S. M., & Spear, N. E. (1998). A sensory-enhanced context facilitates learning and multiple measures of unconditioned stimulus processing in the preweanling rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112, 126-140. – reference: Revillo, D. A., Spear, N. E., & Arias, C. (2011). Ontogenetic differences in sensitivity to LiCl- and amphetamine-induced taste avoidance in preweanling rats. Chemical Senses, 36, 565-577. – reference: Arias, C., & Gabriela Chotro, M. (2006). Interactions between prenatal ethanol exposure and postnatal learning about ethanol in rat pups. Alcohol, 40, 51-59. – reference: Cannon, D. S., Berman, R. F., Baker, T. B., & Atkins, C. A. (1975). Effect of preconditioning unconditioned stimulus experience on learned taste aversions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 270-284. – reference: de Brugada, I., Gonzalez, F., & Candido, A. (2003). The role of injection cues in the associative control of the US pre-exposure effect in flavour aversion learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 241-252. – reference: Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources. (1996). National Research Council. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. Washington, DC: National Academic Press. – reference: Wasserman, E. A., & Miller, R. R. (1997). What's elementary about associative learning? Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 573-607. – reference: Chotro, M. G., & Alonso, G. (2003). Stimulus preexposure reduces generalization of conditioned taste aversions between alcohol and non-alcohol flavors in infant rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 113-122. – reference: Cole, K. C., VanTilburg, D., Burch-Vernon, A., & Riccio, D. C. (1996). The importance of context in the US preexposure effect in CTA: Novel versus latently inhibited contextual stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 27, 362-374. – reference: Arias, C., & Chotro, M. G. (2006). Ethanol-induced preferences or aversions as a function of age in preweanling rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 710-718. – reference: Arias, C., Pautassi, R. M., Molina, J. C., & Spear, N. E. (2010). A comparison between taste avoidance and conditioned disgust reactions induced by ethanol and lithium chloride in preweanling rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 545-557. – reference: Gautam, S. H., & Verhagen, J. V. (2010). Evidence that the sweetness of odors depends on experience in rats. Chemical Senses, 35, 767-776. – volume: 12 start-page: 307 year: 1984 end-page: 315 article-title: Behavioral CRs elicited by a lithium or an amphetamine‐paired contextual test chamber publication-title: Animal Learning and Behavior – volume: 48 start-page: 573 year: 1997 end-page: 607 article-title: What's elementary about associative learning? publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology – volume: 1 start-page: 270 year: 1975 end-page: 284 article-title: Effect of preconditioning unconditioned stimulus experience on learned taste aversions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes – volume: 17 start-page: 259 year: 2010 end-page: 266 article-title: Evidence for hippocampus‐dependent contextual learning at postnatal day 17 in the rat publication-title: Learning & Memory – volume: 36 start-page: 565 year: 2011 end-page: 577 article-title: Ontogenetic differences in sensitivity to LiCl‐ and amphetamine‐induced taste avoidance in preweanling rats publication-title: Chemical Senses – volume: 56 start-page: 241 year: 2003 end-page: 252 article-title: The role of injection cues in the associative control of the US pre‐exposure effect in flavour aversion learning publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – year: 2001 – volume: 14 start-page: 221 year: 1992 end-page: 228 article-title: Principles and pitfalls in the analysis of prenatal treatment effects in multiparous species publication-title: Neurotoxicology & Teratology – year: 1996 – volume: 52 start-page: 545 year: 2010 end-page: 557 article-title: A comparison between taste avoidance and conditioned disgust reactions induced by ethanol and lithium chloride in preweanling rats publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 49 start-page: 565 year: 2007 end-page: 575 article-title: Extinction in the developing rat: An examination of renewal effects publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – start-page: 58 year: 2009 end-page: 73 – volume: 17 start-page: 34 year: 1989 end-page: 42 article-title: Enhancement of flavor aversion conditioning in weanling but not adult rats by prior conditioning to an odor publication-title: Psychobiology – volume: 112 start-page: 126 year: 1998 end-page: 140 article-title: A sensory‐enhanced context facilitates learning and multiple measures of unconditioned stimulus processing in the preweanling rat publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 16 start-page: 13 year: 1983 end-page: 22 article-title: Odor‐aversion learning by rats following LiCl exposure: Ontogenetic influences publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 40 start-page: 51 year: 2006 end-page: 59 article-title: Interactions between prenatal ethanol exposure and postnatal learning about ethanol in rat pups publication-title: Alcohol – volume: 47 start-page: 55 year: 2005 end-page: 65 article-title: Latent inhibition in the developing rat: An examination of context‐specific effects publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 15 start-page: 709 year: 1979 end-page: 720 article-title: Pre‐exposure to the Cs alone, Us alone, or Cs and Us uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context, or learned irrelevance? publication-title: Learning and Motivation – volume: 86 start-page: 523 year: 1979 end-page: 548 article-title: Associative and nonassociative theories of the UCS preexposure phenomenon: Implications for Pavlovian conditioning publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 17 start-page: 407 year: 1984 end-page: 422 article-title: Short‐term exposure to an odor increases its subsequent preference in preweanling rats: A descriptive profile of the phenomenon publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – start-page: 279 year: 1969 end-page: 296 – volume: 22 start-page: 401 year: 1989 end-page: 411 article-title: Anterior and posterior, but not cheek, intraoral cannulation procedures elevate serum corticosterone levels in neonatal rat pups publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 35 start-page: 767 year: 2010 end-page: 776 article-title: Evidence that the sweetness of odors depends on experience in rats publication-title: Chemical Senses – volume: 117 start-page: 113 year: 2003 end-page: 122 article-title: Stimulus preexposure reduces generalization of conditioned taste aversions between alcohol and non‐alcohol flavors in infant rats publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 108 start-page: 227 year: 1994 end-page: 234 article-title: Ontogeny of contextual fear conditioning in rats: Implications for consolidation, infantile amnesia, and hippocampal system function publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 33 start-page: 363 year: 2005 end-page: 370 article-title: The role of habituation of the response to LiCl in the US‐preexposure effect publication-title: Learning & Behavior – volume: 12 start-page: 321 year: 1981 end-page: 341 article-title: The US preexposure phenomenon in the conditioned suppression paradigm: A role for conditioned situational stimuli publication-title: Learning and Motivation – start-page: 64 year: 1972 end-page: 99 – volume: 120 start-page: 710 year: 2006 end-page: 718 article-title: Ethanol‐induced preferences or aversions as a function of age in preweanling rats publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 27 start-page: 362 year: 1996 end-page: 374 article-title: The importance of context in the US preexposure effect in CTA: Novel versus latently inhibited contextual stimuli publication-title: Learning and Motivation – volume: 52 start-page: 109 year: 2010 end-page: 120 article-title: The effect of taste familiarity on intake and taste reactivity in infant rats publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 30 start-page: 58 year: 2004 end-page: 66 article-title: The US‐preexposure effect in lithium‐induced flavor‐aversion conditioning is a consequence of blocking by injection cues publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes – volume: 3 start-page: 189 year: 1977 end-page: 201 article-title: Illness‐alone exposure as a source of interference with the acquisition and retention of a taste aversion publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes – volume: 112 start-page: 1142 year: 1998 end-page: 1151 article-title: Illness‐induced context aversion learning in rats with lesions of the dorsal hippocampus publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 9 start-page: 524 year: 1981 end-page: 532 article-title: Generality of US preexposure effects: Transfer from food to shock or shock to food with and without the same response requirements publication-title: Animal Learning & Behavior – ident: e_1_2_7_12_1 doi: 10.1006/lmot.1996.0021 – ident: e_1_2_7_33_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.420220407 – ident: e_1_2_7_4_1 doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2006.10.002 – ident: e_1_2_7_15_1 doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.1.58 – volume-title: Handbook of contemporary learning theories year: 2001 ident: e_1_2_7_30_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_27_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.523 – start-page: 279 volume-title: Punishment and aversive behavior year: 1969 ident: e_1_2_7_22_1 – start-page: 58 volume-title: Conditioned taste aversion: Behavioral and neural processes year: 2009 ident: e_1_2_7_19_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_34_1 doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.573 – ident: e_1_2_7_5_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.20460 – start-page: 64 volume-title: Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory year: 1972 ident: e_1_2_7_29_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_17_1 doi: 10.1101/lm.1755810 – volume: 15 start-page: 709 year: 1979 ident: e_1_2_7_6_1 article-title: Pre‐exposure to the Cs alone, Us alone, or Cs and Us uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context, or learned irrelevance? publication-title: Learning and Motivation – ident: e_1_2_7_16_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.20418 – ident: e_1_2_7_18_1 doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq075 – volume: 17 start-page: 34 year: 1989 ident: e_1_2_7_23_1 article-title: Enhancement of flavor aversion conditioning in weanling but not adult rats by prior conditioning to an odor publication-title: Psychobiology doi: 10.3758/BF03337815 – ident: e_1_2_7_7_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.126 – ident: e_1_2_7_11_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.1.113 – volume-title: National Research Council. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals year: 1996 ident: e_1_2_7_21_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_2_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.5.1142 – ident: e_1_2_7_3_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.710 – volume: 108 start-page: 227 year: 1994 ident: e_1_2_7_32_1 article-title: Ontogeny of contextual fear conditioning in rats: Implications for consolidation, infantile amnesia, and hippocampal system function publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.108.2.227 – ident: e_1_2_7_14_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03192864 – ident: e_1_2_7_24_1 doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.3.2.189 – ident: e_1_2_7_35_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.20074 – ident: e_1_2_7_26_1 doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(81)90012-6 – ident: e_1_2_7_36_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.20244 – ident: e_1_2_7_10_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.420170407 – ident: e_1_2_7_8_1 doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.1.3.270 – ident: e_1_2_7_28_1 doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjr026 – ident: e_1_2_7_31_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.420160103 – ident: e_1_2_7_20_1 doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(92)90020-B – ident: e_1_2_7_25_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03199972 – ident: e_1_2_7_9_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03209785 – ident: e_1_2_7_13_1 doi: 10.1080/02724990244000115 |
SSID | ssj0011487 |
Score | 1.9854668 |
Snippet | The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US‐PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to... The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US-PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref wiley istex |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 808 |
SubjectTerms | Animals associative learning Avoidance Learning - drug effects Avoidance Learning - physiology Conditioning, Classical - drug effects Conditioning, Classical - physiology Cues Extinction, Psychological - drug effects Extinction, Psychological - physiology Female infant Lithium Chloride - pharmacology Male Odorants Rats rodent Taste - drug effects Taste - physiology taste-aversion US preexposure effect |
Title | Signaling the unconditioned stimulus during the preexposure phase does not attenuate the unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect in preweanling rats |
URI | https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-RKNFXS61-H/fulltext.pdf https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002%2Fdev.21001 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22213476 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1143873876 |
Volume | 54 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwED9NmyaNBwblqwyQQQjtJV1iN1_iCbGVCkQfNgYVQrIcxxnTRlI1zRj8Ify93NlN2NBACCkPVny2nPjO97N9_hngqVE80YkJvUjlOEEJeeElxldo7lGB_i0QeU5LA28n0fhw-HoaTlfgeXsWxvFDdAtuZBl2vCYDV1m984s0NDdnA04MQjj-BiIi3vzd_Y46imC-48sMuIeYw29ZhXy-05W85IvW6LeeXwU0L-NW63hGm_CpbbKLNzkZNItsoL__xub4n990A64vASl74TToJqyYsgfr7orKbz24doGwsAcb3XiJWesfK5u6BT8Ojo8I0JdHDPEkQ1dZ0U44kSDlDMeQL81pUzN3ItJKzObGnM8qWp1ks8_oSFlemZqV1YIR32fZIAL-W1UXy7uIFHZc0tuvRpW2HajY9W04HO29ezn2lhc-eFpEIvAyntP8RZgk5Rk-fqGV9tPCT1WhMjUUkV-IvAiV0nkwDDSiR4R3PNVBHCcZ98UdWC2xPfeAcR3zQilUtyFd956qJCV0K1QRZaEK0j5st10v9ZINnS7lOJWOx5lL7Atp-6IPTzrRmaMAuUromdWfTkLNTyhmLg7lh8kruf9mMpoeRIEc9-Fxq2ASLZm2Z1Rpqqam8-EiifGJ-nDXaV5XG6I4OvOLOdtWf_7cELm7994m7v-76BZscGsnFKXzAFYX88Y8RKy1yB5Zo_oJIFommQ |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9QwEB6VVhXlwGOBsjwNQqiXbBPnLXFBtMtC2z30ASskZDmO01YtyWp3A4Ufwu9lxt6EFhWEkHKw4rHlxDOez_b4M8BzLXmiEh06kcxxghLywkm0K9HcowL9m-fnOS0N7AyjwUHwbhSOFuBlcxbG8kO0C25kGWa8JgOnBen1X6yhuf7S40QhdAWWAgQaNPXa2G3JowjoW8ZMjzuIOtyGV8jl623RC95oiX7s2WVQ8yJyNa6nfwM-NY22EScnvXqW9dT33_gc__erbsL1OSZlr6wS3YIFXXZg2d5S-a0D185xFnZgpR0yMWv5Y2VSt-HH3vEhYfrykCGkZOgtK9oMJx6knOEw8rk-rafMHoo0EuOJ1mfjihYo2fgIfSnLKz1lZTVjRPlZ1giC_1bV-fI2KIUdl_T2q5alaQfq9vQOHPQ3918PnPmdD47yI99zMp7TFMbXScozfNxCSeWmhZvKQmYy8CO38PMilFLlXuApBJCI8HiqvDhOMu76d2GxxPbcA8ZVzAspUeMCuvE9lUlKANeXRZSF0ku7sNb0vVBzQnS6l-NUWCpnLrAvhOmLLjxrRceWBeQyoRdGgVoJOTmhsLk4FB-Gb8Tu1rA_2os8MejC00bDBBoz7dDIUlf1lI6I-0mMT9SFVat6bW0I5OjYL-asGQX6c0PExuZ7k7j_76JP4Opgf2dbbL8dbj2AFW6MhoJ2HsLibFLrRwi9ZtljY2E_AXPZKrg |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3bbtQwEB2VVkXlgctyW64GIdSXbBPnLp4Q7bJQWKGWwgohRY5jl6oliXY3UPgQvpcZexNaVBBCyoMVjy0nnvEc2-NjgEdK8EQmKnQiUeAEJeTaSZQr0Nwjjf7N84uClgZej6PRXvByEk6W4El7FsbyQ3QLbmQZZrwmA68LvfGLNLRQXwacGITOwUoQIZIgRLTTcUcRzreEmR53EHS4La2Qyze6oqec0Qr91-OzkOZp4Go8z_ASfGzbbANODgfNPB_I77_ROf7nR12GiwtEyp5aFboCS6rswaq9o_JbDy6cYCzswVo3YGLW6ofKpK7Cj92DfUL05T5DQMnQV1a0FU4sSAXDQeRzc9TMmD0SaSTqqVLHdUXLk6z-hJ6UFZWasbKaMyL8LBuEwH-r6mR5G5LCDkp6-1WJ0rQDNXt2DfaGW2-fjZzFjQ-O9CPfc3Je0ATGV0nKc3xcLYV0U-2mQotcBH7kar_QoRCy8AJPInxEfMdT6cVxknPXvw7LJbbnJjAuY66FQH0L6L73VCQpwVtf6CgPhZf2Yb3t-kwu6NDpVo6jzBI58wz7IjN90YeHnWhtOUDOEnps9KeTENNDCpqLw-z9-Hm2sz0eTnYjLxv14UGrYBmaMu3PiFJVzYwOiPtJjE_UhxtW87raEMbRoV_MWTf68-eGZJtb70zi1r-L3ofzbzaH2asX4-3bsMaNyVDEzh1Ynk8bdRdx1zy_Z-zrJ9scKWc |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Signaling+the+unconditioned+stimulus+during+the+preexposure+phase+does+not+attenuate+the+unconditioned+stimulus+preexposure+effect+in+preweanling+rats&rft.jtitle=Developmental+psychobiology&rft.au=Castello%2C+Stefania&rft.au=Bobbio%2C+Antonella&rft.au=Orellana%2C+Estefania&rft.au=Arias%2C+Carlos&rft.date=2012-12-01&rft.pub=Wiley+Subscription+Services%2C+Inc.%2C+A+Wiley+Company&rft.issn=0012-1630&rft.eissn=1098-2302&rft.volume=54&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=808&rft.epage=817&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002%2Fdev.21001&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=ark_67375_WNG_RKNFXS61_H |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0012-1630&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0012-1630&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0012-1630&client=summon |