The medial olivocochlear reflex strength is modulated during a visual working memory task
The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of neurophysiology Vol. 125; no. 6; pp. 2309 - 2321 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
01.06.2021
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory.
Top-down modulation of sensory responses to distracting stimuli by selective attention has been proposed as an important mechanism by which our brain can maintain relevant information during working memory tasks. Previous works in visual working memory (VWM) have reported modulation of neural responses to distracting sounds at different levels of the central auditory pathways. Whether these modulations occur also at the level of the auditory receptor is unknown. Here, we hypothesize that cochlear responses to irrelevant auditory stimuli can be modulated by the medial olivocochlear system during VWM. Twenty-one subjects (13 males, mean age 25.3 yr) with normal hearing performed a visual change detection task with different VWM load conditions (high load = 4 visual objects; low load = 2 visual objects). Auditory stimuli were presented as distractors and allowed the measurement of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) and scalp auditory evoked potentials. In addition, the medial olivocochlear reflex strength was evaluated by adding contralateral acoustic stimulation. We found larger contralateral acoustic suppression of DPOAEs during the visual working memory period ( n = 21) compared with control experiments ( n = 10), in which individuals were passively exposed to the same experimental conditions. These results show that during the visual working memory period there is a modulation of the medial olivocochlear reflex strength, suggesting a possible common mechanism for top-down filtering of auditory responses during cognitive processes.
NEW & NOTEWORTHY The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Top-down modulation of sensory responses to distracting stimuli by selective attention has been proposed as an important mechanism by which our brain can maintain relevant information during working memory tasks. Previous works in visual working memory (VWM) have reported modulation of neural responses to distracting sounds at different levels of the central auditory pathways. Whether these modulations occur also at the level of the auditory receptor is unknown. Here, we hypothesize that cochlear responses to irrelevant auditory stimuli can be modulated by the medial olivocochlear system during VWM. Twenty-one subjects (13 males, mean age 25.3 yr) with normal hearing performed a visual change detection task with different VWM load conditions (high load = 4 visual objects; low load = 2 visual objects). Auditory stimuli were presented as distractors and allowed the measurement of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) and scalp auditory evoked potentials. In addition, the medial olivocochlear reflex strength was evaluated by adding contralateral acoustic stimulation. We found larger contralateral acoustic suppression of DPOAEs during the visual working memory period (
= 21) compared with control experiments (
= 10), in which individuals were passively exposed to the same experimental conditions. These results show that during the visual working memory period there is a modulation of the medial olivocochlear reflex strength, suggesting a possible common mechanism for top-down filtering of auditory responses during cognitive processes.
The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory. Top-down modulation of sensory responses to distracting stimuli by selective attention has been proposed as an important mechanism by which our brain can maintain relevant information during working memory tasks. Previous works in visual working memory (VWM) have reported modulation of neural responses to distracting sounds at different levels of the central auditory pathways. Whether these modulations occur also at the level of the auditory receptor is unknown. Here, we hypothesize that cochlear responses to irrelevant auditory stimuli can be modulated by the medial olivocochlear system during VWM. Twenty-one subjects (13 males, mean age 25.3 yr) with normal hearing performed a visual change detection task with different VWM load conditions (high load = 4 visual objects; low load = 2 visual objects). Auditory stimuli were presented as distractors and allowed the measurement of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) and scalp auditory evoked potentials. In addition, the medial olivocochlear reflex strength was evaluated by adding contralateral acoustic stimulation. We found larger contralateral acoustic suppression of DPOAEs during the visual working memory period (n = 21) compared with control experiments (n = 10), in which individuals were passively exposed to the same experimental conditions. These results show that during the visual working memory period there is a modulation of the medial olivocochlear reflex strength, suggesting a possible common mechanism for top-down filtering of auditory responses during cognitive processes.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory.Top-down modulation of sensory responses to distracting stimuli by selective attention has been proposed as an important mechanism by which our brain can maintain relevant information during working memory tasks. Previous works in visual working memory (VWM) have reported modulation of neural responses to distracting sounds at different levels of the central auditory pathways. Whether these modulations occur also at the level of the auditory receptor is unknown. Here, we hypothesize that cochlear responses to irrelevant auditory stimuli can be modulated by the medial olivocochlear system during VWM. Twenty-one subjects (13 males, mean age 25.3 yr) with normal hearing performed a visual change detection task with different VWM load conditions (high load = 4 visual objects; low load = 2 visual objects). Auditory stimuli were presented as distractors and allowed the measurement of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) and scalp auditory evoked potentials. In addition, the medial olivocochlear reflex strength was evaluated by adding contralateral acoustic stimulation. We found larger contralateral acoustic suppression of DPOAEs during the visual working memory period (n = 21) compared with control experiments (n = 10), in which individuals were passively exposed to the same experimental conditions. These results show that during the visual working memory period there is a modulation of the medial olivocochlear reflex strength, suggesting a possible common mechanism for top-down filtering of auditory responses during cognitive processes.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory. The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory. Top-down modulation of sensory responses to distracting stimuli by selective attention has been proposed as an important mechanism by which our brain can maintain relevant information during working memory tasks. Previous works in visual working memory (VWM) have reported modulation of neural responses to distracting sounds at different levels of the central auditory pathways. Whether these modulations occur also at the level of the auditory receptor is unknown. Here, we hypothesize that cochlear responses to irrelevant auditory stimuli can be modulated by the medial olivocochlear system during VWM. Twenty-one subjects (13 males, mean age 25.3 yr) with normal hearing performed a visual change detection task with different VWM load conditions (high load = 4 visual objects; low load = 2 visual objects). Auditory stimuli were presented as distractors and allowed the measurement of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) and scalp auditory evoked potentials. In addition, the medial olivocochlear reflex strength was evaluated by adding contralateral acoustic stimulation. We found larger contralateral acoustic suppression of DPOAEs during the visual working memory period ( n = 21) compared with control experiments ( n = 10), in which individuals were passively exposed to the same experimental conditions. These results show that during the visual working memory period there is a modulation of the medial olivocochlear reflex strength, suggesting a possible common mechanism for top-down filtering of auditory responses during cognitive processes. NEW & NOTEWORTHY The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded electroencephalographic activity and otoacoustic emissions in response to auditory distractors during a visual working memory task in humans. We found that the olivocochlear efferent activity is modulated during the visual working memory period suggesting a common mechanism for suppressing cochlear responses during selective attention and working memory. |
Author | Marcenaro, Bruno López, Vladimir Dragicevic, Constantino Delano, Paul H. Leiva, Alexis |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Bruno surname: Marcenaro fullname: Marcenaro, Bruno organization: Neuroscience Department, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Centro Avanzado de Ingeniería Eléctrica y Electrónica, AC3E, Universidad Técnica Federico Santa María, Valparaiso, Chile, Interdisciplinary Center of Neuroscience, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile – sequence: 2 givenname: Alexis surname: Leiva fullname: Leiva, Alexis organization: Neuroscience Department, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Biomedical Neuroscience Institute, BNI, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile – sequence: 3 givenname: Constantino orcidid: 0000-0002-1959-8271 surname: Dragicevic fullname: Dragicevic, Constantino organization: Neuroscience Department, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Biomedical Neuroscience Institute, BNI, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile – sequence: 4 givenname: Vladimir orcidid: 0000-0001-5794-9338 surname: López fullname: López, Vladimir organization: Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile – sequence: 5 givenname: Paul H. orcidid: 0000-0003-2588-4757 surname: Delano fullname: Delano, Paul H. organization: Neuroscience Department, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Otolaryngology Department, Hospital Clínico de la Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Biomedical Neuroscience Institute, BNI, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile, Centro Avanzado de Ingeniería Eléctrica y Electrónica, AC3E, Universidad Técnica Federico Santa María, Valparaiso, Chile |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33978484$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNptkDlPxDAQRi0EguUoaZFLmiw-cjglQlwSEg0UVNasM2G9ODHYDse_x8vRIKoZjd73SfN2yeboRyTkkLM555U4WY1zxpgUc8EE2yCzfBMFr1q1SWaM5V2yptkhuzGuMtdUTGyTHSnbRpWqnJGHuyXSATsLjnpnX73xZukQAg3YO3ynMQUcH9OS2kgH300OEna0m4IdHynQVxunHH3z4Wl9GHDw4YMmiE_7ZKsHF_HgZ-6R-4vzu7Or4ub28vrs9KYwsmpTIVG0XdPUCqHipjedMGWHFbQ9lEwozD-gEj3Usu6R9xIXYISqmQIGC6a43CPH373Pwb9MGJMebDToHIzop6hFJWou25qXGT36QadF_lk_BztA-NC_OjIgvwETfIzZgDY2QbJ-TAGs05zptXS9GvWXdL2WnlPFn9Rv8f_8JwdWg90 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2022_03_029 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2022_866161 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0665_21_2021 crossref_primary_10_1121_10_0014900 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhac002 crossref_primary_10_3389_fncir_2023_1301962 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01959 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2021_759219 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2023_09_016 |
Cites_doi | 10.1038/36846 10.1016/s0021-9924(98)00019-7 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00134 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1436-19.2019 10.1038/s41598-020-71399-8 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00137-x 10.1002/cne.20550 10.1038/nature04171 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00030 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.08.016 10.1371/journal.pone.0052267 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4031-15.2016 10.1111/ejn.12746 10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00380 10.3758/s13423-011-0055-3 10.3758/bf03208800 10.1007/s00359-008-0340-4 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4861-13.2014 10.1371/journal.pone.0155991 10.1046/j.1365-2273.2002.00541.x 10.1037/a0037163 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.017 10.1016/0014-4886(71)90003-3 10.1017/s0140525x01003922 10.4324/9781315625560 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.062 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.3.1305 10.1121/1.414508 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00845.x 10.1016/j.heares.2019.04.010 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31827ada02 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2385 10.1038/nn.3655 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-12-04701.2000 10.1097/00001756-200003200-00043 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00221 10.1037/xhp0000413 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 10.1016/j.heares.2017.02.003 10.1162/jocn_a_00275 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00619.x 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.015 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00012-8 10.1007/s12311-015-0694-4 10.55782/ane-2012-1902 10.1186/s12915-021-00992-8 10.1038/349413a0 10.1371/journal.pone.0165645 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00042 10.1093/cercor/bhj035 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3702-06.2007 10.1371/journal.pone.0208939 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.021 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.09.012 10.1007/s11065-012-9202-5 10.1007/s10162-015-0509-9 10.1073/pnas.1523471113 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1152/jn.00032.2020 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1522-1598 |
EndPage | 2321 |
ExternalDocumentID | 33978484 10_1152_jn_00032_2020 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 18M 29L 2WC 39C 4.4 53G 5GY 5VS AAYXX ABCQX ABHWK ABIVO ABJNI ABKWE ACGFO ACGFS ACNCT ADBBV ADFNX ADHGD ADIYS AENEX AFFNX AFOSN AIZAD ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BAWUL BKKCC BTFSW CITATION CS3 DIK DU5 E3Z EBS EMOBN F5P H13 H~9 ITBOX KQ8 L7B OK1 P2P RAP RHI RPL RPRKH SJN TR2 UHB UPT W8F WH7 WOQ WOW X7M XSW YBH YQT YSK CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c359t-3e29d7768ea51cfcd2c4de5a9fa4028e002e82fa636fe1f3ebac28608a0ab0813 |
ISSN | 0022-3077 1522-1598 |
IngestDate | Thu Jul 10 17:30:53 EDT 2025 Tue Apr 01 03:09:35 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:52:14 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:33:55 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 6 |
Keywords | cochlea corticofugal olivocochlear auditory efferent working memory |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c359t-3e29d7768ea51cfcd2c4de5a9fa4028e002e82fa636fe1f3ebac28608a0ab0813 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-2588-4757 0000-0002-1959-8271 0000-0001-5794-9338 |
PMID | 33978484 |
PQID | 2526139614 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 13 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2526139614 pubmed_primary_33978484 crossref_citationtrail_10_1152_jn_00032_2020 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00032_2020 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2021-06-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-06-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 06 year: 2021 text: 2021-06-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Journal of neurophysiology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Neurophysiol |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
References | B20 B21 B22 B23 B24 Schochat E (B44) 2012; 72 B25 B26 B27 B28 B29 Bishop DVM (B39) 2007; 10 B30 B31 B32 B33 B34 B35 B36 B37 B38 B1 B2 B3 B4 B5 B6 B8 B9 B40 B41 B42 B43 B45 B46 B47 B48 B49 Cowan N (B7) 2016 B50 B51 B52 B53 B10 B54 B11 B55 B12 B56 B13 B57 B14 B58 B15 B59 B16 B17 B18 B19 B60 B61 |
References_xml | – ident: B26 doi: 10.1038/36846 – ident: B45 doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(98)00019-7 – ident: B11 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00134 – ident: B61 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1436-19.2019 – ident: B58 doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-71399-8 – ident: B35 doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00137-x – ident: B46 doi: 10.1002/cne.20550 – ident: B52 doi: 10.1038/nature04171 – ident: B55 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00030 – ident: B13 doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.08.016 – ident: B34 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052267 – ident: B15 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4031-15.2016 – ident: B56 doi: 10.1111/ejn.12746 – ident: B32 doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979 – ident: B22 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00380 – ident: B29 doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0055-3 – ident: B28 doi: 10.3758/bf03208800 – ident: B40 doi: 10.1007/s00359-008-0340-4 – ident: B18 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4861-13.2014 – ident: B53 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155991 – ident: B48 doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2273.2002.00541.x – ident: B4 doi: 10.1037/a0037163 – ident: B6 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.017 – ident: B16 doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(71)90003-3 – ident: B27 doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01003922 – volume-title: Working Memory Capacity year: 2016 ident: B7 doi: 10.4324/9781315625560 – ident: B14 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.062 – ident: B20 doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.3.1305 – ident: B49 doi: 10.1121/1.414508 – ident: B43 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00845.x – ident: B12 doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2019.04.010 – ident: B41 doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31827ada02 – ident: B21 doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2385 – ident: B3 doi: 10.1038/nn.3655 – ident: B1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023 – ident: B57 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-12-04701.2000 – ident: B36 doi: 10.1097/00001756-200003200-00043 – ident: B8 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014 – ident: B23 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00221 – ident: B5 doi: 10.1037/xhp0000413 – ident: B31 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 – ident: B47 doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.02.003 – ident: B25 doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00275 – volume: 10 start-page: 565 year: 2007 ident: B39 publication-title: Dev Sci doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00619.x – ident: B33 doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013 – ident: B37 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.015 – ident: B50 doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00012-8 – ident: B10 doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0694-4 – volume: 72 start-page: 296 year: 2012 ident: B44 publication-title: Acta Neurobiol Exp doi: 10.55782/ane-2012-1902 – ident: B60 doi: 10.1186/s12915-021-00992-8 – ident: B30 doi: 10.1038/349413a0 – ident: B24 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165645 – ident: B59 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00042 – ident: B9 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj035 – ident: B17 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3702-06.2007 – ident: B19 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208939 – ident: B38 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.021 – ident: B42 doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.09.012 – ident: B2 doi: 10.1007/s11065-012-9202-5 – ident: B54 doi: 10.1007/s10162-015-0509-9 – ident: B51 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1523471113 |
SSID | ssj0007502 |
Score | 2.4189782 |
Snippet | The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a biological filter of cochlear responses during selective attention. Here, we recorded... Top-down modulation of sensory responses to distracting stimuli by selective attention has been proposed as an important mechanism by which our brain can... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 2309 |
SubjectTerms | Acoustic Stimulation Adult Auditory Perception - physiology Cochlea - physiology Cochlear Nucleus - physiology Efferent Pathways - physiology Electroencephalography Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology Female Hearing - physiology Humans Male Memory, Short-Term - physiology Reflex - physiology Superior Olivary Complex - physiology Visual Perception - physiology Young Adult |
Title | The medial olivocochlear reflex strength is modulated during a visual working memory task |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33978484 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2526139614 |
Volume | 125 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwELdgvPCCgPFRNpCR0F5GoPVH5jyOadMEY4DUovIUXRKHZbTJ1Gbl46_nbMdJK7XSoA9RZSVW5Pvp7mdffneEvEoAf6FIAim1CgQaPUiycBBoOIB-LjAkafuB7Hl4OhLvx3LcZXStuqRO3qR_1upK_seqOIZ2NSrZf7BsOykO4H-0L17Rwni9sY0b6Uc1KRYVOrcL0wZiH8PeRP-yQpDye31hupZPq8x06kJ-6ZWJ-4tibsQjP915Oc40NQn3GuY_NlBWW_zSnoWsHMZ_NHrJEpxixpQyqLrPfIoFeB1N0fWwnwE6XAzJtr36kaOodbH0nMnfv-NX7nj76wSyYlrMlk8o2NKXVN6p4oYXaZPzs3rNmPfETgPdQG7Fr3JbRGGNw5emgOxlaepQciOsY_0usvls_vmn-GR0dhYPj8fD2-QOwx2FaXbx4UtXWB6Jk02M-9fy5Vgle7sy-Sp92bAnsdxkeJ_cayxEDx1CHpBbunxItg9LqKvpb7pHP7cm2ybfEDTUgYaugIY60FAPGlrMaQsa6kBDgTrQ0AY01IGGGtA8IqOT4-HRadD01whSLqM64JpF2QHuNzXIQZqnGUtFpiVEOQiknRqDpVYsh5CHuR7kXCeQMhX2FfQhQSrJH5Otsir1U0ITlaqMh5AnAxCQSqUggggXWmD8ZJnukdd-2eK0KT5veqBMYrsJlSy-LGO7yrFZ5R7Za2-_clVXNt340tsgRr9okl1Q6up6HjPJkKlGyD575IkzTjsVRxKuhBLPbvD0DrnbYXqXbNWza_0ceWidvLAI-gsX1o0u |
linkProvider | Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+medial+olivocochlear+reflex+strength+is+modulated+during+a+visual+working+memory+task&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+neurophysiology&rft.au=Marcenaro%2C+Bruno&rft.au=Leiva%2C+Alexis&rft.au=Dragicevic%2C+Constantino&rft.au=L%C3%B3pez%2C+Vladimir&rft.date=2021-06-01&rft.issn=1522-1598&rft.eissn=1522-1598&rft.volume=125&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=2309&rft_id=info:doi/10.1152%2Fjn.00032.2020&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0022-3077&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0022-3077&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0022-3077&client=summon |