Watch Your Tone: Social Conditions Modulate Singing Strategies
Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long‐distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females. Nevertheless, many songbirds sing from close distances to a known receiver; males of these species may be under more intense selective pressure to mod...
Saved in:
Published in | Ethology Vol. 121; no. 11; pp. 1104 - 1115 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Hamburg
P. Parey
01.11.2015
Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long‐distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females. Nevertheless, many songbirds sing from close distances to a known receiver; males of these species may be under more intense selective pressure to modify their songs depending on the sex of the receiver in order to convey different motivational states (aggression versus courtship) to the different sexes. In a laboratory setting, we examined how receiver sex affected within‐song variation of the close‐range singing behavior in the brown‐headed cowbird (Molothrus ater). Although we know that cowbird song is influenced by flock composition, it is still unclear as to how the cowbird modifies his song based on social context. Using a cross‐correlation analysis of each male's different song types, we found that pairs of songs were significantly more dissimilar if they were directed to females compared with songs directed to males. We subsequently tested whether there were any consistent spectral or temporal patterns in the songs males gave to females versus to males. Our results lend support for the Motivational Structural Rules Hypothesis as songs directed toward males had higher entropy (i.e., harshness) than the same song type directed toward females. Our results suggest that cowbirds may have evolved the ability to alter multiple dimensions of their singing behavior based on receiver sex. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Abstract
Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long‐distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females. Nevertheless, many songbirds sing from close distances to a known receiver; males of these species may be under more intense selective pressure to modify their songs depending on the sex of the receiver in order to convey different motivational states (aggression versus courtship) to the different sexes. In a laboratory setting, we examined how receiver sex affected within‐song variation of the close‐range singing behavior in the brown‐headed cowbird (
Molothrus ater
). Although we know that cowbird song is influenced by flock composition, it is still unclear as to how the cowbird modifies his song based on social context. Using a cross‐correlation analysis of each male's different song types, we found that pairs of songs were significantly more dissimilar if they were directed to females compared with songs directed to males. We subsequently tested whether there were any consistent spectral or temporal patterns in the songs males gave to females versus to males. Our results lend support for the Motivational Structural Rules Hypothesis as songs directed toward males had higher entropy (i.e., harshness) than the same song type directed toward females. Our results suggest that cowbirds may have evolved the ability to alter multiple dimensions of their singing behavior based on receiver sex. Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long-distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females. Nevertheless, many songbirds sing from close distances to a known receiver; males of these species may be under more intense selective pressure to modify their songs depending on the sex of the receiver in order to convey different motivational states (aggression versus courtship) to the different sexes. In a laboratory setting, we examined how receiver sex affected within-song variation of the close-range singing behavior in the brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater). Although we know that cowbird song is influenced by flock composition, it is still unclear as to how the cowbird modifies his song based on social context. Using a cross-correlation analysis of each male's different song types, we found that pairs of songs were significantly more dissimilar if they were directed to females compared with songs directed to males. We subsequently tested whether there were any consistent spectral or temporal patterns in the songs males gave to females versus to males. Our results lend support for the Motivational Structural Rules Hypothesis as songs directed toward males had higher entropy (i.e., harshness) than the same song type directed toward females. Our results suggest that cowbirds may have evolved the ability to alter multiple dimensions of their singing behavior based on receiver sex. |
Author | Li, Qingling Skillman, Tasha Lucas, Jeffrey R Lin, Andy Fernandez‐Juricic, Esteban Koenig, W Ronald, Kelly L |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 fullname: Ronald, Kelly L – sequence: 2 fullname: Skillman, Tasha – sequence: 3 fullname: Lin, Andy – sequence: 4 fullname: Li, Qingling – sequence: 5 fullname: Fernandez‐Juricic, Esteban – sequence: 6 fullname: Lucas, Jeffrey R – sequence: 7 fullname: Koenig, W |
BookMark | eNp1kE1LAzEQhoMo2FYP_gIXPHnYNpNs9sODILUflqrgthRPIbubbVPrpiZbtP_e6Ko3h4Fh4Hnn422jw0pXEqEzwF1w0ZP1qgskIOwAtSCgiY8p4EPUwhAlPoRAj1Hb2jV2PY1oC10vRJ2vvGe9M97MjbryUp0rsfH6uipUrXRlvXtd7Daill6qqqVLL62Na5dK2hN0VIqNlac_tYPmw8GsP_anj6O7_s3UzylLmM8EKUPKSppTwkQWkYDhOMoLAQxTIUoSRhkhCYisAMiAZpiRAMIkD2ISlFTQDrpo5m6NfttJW_O1u7hyKzlEhOAkBhw76rKhcqOtNbLkW6NehdlzwPzLHu7s4d_2OLbXsO9qI_f_g3wwG_8q_EahbC0__hTCvPDQmcn44mHEJ-lwQp9upzxw_HnDl0JzsTTK8nlKsHsZExIxGtNPq4t-QA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2018_0713 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40857_024_00314_2 crossref_primary_10_1002_ece3_8883 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_arx015 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_araa132 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0208619 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psj_2024_103738 crossref_primary_10_1163_1568539X_00003475 crossref_primary_10_1002_ece3_7421 crossref_primary_10_1676_21_00004 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/s00265-002-0551-6 10.1037/h0077657 10.1163/156853902320262781 10.1126/science.841321 10.1086/283219 10.1676/0043-5643(2000)112[0035:GCOBFA]2.0.CO;2 10.1007/BF00165834 10.1126/science.157.3790.769 10.1006/anbe.2001.1860 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.016 10.1017/CBO9780511754791 10.1111/eth.12341 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.001 10.1007/978-1-4615-6784-4_3 10.1002/neu.480250910 10.1016/j.ygcen.2004.03.015 10.1525/cond.2013.120073 10.1371/journal.pone.0036130 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.016 10.1016/B978-0-08-092416-8.50015-2 10.1037/h0077577 10.1080/09524622.2000.9753447 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01518.x 10.2307/4089197 10.1098/rspb.2009.0580 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.035 10.1086/283568 10.1006/anbe.2003.2167 10.1007/s10682-011-9541-3 10.1017/CBO9780511610363 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.017 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.008 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.005 10.1126/science.1091099 10.1098/rstb.2011.0223 10.3838/jjo.49.69 10.1016/B978-0-08-092416-8.50014-0 10.1038/334244a0 10.1037/0735-7036.109.4.357 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00680.x 10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80251-3 10.1007/BF00612017 10.1006/anbe.1996.0304 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747 10.1086/286114 10.1007/BF00300047 10.1006/anbe.1999.1416 10.1080/09524622.1998.9753385 10.1075/is.12.2.08wes 10.1007/s00265-010-1120-z 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.014 10.1016/j.tree.2012.09.003 10.1006/anbe.1993.1190 10.1163/1568539X-00003090 10.1006/anbe.2001.1951 10.1093/beheco/aru014 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.08.011 10.1007/BF00303842 10.1037/a0029681 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2015 Blackwell Verlag GmbH Copyright © 2015 Blackwell Verlag GmbH |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2015 Blackwell Verlag GmbH – notice: Copyright © 2015 Blackwell Verlag GmbH |
DBID | FBQ BSCLL AAYXX CITATION 7QG 7QR 7SN 7SS 8FD C1K FR3 P64 |
DOI | 10.1111/eth.12425 |
DatabaseName | AGRIS Istex CrossRef Animal Behavior Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Engineering Research Database Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef Entomology Abstracts Technology Research Database Animal Behavior Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Engineering Research Database Ecology Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef Entomology Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: FBQ name: AGRIS url: http://www.fao.org/agris/Centre.asp?Menu_1ID=DB&Menu_2ID=DB1&Language=EN&Content=http://www.fao.org/agris/search?Language=EN sourceTypes: Publisher |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Zoology Psychology |
EISSN | 1439-0310 |
Editor | Koenig, W. |
Editor_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: W. surname: Koenig fullname: Koenig, W. – sequence: 7 givenname: W. surname: Koenig fullname: Koenig, W. |
EndPage | 1115 |
ExternalDocumentID | 3836277221 10_1111_eth_12425 ETH12425 ark_67375_WNG_JSFJ3RDL_4 US201500227538 |
Genre | article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: National Science Foundation (NSF) funderid: IOS 1121728 |
GroupedDBID | -ET .3N .GA .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1OB 1OC 29G 31~ 33P 3SF 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51X 52M 52N 52O 52P 52S 52T 52U 52W 52X 53G 5GY 5HH 5LA 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A03 AAESR AAEVG AAHHS AANLZ AAONW AASGY AAXRX AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABEML ABHUG ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABPTK ABPVW ABTAH ABWRO ACAHQ ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFS ACIWK ACPOU ACPRK ACSCC ACXBN ACXME ACXQS ADAWD ADBBV ADDAD ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADOZA ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFPM AFGKR AFPWT AFRAH AFVGU AFZJQ AGJLS AHEFC AI. AIURR AIWBW AJBDE AJXKR ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ATUGU AUFTA AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BHBCM BIYOS BMNLL BMXJE BNHUX BROTX BRXPI BY8 CAG COF CS3 D-E D-F DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRSTM DU5 EBS ECGQY EJD ESX F00 F01 F04 FBQ FEDTE FZ0 G-S G.N GODZA H.T H.X HF~ HVGLF HZI HZ~ H~9 IHE IX1 J0M K48 LATKE LC2 LC3 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRSTM MSFUL MSSTM MVM MXFUL MXSTM N04 N05 N9A NF~ O66 O9- P2P P2W P2X P4D PALCI PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 R.K RIWAO RJQFR ROL RX1 SAMSI SUPJJ UB1 VH1 W8V W99 WBKPD WIH WIK WNSPC WOHZO WQJ WRC WXSBR WYISQ XG1 XOL Y6R ZY4 ZZTAW ~IA ~KM ~WT AAHBH AHBTC AITYG BSCLL HGLYW OIG AAYXX CITATION 7QG 7QR 7SN 7SS 8FD C1K FR3 P64 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c3595-5a2f635f3c325ab7245087cda1503aaf267b2291abd11b13b0524169c4824f3a3 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0179-1613 |
IngestDate | Thu Oct 10 17:07:59 EDT 2024 Fri Aug 23 00:20:01 EDT 2024 Sat Aug 24 00:51:16 EDT 2024 Wed Oct 30 09:52:41 EDT 2024 Wed Dec 27 19:19:41 EST 2023 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 11 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c3595-5a2f635f3c325ab7245087cda1503aaf267b2291abd11b13b0524169c4824f3a3 |
Notes | http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/eth.12425 ark:/67375/WNG-JSFJ3RDL-4 ArticleID:ETH12425 istex:F0CF5AAE5500C3CB75AE9046A96D0C616342BD5A Table S1: Data Description. Fig S1-S3: Interaction between Song Type (Singer) and sex on entropy across the different song components: Glug 1, Glug 2, and P2 for 5 of the 7 birds examined. Entropy is consistently lower in songs sung to females (dark gray circles) than those songs sung to males (light gray circles). Least Squares Means were derived from a repeated measures analysis of variance analysis (see text). Note that these LS Means are estimated within songs sung by each male.Table S2: Statistics associated with the main effect of song type on spectral and temporal song properties. National Science Foundation (NSF) - No. IOS 1121728 |
PQID | 1722098108 |
PQPubID | 46960 |
PageCount | 12 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_journals_1722098108 crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_12425 wiley_primary_10_1111_eth_12425_ETH12425 istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_JSFJ3RDL_4 fao_agris_US201500227538 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | November 2015 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2015-11-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2015 text: November 2015 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Hamburg |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Hamburg |
PublicationTitle | Ethology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Ethology |
PublicationYear | 2015 |
Publisher | P. Parey Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
Publisher_xml | – name: P. Parey – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
References | Wiley, R. H. & Richards, D. G. 1978: Physical constraints on acoustic communication in atmosphere-implications for evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 3, 69-94. Reichard, D. G., Rice, R. J., Schultz, E. M. & Schrock, S. E. 2013: Low-amplitude songs produced by male dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) differ when sung during intra- and inter-sexual interactions. Behaviour 150, 1183-1202. Naguib, M. & Wiley, R. H. 2001: Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication. Anim. Behav. 62, 825-837. Rothstein, S. I., Yokel, D. A. & Fleischer, R. C. 1988: The agonistic and sexual functions of vocalizations of male brown-headed cowbirds, molothrus ater. Anim. Behav. 36, 73-86. Marler, P. 1967: Animal communication signals. Science 157, 769-774. Bradbury, J. W. & Vehrencamp, S. L. 2014: Complexity and behavioral ecology. Behav. Ecol. 25, 435-442. Benedict, L., Rose, A. & Warning, N. 2012: Canyon wrens alter their songs in response to territorial challenges. Anim. Behav. 84, 1463-1467. Leitao, A. & Riebel, K. 2003: Are good ornaments bad armaments? male chaffinch perception of songs with varying flourish length. Anim. Behav. 66, 161-167. Rothstein, S. I., Yokel, D. A. & Fleischer, R. C. 1986: Social dominance, mating and spacing systems, female fecundity, and vocal dialects in captive and free ranging brown headed cowbirds. Curr. Ornithol. 3, 127-185. Ritschard, M. & Brumm, H. 2012: Zebra finch song reflects current food availability. Evol. Ecol. 26, 801-812. West, M. J. & King, A. P. 1980: Enriching cowbird song by social deprivation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 94, 263-270. Balsby, T. J. S. 2000: The function of song in Whitethroats Sylvia communis. Bioacoustics 11, 17-30. King, A. P. & West, M. J. 1983: Dissecting cowbird song potency-assessing a songs geographic identity and relative appeal. J. Comp. Ethol. 63, 37-50. Kohn, G. M., King, A. P., Dohme, R., Meredith, G. R. & West, M. J. 2013: In the company of cowbirds, molothrus ater ater: robust patterns of sociability predict reproductive performance. J. Comp. Psychol. 127, 40-48. Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. 2008: Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge U.K. Fernandez-Juricic, E. & Martella, M. B. 2000: Guttural calls of Blue-fronted Amazons: structure, context, and their possible role in short range communication. Wilson Bull. 112, 35-43. Cooper, B. G. & Goller, F. 2004: Multimodal signals: Enhancement and constraint of song motor patterns by visual display. Science 303, 544-546. Dabelsteen, T., McGregor, P. K., Lampe, H. M., Langmore, N. E. & Holland, J. 1998: Quiet song in song birds: an overlooked phenomenon. Bioacoustics 9, 89-105. McGregor, P. K. 2005: Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK. Dufty, A. M. 1986: Singing and the establishment and maintenance of dominance hierarchies in captive brown-headed cowbirds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19, 49-55. Anderson, R. C., Searcy, W. A., Hughes, M. & Nowicki, S. 2012: The receiver-dependent cost of soft song: a signal of aggressive intent in songbirds. Anim. Behav. 83, 1443-1448. Brenowitz, E. A. 1982: The active space or red-winged blackbird song. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 147, 511-522. Hunt, K. E. & Wingfield, J. C. 2004: Effect of estradiol implants on reproductive behavior of female Lapland longspurs (Calcarius lapponicus). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 137, 248-262. Morton, E. S. 2000: An evolutionary view of the origins and functions of avian vocal communication. Jpn. J. Ornithol. 49, 69-78. Dohme, R., King, A. P., Meredith, G. R. & West, M. J. 2015: Is female visual signaling to male song socially regulated in brown-headed cowbirds? Ethology 121, 327-334. White, D. J., Gersick, A. S. & Snyder-Mackler, N. 2012: Social networks and the development of social skills in cowbirds. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 367, 1892-1900. Nelson, D. A. & Poesel, A. 2011: Song length variation serves multiple functions in the white-crowned sparrow. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 1103-1111. Kohn, G. M., King, A. P., Scherschel, L. L. & West, M. J. 2011: Social niches and sex assortment: uncovering the developmental ecology of brown-headed cowbirds, Molothrus ater. Anim. Behav. 82, 1015-1022. Møller, A. P., Saino, N., Taramino, G., Galeotti, P. & Ferrario, S. 1998: Paternity and multiple signaling: effects of a secondary sexual character and song on paternity in the barn swallow. Am. Nat. 151, 236-242. Galeotti, P., Saino, N., Sacchi, R. & Moller, A. P. 1997: Song correlates with social context, testosterone and body condition in male barn swallows. Anim. Behav. 53, 687-700. O'Loghlen, A. L. & Rothstein, S. I. 2010: Multimodal signalling in a songbird: male audiovisual displays vary significantly by social context in brown-headed cowbirds. Anim. Behav. 79, 1285-1292. Allan, S. E. & Suthers, R. A. 1994: Lateralization and motor stereotypy of song production in the brown-headed cowbird. J. Neurobiol. 25, 1154-1166. Tchernichovski, O., Nottebohm, F., Ho, C. E., Pesaran, B. & Mitra, P. P. 2000: A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity. Anim. Behav. 59, 1167-1176. McGraw, K. J., Mackillop, E. A., Dale, J. & Hauber, M. E. 2002: Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin- and structurally based ornamental plumage. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 3747-3755. Poesel, A., Foerster, K. & Kempenaers, B. 2001: The dawn song of the blue tit Parus caeruleus and its role in sexual selection. Ethology 107, 521-531. O'Loghlen, A. L. & Rothstein, S. I. 2002: Ecological effects on song learning: delayed development is widespread in wild populations of brown-headed cowbirds. Anim. Behav. 63, 475-486. Anderson, R. C., Searcy, W. A., Peters, S. & Nowicki, S. 2008: Soft song in song sparrows: acoustic structure and implications for signal function. Ethology 114, 662-676. Freeberg, T. M., King, A. P. & West, M. J. 1995: Social malleability in cowbirds (Molothrus-ater-artmisiae)-species and mate recognition in the first 2 years of life. J. Comp. Psychol. 109, 357-367. Taborsky, B. & Oliveira, R. F. 2012: Social competence: an evolutionary approach. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 679-688. O'Loghlen, A. L., Merrill, L. & Rothstein, S. I. 2013: Fidelity of song imitation and stability of dialect songs in brown-headed cowbirds. Condor 115, 677-686. O'Loghlen, A. L. & Rothstein, S. I. 2012: When less is best: female brown-headed cowbirds prefer less intense male displays. PLoS ONE 7, 8. Leitao, A., Ten Cate, C. & Riebel, K. 2006: Within-song complexity in a songbird is meaningful to both male and female receivers. Anim. Behav. 71, 1289-1296. West, M. J. & King, A. P. 1988: Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbird. Nature 334, 244-246. Marler, P. & Slabbekoorn, H. 2004: Nature's Music: The Science of Bird Song. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, CA. O'Loghlen, A. L. & Rothstein, S. I. 2003: Female preference for the songs of older males and the maintenance of dialects in brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 53, 102-109. Akcay, C., Tom, M. E., Holmes, D., Campbell, S. E. & Beecher, M. D. 2011: Sing softly and carry a big stick: signals of aggressive intent in the song sparrow. Anim. Behav. 82, 377-382. O'Loghlen, A. L. & Rothstein, S. I. 1995: Culturally correct song dialects are correlated with male age and female song preferences in wild populations of brown-headed cowbirds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36, 251-259. Richards, D. G. & Wiley, R. H. 1980: Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest-implications for animal communication. Am. Nat. 115, 381-399. Gersick, A. S., Snyder-Mackler, N. & White, D. J. 2012: Ontogeny of social skills: social complexity improves mating and competitive strategies in male brown-headed cowbirds. Anim. Behav. 83, 1171-1177. West, M. J., King, A. P. & Kohn, G. 2011: Developmental ecology: platform for designing a communication system. Interact. Stud. 12, 350-370. White, D. J., King, A. P. & West, M. J. 2002: Plasticity in adult development: experience with young males enhances mating competence in adult male cowbirds, Molothrus ater. Behaviour 139, 713-728. Greenewalt, C. H. 1968: Bird Song: Acoustics and Physiology. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D.C. Freed-Brown, G. & White, D. J. 2009: Acoustic mate copying: female cowbirds attend to other females' vocalizations to modify their song preferences. Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 3319-3325. King, A. P. & West, M. J. 1977: Species identification in the brown-headed cowbird: appropriate responses to abnormal song. Science 195, 1002-1004. Morton, E. S. 1977: Occurrence and significance of motivation structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. Am. Nat. 111, 855-869. West, M. J., King, A. P., Eastzer, D. H. & Staddon, J. E. R. 1979: Bioassay of isolate cowbird song. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 93, 124-133. O'Loghlen, A. L. & Rothstein, S. I. 1993: An extreme example of delayed vocal development-song learning in a population of wild brown-headed cowbirds. Anim. Behav. 46, 293-304. White, D. J., Gersick, A. S., Freed-Brown, G. & Snyder-Mackler, N. 2010: The ontogeny of social skills: experimental increases in social complexity enhance reproductive success in adult cowbirds. Anim. Behav. 79, 385-390. Searcy, W. A. & Beecher, M. D. 2009: Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds. Anim. Behav. 78, 1281-1292. Titus, R. C. 1998: Short-range and long-range songs: use of two acoustically distinct song classes by Dark-eyed Juncos. Auk 115, 386-393. 2006; 71 2000; 49 1995; 36 2013; 127 1988; 36 1967; 157 2009; 276 1994; 25 2014; 25 1978; 3 1998; 115 2011; 12 1982; 147 2012; 367 2001; 107 1998; 151 2003; 53 1986; 1 2004; 137 1982; 1 2000; 59 1997; 53 1986; 3 2000; 11 2013; 115 1988; 334 1983; 63 2011; 65 1982 2008; 114 2012; 27 2012; 26 2013; 150 2012; 83 1980; 115 1993; 46 2004; 303 2010; 79 2011; 82 2015; 121 2009 1998 2002; 139 2008 1986; 19 2005 2004 2000; 112 2001; 62 1979; 93 2009; 78 2002; 63 1980; 94 1995; 109 2002; 205 1977; 111 2012; 7 1977; 195 1968 1998; 9 2003; 66 2012; 84 e_1_2_6_51_1 e_1_2_6_53_1 e_1_2_6_32_1 e_1_2_6_30_1 Marler P. (e_1_2_6_31_1) 2004 e_1_2_6_19_1 e_1_2_6_13_1 e_1_2_6_36_1 e_1_2_6_59_1 King A. P. (e_1_2_6_25_1) 1983; 63 e_1_2_6_11_1 e_1_2_6_34_1 e_1_2_6_17_1 e_1_2_6_55_1 e_1_2_6_15_1 e_1_2_6_38_1 e_1_2_6_57_1 e_1_2_6_62_1 e_1_2_6_64_1 e_1_2_6_43_1 e_1_2_6_20_1 e_1_2_6_41_1 e_1_2_6_60_1 e_1_2_6_9_1 e_1_2_6_5_1 e_1_2_6_7_1 e_1_2_6_24_1 e_1_2_6_49_1 e_1_2_6_3_1 e_1_2_6_22_1 e_1_2_6_66_1 e_1_2_6_28_1 e_1_2_6_45_1 e_1_2_6_26_1 e_1_2_6_47_1 e_1_2_6_52_1 e_1_2_6_54_1 e_1_2_6_10_1 e_1_2_6_50_1 e_1_2_6_14_1 e_1_2_6_35_1 e_1_2_6_12_1 e_1_2_6_33_1 e_1_2_6_18_1 e_1_2_6_39_1 e_1_2_6_56_1 e_1_2_6_16_1 e_1_2_6_37_1 e_1_2_6_58_1 e_1_2_6_63_1 e_1_2_6_42_1 e_1_2_6_65_1 e_1_2_6_40_1 e_1_2_6_61_1 e_1_2_6_8_1 e_1_2_6_4_1 e_1_2_6_6_1 Greenewalt C. H. (e_1_2_6_21_1) 1968 e_1_2_6_48_1 e_1_2_6_23_1 e_1_2_6_2_1 e_1_2_6_29_1 e_1_2_6_44_1 e_1_2_6_27_1 e_1_2_6_46_1 |
References_xml | – volume: 19 start-page: 49 year: 1986 end-page: 55 article-title: Singing and the establishment and maintenance of dominance hierarchies in captive brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. – year: 2009 – volume: 63 start-page: 475 year: 2002 end-page: 486 article-title: Ecological effects on song learning: delayed development is widespread in wild populations of brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – start-page: 76 year: 1998 end-page: 83 – volume: 367 start-page: 1892 year: 2012 end-page: 1900 article-title: Social networks and the development of social skills in cowbirds publication-title: Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. – volume: 150 start-page: 1183 year: 2013 end-page: 1202 article-title: Low‐amplitude songs produced by male dark‐eyed juncos ( ) differ when sung during intra‐ and inter‐sexual interactions publication-title: Behaviour – year: 2005 – volume: 3 start-page: 69 year: 1978 end-page: 94 article-title: Physical constraints on acoustic communication in atmosphere‐implications for evolution of animal vocalizations publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. – volume: 7 start-page: 8 year: 2012 article-title: When less is best: female brown‐headed cowbirds prefer less intense male displays publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 151 start-page: 236 year: 1998 end-page: 242 article-title: Paternity and multiple signaling: effects of a secondary sexual character and song on paternity in the barn swallow publication-title: Am. Nat. – volume: 82 start-page: 1015 year: 2011 end-page: 1022 article-title: Social niches and sex assortment: uncovering the developmental ecology of brown‐headed cowbirds, publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 82 start-page: 377 year: 2011 end-page: 382 article-title: Sing softly and carry a big stick: signals of aggressive intent in the song sparrow publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 115 start-page: 381 year: 1980 end-page: 399 article-title: Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest‐implications for animal communication publication-title: Am. Nat. – volume: 114 start-page: 662 year: 2008 end-page: 676 article-title: Soft song in song sparrows: acoustic structure and implications for signal function publication-title: Ethology – volume: 9 start-page: 89 year: 1998 end-page: 105 article-title: Quiet song in song birds: an overlooked phenomenon publication-title: Bioacoustics – volume: 139 start-page: 713 year: 2002 end-page: 728 article-title: Plasticity in adult development: experience with young males enhances mating competence in adult male cowbirds, publication-title: Behaviour – volume: 65 start-page: 1103 year: 2011 end-page: 1111 article-title: Song length variation serves multiple functions in the white‐crowned sparrow publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. – volume: 11 start-page: 17 year: 2000 end-page: 30 article-title: The function of song in Whitethroats Sylvia communis publication-title: Bioacoustics – volume: 66 start-page: 161 year: 2003 end-page: 167 article-title: Are good ornaments bad armaments? male chaffinch perception of songs with varying flourish length publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 83 start-page: 1171 year: 2012 end-page: 1177 article-title: Ontogeny of social skills: social complexity improves mating and competitive strategies in male brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 1 start-page: 131 year: 1982 end-page: 181 – volume: 36 start-page: 73 year: 1988 end-page: 86 article-title: The agonistic and sexual functions of vocalizations of male brown‐headed cowbirds, molothrus ater publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 94 start-page: 263 year: 1980 end-page: 270 article-title: Enriching cowbird song by social deprivation publication-title: J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. – volume: 303 start-page: 544 year: 2004 end-page: 546 article-title: Multimodal signals: Enhancement and constraint of song motor patterns by visual display publication-title: Science – volume: 1 start-page: 164 year: 1986 end-page: 172 – volume: 276 start-page: 3319 year: 2009 end-page: 3325 article-title: Acoustic mate copying: female cowbirds attend to other females’ vocalizations to modify their song preferences publication-title: Proc. Biol. Sci. – volume: 127 start-page: 40 year: 2013 end-page: 48 article-title: In the company of cowbirds, molothrus ater ater: robust patterns of sociability predict reproductive performance publication-title: J. Comp. Psychol. – volume: 78 start-page: 1281 year: 2009 end-page: 1292 article-title: Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – year: 2008 – volume: 63 start-page: 37 year: 1983 end-page: 50 article-title: Dissecting cowbird song potency‐assessing a songs geographic identity and relative appeal publication-title: J. Comp. Ethol. – year: 2004 – volume: 112 start-page: 35 year: 2000 end-page: 43 article-title: Guttural calls of Blue‐fronted Amazons: structure, context, and their possible role in short range communication publication-title: Wilson Bull. – volume: 121 start-page: 327 year: 2015 end-page: 334 article-title: Is female visual signaling to male song socially regulated in brown‐headed cowbirds? publication-title: Ethology – volume: 79 start-page: 385 year: 2010 end-page: 390 article-title: The ontogeny of social skills: experimental increases in social complexity enhance reproductive success in adult cowbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – start-page: 183 year: 1982 end-page: 212 – volume: 27 start-page: 679 year: 2012 end-page: 688 article-title: Social competence: an evolutionary approach publication-title: Trends Ecol. Evol. – volume: 137 start-page: 248 year: 2004 end-page: 262 article-title: Effect of estradiol implants on reproductive behavior of female Lapland longspurs ( ) publication-title: Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. – volume: 3 start-page: 127 year: 1986 end-page: 185 article-title: Social dominance, mating and spacing systems, female fecundity, and vocal dialects in captive and free ranging brown headed cowbirds publication-title: Curr. Ornithol. – volume: 25 start-page: 1154 year: 1994 end-page: 1166 article-title: Lateralization and motor stereotypy of song production in the brown‐headed cowbird publication-title: J. Neurobiol. – volume: 109 start-page: 357 year: 1995 end-page: 367 article-title: Social malleability in cowbirds (Molothrus‐ater‐artmisiae)‐species and mate recognition in the first 2 years of life publication-title: J. Comp. Psychol. – year: 1968 – volume: 46 start-page: 293 year: 1993 end-page: 304 article-title: An extreme example of delayed vocal development‐song learning in a population of wild brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 147 start-page: 511 year: 1982 end-page: 522 article-title: The active space or red‐winged blackbird song publication-title: J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. – volume: 111 start-page: 855 year: 1977 end-page: 869 article-title: Occurrence and significance of motivation structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds publication-title: Am. Nat. – volume: 12 start-page: 350 year: 2011 end-page: 370 article-title: Developmental ecology: platform for designing a communication system publication-title: Interact. Stud. – volume: 49 start-page: 69 year: 2000 end-page: 78 article-title: An evolutionary view of the origins and functions of avian vocal communication publication-title: Jpn. J. Ornithol. – volume: 36 start-page: 251 year: 1995 end-page: 259 article-title: Culturally correct song dialects are correlated with male age and female song preferences in wild populations of brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. – volume: 79 start-page: 1285 year: 2010 end-page: 1292 article-title: Multimodal signalling in a songbird: male audiovisual displays vary significantly by social context in brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 59 start-page: 1167 year: 2000 end-page: 1176 article-title: A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 115 start-page: 386 year: 1998 end-page: 393 article-title: Short‐range and long‐range songs: use of two acoustically distinct song classes by Dark‐eyed Juncos publication-title: Auk – volume: 83 start-page: 1443 year: 2012 end-page: 1448 article-title: The receiver‐dependent cost of soft song: a signal of aggressive intent in songbirds publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 334 start-page: 244 year: 1988 end-page: 246 article-title: Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbird publication-title: Nature – volume: 71 start-page: 1289 year: 2006 end-page: 1296 article-title: Within‐song complexity in a songbird is meaningful to both male and female receivers publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 53 start-page: 102 year: 2003 end-page: 109 article-title: Female preference for the songs of older males and the maintenance of dialects in brown‐headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) publication-title: Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. – volume: 26 start-page: 801 year: 2012 end-page: 812 article-title: Zebra finch song reflects current food availability publication-title: Evol. Ecol. – volume: 93 start-page: 124 year: 1979 end-page: 133 article-title: Bioassay of isolate cowbird song publication-title: J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. – volume: 157 start-page: 769 year: 1967 end-page: 774 article-title: Animal communication signals publication-title: Science – volume: 84 start-page: 1463 year: 2012 end-page: 1467 article-title: Canyon wrens alter their songs in response to territorial challenges publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 25 start-page: 435 year: 2014 end-page: 442 article-title: Complexity and behavioral ecology publication-title: Behav. Ecol. – volume: 62 start-page: 825 year: 2001 end-page: 837 article-title: Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long‐range communication publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 107 start-page: 521 year: 2001 end-page: 531 article-title: The dawn song of the blue tit Parus caeruleus and its role in sexual selection publication-title: Ethology – volume: 115 start-page: 677 year: 2013 end-page: 686 article-title: Fidelity of song imitation and stability of dialect songs in brown‐headed cowbirds publication-title: Condor – volume: 205 start-page: 3747 year: 2002 end-page: 3755 article-title: Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin‐ and structurally based ornamental plumage publication-title: J. Exp. Biol. – volume: 53 start-page: 687 year: 1997 end-page: 700 article-title: Song correlates with social context, testosterone and body condition in male barn swallows publication-title: Anim. Behav. – volume: 195 start-page: 1002 year: 1977 end-page: 1004 article-title: Species identification in the brown‐headed cowbird: appropriate responses to abnormal song publication-title: Science – ident: e_1_2_6_43_1 doi: 10.1007/s00265-002-0551-6 – ident: e_1_2_6_57_1 doi: 10.1037/h0077657 – ident: e_1_2_6_61_1 doi: 10.1163/156853902320262781 – ident: e_1_2_6_24_1 doi: 10.1126/science.841321 – ident: e_1_2_6_35_1 doi: 10.1086/283219 – ident: e_1_2_6_16_1 doi: 10.1676/0043-5643(2000)112[0035:GCOBFA]2.0.CO;2 – ident: e_1_2_6_41_1 doi: 10.1007/BF00165834 – ident: e_1_2_6_30_1 doi: 10.1126/science.157.3790.769 – ident: e_1_2_6_38_1 doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1860 – volume-title: Nature's Music: The Science of Bird Song year: 2004 ident: e_1_2_6_31_1 contributor: fullname: Marler P. – ident: e_1_2_6_2_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.016 – ident: e_1_2_6_11_1 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511754791 – ident: e_1_2_6_14_1 doi: 10.1111/eth.12341 – ident: e_1_2_6_22_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_44_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.001 – volume-title: Bird Song: Acoustics and Physiology year: 1968 ident: e_1_2_6_21_1 contributor: fullname: Greenewalt C. H. – ident: e_1_2_6_51_1 doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-6784-4_3 – ident: e_1_2_6_3_1 doi: 10.1002/neu.480250910 – ident: e_1_2_6_23_1 doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2004.03.015 – ident: e_1_2_6_46_1 doi: 10.1525/cond.2013.120073 – ident: e_1_2_6_45_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036130 – ident: e_1_2_6_5_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.016 – ident: e_1_2_6_36_1 doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-092416-8.50015-2 – ident: e_1_2_6_59_1 doi: 10.1037/h0077577 – ident: e_1_2_6_6_1 doi: 10.1080/09524622.2000.9753447 – ident: e_1_2_6_66_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_4_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01518.x – volume: 63 start-page: 37 year: 1983 ident: e_1_2_6_25_1 article-title: Dissecting cowbird song potency‐assessing a songs geographic identity and relative appeal publication-title: J. Comp. Ethol. contributor: fullname: King A. P. – ident: e_1_2_6_56_1 doi: 10.2307/4089197 – ident: e_1_2_6_18_1 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0580 – ident: e_1_2_6_26_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.035 – ident: e_1_2_6_49_1 doi: 10.1086/283568 – ident: e_1_2_6_28_1 doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2167 – ident: e_1_2_6_50_1 doi: 10.1007/s10682-011-9541-3 – ident: e_1_2_6_33_1 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511610363 – ident: e_1_2_6_7_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.017 – ident: e_1_2_6_29_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.008 – ident: e_1_2_6_20_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.005 – ident: e_1_2_6_12_1 doi: 10.1126/science.1091099 – ident: e_1_2_6_63_1 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0223 – ident: e_1_2_6_37_1 doi: 10.3838/jjo.49.69 – ident: e_1_2_6_65_1 doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-092416-8.50014-0 – ident: e_1_2_6_58_1 doi: 10.1038/334244a0 – ident: e_1_2_6_17_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.4.357 – ident: e_1_2_6_47_1 doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00680.x – ident: e_1_2_6_52_1 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80251-3 – ident: e_1_2_6_10_1 doi: 10.1007/BF00612017 – ident: e_1_2_6_19_1 doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0304 – ident: e_1_2_6_32_1 doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747 – ident: e_1_2_6_34_1 doi: 10.1086/286114 – ident: e_1_2_6_64_1 doi: 10.1007/BF00300047 – ident: e_1_2_6_55_1 doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1416 – ident: e_1_2_6_13_1 doi: 10.1080/09524622.1998.9753385 – ident: e_1_2_6_60_1 doi: 10.1075/is.12.2.08wes – ident: e_1_2_6_39_1 doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1120-z – ident: e_1_2_6_62_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.014 – ident: e_1_2_6_54_1 doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.09.003 – ident: e_1_2_6_40_1 doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1190 – ident: e_1_2_6_48_1 doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003090 – ident: e_1_2_6_8_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_42_1 doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1951 – ident: e_1_2_6_9_1 doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru014 – ident: e_1_2_6_53_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.08.011 – ident: e_1_2_6_15_1 doi: 10.1007/BF00303842 – ident: e_1_2_6_27_1 doi: 10.1037/a0029681 |
SSID | ssj0017373 |
Score | 2.1962948 |
Snippet | Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long‐distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females.... Abstract Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long‐distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females.... Bird song is typically depicted as a male singing a long-distance signal to potentially unknown receivers to (1) deter males and (2) attract females.... |
SourceID | proquest crossref wiley istex fao |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Publisher |
StartPage | 1104 |
SubjectTerms | aggression Animal behavior Animal communication bird song Birds courtship females flocks Males Molothrus ater Motivational Structural Rules songbirds within-song variation |
Title | Watch Your Tone: Social Conditions Modulate Singing Strategies |
URI | https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-JSFJ3RDL-4/fulltext.pdf https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Feth.12425 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1722098108 |
Volume | 121 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rS8MwED-mIOyLb3G-CCLil47m0XZTEEQ3x3B-cBsTEULSpgqDVfYA9a_30q7FCYL4rZQmXHN3v_yS3F0ATvC1UmHsOqFvPEeYUDjI8o0T1XD649oIP7Ib-p17v9UX7UfvsQQXeS5MVh-i2HCznpHitXVwpSffnNxMX6vUEmbEX8oDG85181CUjqIBz06X0eAcZDV8XlXIRvEULRfmoqVYJchQ7eC-L9DN76Q1nXWaa_Ccy5sFmwyrs6muhp8_Sjn-84fWYXXORslVZj4bUDKjTSgXoPixCStPSfq0BZcDRO1XgvAwJr1kZM5JltpLrhN77G3Nl3SSyF4HZkjX3oM0eiF58Vsz2YZ-s9G7bjnz2xec0CbrOp5iMbKRmIeceUoHTCCXC8JIIYXkSsXMDzRjdap0RKmmXLsesgG_HooaEzFXfAeWRyjNLpDIM74ba9cYlwsj0G6QhASqjmCLy0dFK3Cc60G-ZUU2ZL44wVGR6ahUYBc1JNULgp_sd5ndqrH1D7GTCpymaisaq_HQBqwFnhzc38p2t9nmDzd3UlTgINernPvqRCKFY269Rl3s5yxV0O9CyEavlT7s_f3TfShbYbMExgNYno5n5hCZzFQfpSb7BUF46f4 |
link.rule.ids | 315,783,787,1378,27936,27937,46306,46730 |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rSyMxEB98cFy_-LoT6zOIHPdly26S3W1FBFFrrW0_2BZFkJDsZhWErtQK6l_vzG530QPh8FsImzCbeeSXSWYGYA-7tY4S14kC6zvSRtJBlG-duI7bnzBWBjE59Lu9oDWU7Wv_egYOiliYPD9E6XAjzcjsNSk4OaQ_aLmd3Nc8QsyzMI_qLqhww8llmTzKC0V-v4wi5yCuEdO8QvSOpxz6aTeaTXSKGJWW9-UT4PwIW7N9p7kItwXF-XOTh9rzxNSit3-SOX73l5ZgYQpI2VEuQcswY0crUCnt4usK_LhJs9YvOLxCw33P0EKM2SAd2X2WR_ey45RuvkmCWTeNqSKYZX0qhTS6Y0X-W_v0G4bN08Fxy5kWYHAiitd1fM0TBCSJiAT3tQm5RDgXRrFGFCm0TngQGs4bnjax5xlPGNdHQBA0IlnnMhFarMLcCKlZAxb7NnAT41rrCmklig7ikFA30N7iCVJ7VdgtGKEe8zwbqjif4KqobFWqsIYsUvoO7Z8a9jl5aygFIk5ShT8Z38rBevxAb9ZCX131zlS732yLy5OOklXYLBirpur6pBDFcbdR91yc52_Goa-JUKeDVtZY__9Pd-Bna9DtqM5572IDKkR4Hs-4CXOT8bPdQmAzMduZ_L4D6c_uFg |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnZ1bS8MwFIAPU1F88S6b1yAivnS0SdpuCoI455w6xG0oIoSkTTcYrDInqL_ek3YtKgjiWylNOM255MvtBGAfX0sZRLYVeNq1uA64hZSvrbCC3R9TmnuhmdC_aXmNLm8-uA8FOM7OwqT5IfIJN-MZSbw2Dv4cRl-cXI_7ZccA8xTMcA_J1xDRXZ47yvFZuryMFmch1rBJWiGzjScv-q0zmopkjIhqWvftG29-pdak26kvwlMmcLrbZFB-Haty8PEjl-M__2gJFiY4Sk5T-1mGgh6uwHweFd9XYPYxTp5W4eQew3afYHwYkU481EckPdtLzmKz7m3sl9zEobkPTJO2uQhp2CNZ9lv9sgbd-nnnrGFNrl-wAnNa13IljRBHIhYw6krlU44w5wehRIZkUkbU8xWlVUeq0HGUw5TtIg541YBXKI-YZOswPURpikBCV3t2pGytbcY1R8NBCvFlFaMtjh-lU4K9TA_iOc2yIbLRCbaKSFqlBEXUkJA9jH6i26ZmrsYkQMRKSnCQqC0vLEcDs2PNd8V960I02_Umu6tdC16CrUyvYuKsLwIZjtrVimNjPYeJgn4XQpx3GsnDxt8_3YW521pdXF-2rjZh3sidHmbcgunx6FVvI9WM1U5ivZ8m9-zF |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Watch+Your+Tone%3A+Social+Conditions+Modulate+Singing+Strategies&rft.jtitle=Ethology&rft.au=Ronald%2C+Kelly+L.&rft.au=Skillman%2C+Tasha&rft.au=Lin%2C+Andy&rft.au=Li%2C+Qingling&rft.date=2015-11-01&rft.issn=0179-1613&rft.eissn=1439-0310&rft.volume=121&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=1104&rft.epage=1115&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Feth.12425&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1111_eth_12425 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0179-1613&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0179-1613&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0179-1613&client=summon |