Quantitative and Qualitative Differences in the Canonical and the Reverse Distance Effect and Their Selective Association With Arithmetic and Mathematical Competencies
Understanding the relationship between symbolic numerical abilities and individual differences in mathematical competencies has become a central research endeavor in the last years. Evidence on this foundational relationship is often based on two behavioral signatures of numerical magnitude and nume...
Saved in:
Published in | Frontiers in education (Lausanne) Vol. 6 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Frontiers Media S.A
27.07.2021
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 2504-284X 2504-284X |
DOI | 10.3389/feduc.2021.655747 |
Cover
Abstract | Understanding the relationship between symbolic numerical abilities and individual differences in mathematical competencies has become a central research endeavor in the last years. Evidence on this foundational relationship is often based on two behavioral signatures of numerical magnitude and numerical order processing: the
canonical
and the
reverse distance effect.
The
former
indicates faster reaction times for the comparison of numerals that are far in distance (e.g., 2 8) compared to numerals that are close in distance (e.g., 2 3). The latter indicates faster reaction times for the ordinal judgment of numerals (i.e., are numerals in ascending/descending order) that are close in distance (e.g., 2 3 4) compared to numerals that are far in distance (e.g., 2 4 6). While a substantial body of literature has reported consistent associations between the
canonical distance effect
and arithmetic abilities, rather inconsistent findings have been found for the
reverse distance effect
. Here, we tested the hypothesis that estimates of the
reverse distance effect
show qualitative differences (i.e., not all participants show a
reverse distance effect
in the expected direction) rather than quantitative differences (i.e., all individuals show a
reverse distance effect
, but to a different degree), and that inconsistent findings might be a consequence of this variation. We analyzed data from 397 adults who performed a computerized numerical comparison task, a computerized numerical order verification task (i.e., are three numerals presented in order or not), a paper pencil test of arithmetic fluency, as well as a standardized test to assess more complex forms of mathematical competencies. We found discriminatory evidence for the two distance effects. While estimates of the
canonical distance effect
showed quantitative differences, estimates of the
reverse distance effect
showed qualitative differences
.
Comparisons between individuals who demonstrated an effect and individuals who demonstrated no
reverse distance effect
confirmed a significant moderation on the correlation with mathematical abilities. Significantly larger effects were found in the group who showed an effect. These findings confirm that estimates of the
reverse distance effect
are subject to qualitative differences and that we need to better characterize the underlying mechanisms/strategies that might lead to these qualitative differences. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Understanding the relationship between symbolic numerical abilities and individual differences in mathematical competencies has become a central research endeavor in the last years. Evidence on this foundational relationship is often based on two behavioral signatures of numerical magnitude and numerical order processing: the canonical and the reverse distance effect. The former indicates faster reaction times for the comparison of numerals that are far in distance (e.g., 2 8) compared to numerals that are close in distance (e.g., 2 3). The latter indicates faster reaction times for the ordinal judgment of numerals (i.e., are numerals in ascending/descending order) that are close in distance (e.g., 2 3 4) compared to numerals that are far in distance (e.g., 2 4 6). While a substantial body of literature has reported consistent associations between the canonical distance effect and arithmetic abilities, rather inconsistent findings have been found for the reverse distance effect. Here, we tested the hypothesis that estimates of the reverse distance effect show qualitative differences (i.e., not all participants show a reverse distance effect in the expected direction) rather than quantitative differences (i.e., all individuals show a reverse distance effect, but to a different degree), and that inconsistent findings might be a consequence of this variation. We analyzed data from 397 adults who performed a computerized numerical comparison task, a computerized numerical order verification task (i.e., are three numerals presented in order or not), a paper pencil test of arithmetic fluency, as well as a standardized test to assess more complex forms of mathematical competencies. We found discriminatory evidence for the two distance effects. While estimates of the canonical distance effect showed quantitative differences, estimates of the reverse distance effect showed qualitative differences. Comparisons between individuals who demonstrated an effect and individuals who demonstrated no reverse distance effect confirmed a significant moderation on the correlation with mathematical abilities. Significantly larger effects were found in the group who showed an effect. These findings confirm that estimates of the reverse distance effect are subject to qualitative differences and that we need to better characterize the underlying mechanisms/strategies that might lead to these qualitative differences. Understanding the relationship between symbolic numerical abilities and individual differences in mathematical competencies has become a central research endeavor in the last years. Evidence on this foundational relationship is often based on two behavioral signatures of numerical magnitude and numerical order processing: the canonical and the reverse distance effect. The former indicates faster reaction times for the comparison of numerals that are far in distance (e.g., 2 8) compared to numerals that are close in distance (e.g., 2 3). The latter indicates faster reaction times for the ordinal judgment of numerals (i.e., are numerals in ascending/descending order) that are close in distance (e.g., 2 3 4) compared to numerals that are far in distance (e.g., 2 4 6). While a substantial body of literature has reported consistent associations between the canonical distance effect and arithmetic abilities, rather inconsistent findings have been found for the reverse distance effect . Here, we tested the hypothesis that estimates of the reverse distance effect show qualitative differences (i.e., not all participants show a reverse distance effect in the expected direction) rather than quantitative differences (i.e., all individuals show a reverse distance effect , but to a different degree), and that inconsistent findings might be a consequence of this variation. We analyzed data from 397 adults who performed a computerized numerical comparison task, a computerized numerical order verification task (i.e., are three numerals presented in order or not), a paper pencil test of arithmetic fluency, as well as a standardized test to assess more complex forms of mathematical competencies. We found discriminatory evidence for the two distance effects. While estimates of the canonical distance effect showed quantitative differences, estimates of the reverse distance effect showed qualitative differences . Comparisons between individuals who demonstrated an effect and individuals who demonstrated no reverse distance effect confirmed a significant moderation on the correlation with mathematical abilities. Significantly larger effects were found in the group who showed an effect. These findings confirm that estimates of the reverse distance effect are subject to qualitative differences and that we need to better characterize the underlying mechanisms/strategies that might lead to these qualitative differences. |
Author | Vogel, Stephan E. Grabner, Roland H. Faulkenberry, Thomas J. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Stephan E. surname: Vogel fullname: Vogel, Stephan E. – sequence: 2 givenname: Thomas J. surname: Faulkenberry fullname: Faulkenberry, Thomas J. – sequence: 3 givenname: Roland H. surname: Grabner fullname: Grabner, Roland H. |
BookMark | eNp1UdFO3DAQtBCVSikf0Df_wF3t2Intx9MBLRIIQangzdo4G84oFyPbIPFF_U2cHEgIqS-2d3ZmduX5RvbHMCIhPzhbCqHNzx67J7esWMWXTV0rqfbIQVUzuai0vNv_8P5KjlJ6YIxVTDeqMQfk39UTjNlnyP4ZKYwdLcDwXh_7vseIo8NE_UjzBukaynDvYJjJE3KNzxjTRE4ZCpWeFJHLc_9mgz7SPzgUYDJcpRScL-ZhpLc-b-gqlnOL2buZfwHFcVv604B12D5iLtM9pu_kSw9DwqO3-5D8PT25Wf9enF_-OluvzhdO1DIveCU7qV3rnALTSMPRKNaggIYJw4XquWl53fedQCUUdMYxJ1FxpRV3LdTikJztfLsAD_Yx-i3EFxvA2xkI8d5CLOsNaDXXFRqpRIcgtdatqRlK1RrRtsi1Ll5q5-ViSClib938sWHMEfxgObNTfHaOz07x2V18Rck_Kd83-b_mFbCkpBk |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_5964_jnc_9935 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13481 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_025_02090_8 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2022_105478 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218231220912 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0301228 crossref_primary_10_5964_jnc_10201 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101266 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2022_2136186 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101262 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2024_102473 |
Cites_doi | 10.4324/9780203774441 10.3758/PBR.15.2.419 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.002 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.299 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1775-13.2013 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.013 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.019 10.5964/jnc.v3i2.55 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.008 10.1111/desc.12372 10.1007/BF02289823 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.12.001 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.018 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.014 10.1111/mbe.12094 10.1037/met0000156 10.1038/2151519a0 10.1111/desc.12653 10.3389/neuro.11.010.2008 10.1037/0096-3445.125.3.284 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010 10.4324/9780203998045-30 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90051-I 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.019 10.3758/s13423-020-01814-8 10.3758/MC.37.5.644 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.02.004 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103014 10.1080/20445911.2017.1282490 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.013 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.006 10.1016/S1099-4831(10)06803-310.1016/bs.pbr.2016.04.010 10.5964/jnc.v6i2.288 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.013 10.3758/s13423-018-1440-y 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00142-1 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107405 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001 10.3758/s13423-018-1522-x 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.009 10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.07.010 10.1111/cogs.12711 10.31234/osf.io/4b9rs 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.002 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION DOA |
DOI | 10.3389/feduc.2021.655747 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Education |
EISSN | 2504-284X |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_8182e9473dea4888b950e47b93bbe188 10_3389_feduc_2021_655747 |
GroupedDBID | 9T4 AAFWJ AAYXX ACXDI ADBBV AFPKN ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BCNDV CITATION GROUPED_DOAJ M~E OK1 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c354t-124d48cbcc7a96491e9706e3a6039137f19b15ffd3e737ad9c0c4e717871cba53 |
IEDL.DBID | DOA |
ISSN | 2504-284X |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:26:56 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:17:31 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:56:09 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Language | English |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c354t-124d48cbcc7a96491e9706e3a6039137f19b15ffd3e737ad9c0c4e717871cba53 |
OpenAccessLink | https://doaj.org/article/8182e9473dea4888b950e47b93bbe188 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_8182e9473dea4888b950e47b93bbe188 crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_feduc_2021_655747 crossref_primary_10_3389_feduc_2021_655747 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2021-07-27 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-07-27 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 07 year: 2021 text: 2021-07-27 day: 27 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationTitle | Frontiers in education (Lausanne) |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
Publisher | Frontiers Media S.A |
Publisher_xml | – name: Frontiers Media S.A |
References | LeFevre (B29) 1996; 125 Sasanguie (B46) 2018; 21 Vos (B57) 2017; 29 Haaf (B24) 2019; 26 Woodcock (B58) 2001 Price (B41) 2007; 17 Benjamini (B4) 1995; 57 Schnuerch (B49) 2020; 28 Nuerk (B37) 2001; 82 Lyons (B34) 2016; 227 Lyons (B31) 2015; 9 Sasanguie (B45) 2012; 30 Brannon (B5) 2006; 16 Ashkenazi (B2) 2008; 44 Sella (B50) 2020; 204 Baroody (B3) 2018; 25 De Smedt (B10) 2009; 103 Sommerauer (B51) 2020; 141 Cohen (B8) 2013 Rousselle (B44) 2007; 102 Orrantia (B38) 2019; 43 Van Opstal (B53) 2008; 15 Haaf (B23) 2017; 22 B42 Vogel (B56) 2015; 129 Campbell (B7) 2001; 130 Gross (B22) 2009 B43 Lyons (B32) 2011; 121 Campbell (B6) 2005 Lyons (B33) 2013; 33 Peirce (B40) 2008; 2 Schillinger (B47) 2018; 61 French (B17) 1963 Dehaene (B11) 2015; 88 Franklin (B16) 2009; 37 Grabner (B20) 2011; 87 Greenhouse (B21) 1959; 24 Jeffreys (B27) 1968 Holloway (B25) 2009; 103 Faulkenberry (B13) 2020 Lonnemann (B30) 2011; 24 Ashcraft (B1) 1992; 44 Delazer (B12) 2006; 44 Jasper (B26) 2011 Faulkenberry (B14) 2020; 6 Moyer (B36) 1967; 215 Field (B15) 2012 Turconi (B52) 2006; 98 Goffin (B18) 2016; 150 Maloney (B35) 2010; 134 Vogel (B54) 2017; 3 Vogel (B55) 2019; 193 Zorzi (B59) 1999 Kass (B28) 1995; 90 Grabner (B19) 2009; 47 De Smedt (B9) 2013; 2 Parsons (B39) 2005 Schneider (B48) 2017; 20 |
References_xml | – start-page: 536 volume-title: Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences year: 2013 ident: B8 doi: 10.4324/9780203774441 – volume: 15 start-page: 419 year: 2008 ident: B53 article-title: Dissecting the Symbolic Distance Effect: Comparison and Priming Effects in Numerical and Nonnumerical Orders publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.2.419 – volume: 16 start-page: 222 year: 2006 ident: B5 article-title: The Representation of Numerical Magnitude publication-title: Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.002 – volume: 130 start-page: 299 year: 2001 ident: B7 article-title: Cognitive Arithmetic across Cultures publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. GeneralGeneral doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.299 – volume: 33 start-page: 17052 year: 2013 ident: B33 article-title: Ordinality and the Nature of Symbolic Numbers publication-title: J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1775-13.2013 – volume: 57 start-page: 289 year: 1995 ident: B4 article-title: Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing publication-title: J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodological) doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x – volume: 47 start-page: 604 year: 2009 ident: B19 article-title: To Retrieve or to Calculate? Left Angular Gyrus Mediates the Retrieval of Arithmetic Facts during Problem Solving publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.013 – volume: 87 start-page: 128 year: 2011 ident: B20 article-title: Neurophysiological Evidence for the Validity of Verbal Strategy Reports in Mental Arithmetic publication-title: Biol. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.019 – volume: 3 start-page: 288 year: 2017 ident: B54 article-title: Processing the Order of Symbolic Numbers: A Reliable and Unique Predictor of Arithmetic Fluency publication-title: J. Numer. Cogn. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v3i2.55 – volume-title: Woodcock-Johnson III Test of Achievement year: 2001 ident: B58 – volume: 44 start-page: 439 year: 2008 ident: B2 article-title: Basic Numerical Processing in Left Intraparietal Sulcus (IPS) Acalculia publication-title: Cortex doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.008 – volume: 20 start-page: e12372 year: 2017 ident: B48 article-title: Associations of Non-symbolic and Symbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing with Mathematical Competence: A Meta-Analysis publication-title: Dev. Sci. doi: 10.1111/desc.12372 – volume-title: Twenty First Annual Meeting of the year: 1999 ident: B59 article-title: A Computational Model of Number Comparison – volume: 24 start-page: 95 year: 1959 ident: B21 article-title: On Methods in the Analysis of Profile Data publication-title: Psychometrika doi: 10.1007/BF02289823 – volume: 30 start-page: 344 year: 2012 ident: B45 article-title: Association between Basic Numerical Abilities and Mathematics Achievement publication-title: Br. J. Dev. Psychol. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x – volume: 193 start-page: 30 year: 2019 ident: B55 article-title: Automatic and Intentional Processing of Numerical Order and its Relationship to Arithmetic Performance publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.12.001 – volume-title: Kit for Reference Tests for Cognitive Factors year: 1963 ident: B17 – volume: 150 start-page: 68 year: 2016 ident: B18 article-title: Beyond Magnitude: Judging Ordinality of Symbolic Number Is Unrelated to Magnitude Comparison and Independently Relates to Individual Differences in Arithmetic publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.018 – volume: 61 start-page: 109 year: 2018 ident: B47 article-title: Math Anxiety, Intelligence, and Performance in Mathematics: Insights from the German Adaptation of the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS-G) publication-title: Learn. Individual Differences doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.014 – volume: 9 start-page: 207 year: 2015 ident: B31 article-title: Numerical Order Processing in Children: From Reversing the Distance-Effect to Predicting Arithmetic publication-title: Mind, Brain Edu. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12094 – volume: 22 start-page: 779 year: 2017 ident: B23 article-title: Developing Constraint in Bayesian Mixed Models publication-title: Psychol. Methods doi: 10.1037/met0000156 – volume: 215 start-page: 1519 year: 1967 ident: B36 article-title: Time Required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/2151519a0 – volume: 21 start-page: e12653 year: 2018 ident: B46 article-title: About Why There Is a Shift from Cardinal to Ordinal Processing in the Association with Arithmetic between First and Second Grade publication-title: Dev. Sci. doi: 10.1111/desc.12653 – volume: 2 start-page: 10 year: 2008 ident: B40 article-title: Generating Stimuli for Neuroscience Using PsychoPy publication-title: Front. Neuroinform. doi: 10.3389/neuro.11.010.2008 – volume: 125 start-page: 284 year: 1996 ident: B29 article-title: Multiple Routes to Solution of Single-Digit Multiplication Problems publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.125.3.284 – volume: 103 start-page: 469 year: 2009 ident: B10 article-title: The Predictive Value of Numerical Magnitude Comparison for Individual Differences in Mathematics Achievement publication-title: J. Exp. child Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010 – start-page: 347 volume-title: Handbook of Mathematical Cognition year: 2005 ident: B6 article-title: Architectures for Arithmetic doi: 10.4324/9780203998045-30 – volume: 102 start-page: 361 year: 2007 ident: B44 article-title: Basic Numerical Skills in Children with Mathematics Learning Disabilities: A Comparison of Symbolic vs Non-symbolic Number Magnitude Processing publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005 – volume: 44 start-page: 75 year: 1992 ident: B1 article-title: Cognitive Arithmetic: A Review of Data and Theory publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90051-I – volume: 88 start-page: 2 year: 2015 ident: B11 article-title: The Neural Representation of Sequences: From Transition Probabilities to Algebraic Patterns and Linguistic Trees publication-title: Neuron doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.019 – volume: 28 start-page: 750 year: 2020 ident: B49 article-title: The Truth Revisited: Bayesian Analysis of Individual Differences in the Truth Effect publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01814-8 – volume: 37 start-page: 644 year: 2009 ident: B16 article-title: Processing of Order Information for Numbers and Months publication-title: Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.5.644 – ident: B43 – volume-title: Mathematiktest für die Personalauswahl [Mathematics test for selection of personnel] year: 2011 ident: B26 – volume: 90 start-page: 773 year: 1995 ident: B28 article-title: Bayes Factors publication-title: J. Am. Stat. Assoc. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572 – volume: 24 start-page: 583 year: 2011 ident: B30 article-title: Symbolic and Non-symbolic Distance Effects in Children and Their Connection with Arithmetic Skills publication-title: J. Neurolinguist. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.02.004 – volume: 204 start-page: 103014 year: 2020 ident: B50 article-title: Judging the Order of Numbers Relies on Familiarity rather Than Activating the Mental Number Line publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103014 – volume: 29 start-page: 469 year: 2017 ident: B57 article-title: The Role of General and Number-specific Order Processing in Adults' Arithmetic Performance publication-title: J. Cogn. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2017.1282490 – volume: 17 start-page: R1042 year: 2007 ident: B41 article-title: Impaired Parietal Magnitude Processing in Developmental Dyscalculia publication-title: Curr. Biol. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.013 – volume-title: The Longterm Costs of Numeracy Difficulties year: 2009 ident: B22 – volume: 134 start-page: 154 year: 2010 ident: B35 article-title: Challenging the Reliability and Validity of Cognitive Measures: The Case of the Numerical Distance Effect publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.006 – volume: 227 start-page: 187 year: 2016 ident: B34 article-title: On the Ordinality of Numbers publication-title: Prog. Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/S1099-4831(10)06803-310.1016/bs.pbr.2016.04.010 – volume: 6 start-page: 231 year: 2020 ident: B14 article-title: Bayesian Inference in Numerical Cognition: A Tutorial Using JASP publication-title: J. Numer. Cogn. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v6i2.288 – volume: 44 start-page: 36 year: 2006 ident: B12 article-title: Number Processing in Posterior Cortical Atrophy-A Neuropsycholgical Case Study publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.013 – volume-title: Discovering Statistics Using R year: 2012 ident: B15 – volume: 25 start-page: 2398 year: 2018 ident: B3 article-title: A Commentary on Chen and Campbell (2017): Is There a clear Case for Addition Fact Recall? publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1440-y – volume: 82 start-page: B25 year: 2001 ident: B37 article-title: Decade Breaks in the Mental Number Line? Putting the Tens and Units Back in Different Bins publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00142-1 – volume: 141 start-page: 107405 year: 2020 ident: B51 article-title: The Semantic Control Network Mediates the Relationship between Symbolic Numerical Order Processing and Arithmetic Performance in Children publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107405 – volume: 103 start-page: 17 year: 2009 ident: B25 article-title: Mapping Numerical Magnitudes onto Symbols: The Numerical Distance Effect and Individual Differences in Children's Mathematics Achievement publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001 – ident: B42 – volume: 26 start-page: 772 year: 2019 ident: B24 article-title: Some Do and Some Don't? Accounting for Variability of Individual Difference Structures publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1522-x – volume: 121 start-page: 256 year: 2011 ident: B32 article-title: Numerical Ordering Ability Mediates the Relation between Number-Sense and Arithmetic Competence publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.009 – volume: 2 start-page: 48 year: 2013 ident: B9 article-title: How Do Symbolic and Non-symbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing Skills Relate to Individual Differences in Children's Mathematical Skills? A Review of Evidence from Brain and Behavior publication-title: Trends Neurosci. Edu. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001 – volume-title: The Theory of Probability year: 1968 ident: B27 – volume: 129 start-page: 26 year: 2015 ident: B56 article-title: Differential Processing of Symbolic Numerical Magnitude and Order in First-Grade Children publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.07.010 – volume: 43 year: 2019 ident: B38 article-title: Disentangling the Mechanisms of Symbolic Number Processing in Adults' Mathematics and Arithmetic Achievement publication-title: Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12711 – volume-title: Modeling the Latent Structure of Individual Differences in the Numerical Size-Congruity Effect year: 2020 ident: B13 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/4b9rs – volume: 98 start-page: 273 year: 2006 ident: B52 article-title: Numerical Order and Quantity Processing in Number Comparison publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.002 – volume-title: Does Numeracy Matter More year: 2005 ident: B39 |
SSID | ssj0002086769 |
Score | 2.231827 |
Snippet | Understanding the relationship between symbolic numerical abilities and individual differences in mathematical competencies has become a central research... |
SourceID | doaj crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Enrichment Source Index Database |
SubjectTerms | arithmetic abilities canonical distance effect individual differences mathematical competencies reverse distance effect |
Title | Quantitative and Qualitative Differences in the Canonical and the Reverse Distance Effect and Their Selective Association With Arithmetic and Mathematical Competencies |
URI | https://doaj.org/article/8182e9473dea4888b950e47b93bbe188 |
Volume | 6 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LT4QwEG6MJy_GZ3ynB08mKNCW0qOuGmOyJuoavZG2DFGjaAz7m_ybzrTshpNevJBQhlKYgc5HZ75h7NDLUhSOItVSBYksmyKxkJtENcJkdWZ9KSjBeXxTXD3I6yf1NCj1RTFhkR44PrgTnFByMFKLGiwaW-mMSkFqZ4RzkJUhzTc16QBMvYbltZJiN-MyJqIwc9IQEyriwTw7LpTSVE5lMBEN-PrDxHK5wpZ7j5CfxpGssgVo16iYch94sc6-b6e2Dblg-GXiiPx5JL6I--d9hRN83_lLy9Gf4yPbfoR0xyBMLXdA0RckTM6gBx5Ji8PxCS0V8PtQD4c6HCiMP750zzgy3L5TsmOQH8-JXvECo5nbjYB7gz1cXkxGV0lfXyHxQskuwam9lqV33mtrCmkyMDotQNiCaOOFbjLjMtU0tQAttK2NT70ExH8IsryzSmyyRbwf2GJcF1CDU9BYQMCphVUZAO5K6VMJSmyzdPawK9-Tj1MNjLcKQQjppwr6qUg_VdTPNjuan_IZmTd-Ez4jDc4FiTQ7NKApVb0pVX-Z0s5_dLLLlmhc9Ps313tssfuawj76LZ07CCb6Ay1J7bU |
linkProvider | Directory of Open Access Journals |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Quantitative+and+Qualitative+Differences+in+the+Canonical+and+the+Reverse+Distance+Effect+and+Their+Selective+Association+With+Arithmetic+and+Mathematical+Competencies&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+education+%28Lausanne%29&rft.au=Stephan+E.+Vogel&rft.au=Thomas+J.+Faulkenberry&rft.au=Roland+H.+Grabner&rft.date=2021-07-27&rft.pub=Frontiers+Media+S.A&rft.eissn=2504-284X&rft.volume=6&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffeduc.2021.655747&rft.externalDBID=DOA&rft.externalDocID=oai_doaj_org_article_8182e9473dea4888b950e47b93bbe188 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2504-284X&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2504-284X&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2504-284X&client=summon |