Does better explicit knowledge of a morphosyntactic structure guarantee more native-like electrophysiological processing? An ERP study with French learners of English
The possible transformation of the explicit knowledge developed during classroom learning of a second language into implicit knowledge of that language remains an open issue. In this study, we investigated the relationship between the explicit and implicit processing of morphosyntactic violations of...
Saved in:
Published in | SHS web of conferences Vol. 38; p. 2 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article Conference Proceeding |
Language | English |
Published |
Les Ulis
EDP Sciences
01.01.2017
|
Series | Connaissances et Usages en L2 / Knowledge and Usage in L2 |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 2261-2424 2416-5182 2261-2424 |
DOI | 10.1051/shsconf/20173800002 |
Cover
Abstract | The possible transformation of the explicit knowledge developed during classroom learning of a second language into implicit knowledge of that language remains an open issue. In this study, we investigated the relationship between the explicit and implicit processing of morphosyntactic violations of English as an L2. ERP responses were obtained from 24 French learners (12 Intermediate and 12 Advanced) and 12 Native controls (NS) while participants evaluated the grammaticality of orally presented sentences containing subject-verb agreement violations. Results show that NS and Advanced speakers outperformed the Intermediate ones on the behavioural task. A P600 effect was obtained for all groups. Additionally, NS and Advanced learners exhibited an early negativity after violations while there was no significant effect in Intermediate speakers. The presence and amplitude of this early negativity was correlated with the structure-specific proficiency of Intermediate speakers and with the time of instruction of all learners. Results suggest that the superior native-likeness of the early responses obtained in Advanced learners is due more to their better proficiency and superior degree of explicit instruction than to the direct opportunity for implicit knowledge that their stay abroad represented. La possibilité de transformer les connaissances explicites développées en classe durant l’apprentissage d’une langue seconde en connaissances implicites reste une question ouverte. Dans cette étude, nous nous sommes intéressés au lien entre le traitement explicite et implicite de violations morphosyntaxiques en anglais L2. Les réponses en potentiels évoqués à des violations de l’accord sujet-verbe ont été recueillies chez 24 apprenants francophones (12 intermédiaires et 12 avancés) et 12 sujets contrôle (LN) complétant un jugement de grammaticalité. Les résultats montrent que les LN et les apprenants avancés ont de meilleures performances que les apprenants intermédiaires dans la tâche comportementale. Un effet P600 est attesté dans tous les groupes. De plus, les LN et les apprenants avancés présentent une négativité précoce après les violations d’accord, alors qu’il n’y a pas d’effet significatif pour les apprenants intermédiaires. La présence et l’amplitude de cette négativité est corrélée aux connaissances structurales pour les locuteurs intermédiaires et à la durée d’enseignement pour tous les locuteurs. Les résultats suggèrent que le caractère quasi-natif des réponses des apprenants avancés est davantage lié à de meilleures connaissances et un degré supérieur d’enseignement explicite qu’à des connaissances implicites résultant de leur séjour à l’étranger. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The possible transformation of the explicit knowledge developed during classroom learning of a second language into implicit knowledge of that language remains an open issue. In this study, we investigated the relationship between the explicit and implicit processing of morphosyntactic violations of English as an L2. ERP responses were obtained from 24 French learners (12 Intermediate and 12 Advanced) and 12 Native controls (NS) while participants evaluated the grammaticality of orally presented sentences containing subject-verb agreement violations. Results show that NS and Advanced speakers outperformed the Intermediate ones on the behavioural task. A P600 effect was obtained for all groups. Additionally, NS and Advanced learners exhibited an early negativity after violations while there was no significant effect in Intermediate speakers. The presence and amplitude of this early negativity was correlated with the structure-specific proficiency of Intermediate speakers and with the time of instruction of all learners. Results suggest that the superior native-likeness of the early responses obtained in Advanced learners is due more to their better proficiency and superior degree of explicit instruction than to the direct opportunity for implicit knowledge that their stay abroad represented.
La possibilité de transformer les connaissances explicites développées en classe durant l’apprentissage d’une langue seconde en connaissances implicites reste une question ouverte. Dans cette étude, nous nous sommes intéressés au lien entre le traitement explicite et implicite de violations morphosyntaxiques en anglais L2. Les réponses en potentiels évoqués à des violations de l’accord sujet-verbe ont été recueillies chez 24 apprenants francophones (12 intermédiaires et 12 avancés) et 12 sujets contrôle (LN) complétant un jugement de grammaticalité. Les résultats montrent que les LN et les apprenants avancés ont de meilleures performances que les apprenants intermédiaires dans la tâche comportementale. Un effet P600 est attesté dans tous les groupes. De plus, les LN et les apprenants avancés présentent une négativité précoce après les violations d’accord, alors qu’il n’y a pas d’effet significatif pour les apprenants intermédiaires. La présence et l’amplitude de cette négativité est corrélée aux connaissances structurales pour les locuteurs intermédiaires et à la durée d’enseignement pour tous les locuteurs. Les résultats suggèrent que le caractère quasi-natif des réponses des apprenants avancés est davantage lié à de meilleures connaissances et un degré supérieur d’enseignement explicite qu’à des connaissances implicites résultant de leur séjour à l’étranger. The possible transformation of the explicit knowledge developed during classroom learning of a second language into implicit knowledge of that language remains an open issue. In this study, we investigated the relationship between the explicit and implicit processing of morphosyntactic violations of English as an L2. ERP responses were obtained from 24 French learners (12 Intermediate and 12 Advanced) and 12 Native controls (NS) while participants evaluated the grammaticality of orally presented sentences containing subject-verb agreement violations. Results show that NS and Advanced speakers outperformed the Intermediate ones on the behavioural task. A P600 effect was obtained for all groups. Additionally, NS and Advanced learners exhibited an early negativity after violations while there was no significant effect in Intermediate speakers. The presence and amplitude of this early negativity was correlated with the structure-specific proficiency of Intermediate speakers and with the time of instruction of all learners. Results suggest that the superior native-likeness of the early responses obtained in Advanced learners is due more to their better proficiency and superior degree of explicit instruction than to the direct opportunity for implicit knowledge that their stay abroad represented. La possibilité de transformer les connaissances explicites développées en classe durant l’apprentissage d’une langue seconde en connaissances implicites reste une question ouverte. Dans cette étude, nous nous sommes intéressés au lien entre le traitement explicite et implicite de violations morphosyntaxiques en anglais L2. Les réponses en potentiels évoqués à des violations de l’accord sujet-verbe ont été recueillies chez 24 apprenants francophones (12 intermédiaires et 12 avancés) et 12 sujets contrôle (LN) complétant un jugement de grammaticalité. Les résultats montrent que les LN et les apprenants avancés ont de meilleures performances que les apprenants intermédiaires dans la tâche comportementale. Un effet P600 est attesté dans tous les groupes. De plus, les LN et les apprenants avancés présentent une négativité précoce après les violations d’accord, alors qu’il n’y a pas d’effet significatif pour les apprenants intermédiaires. La présence et l’amplitude de cette négativité est corrélée aux connaissances structurales pour les locuteurs intermédiaires et à la durée d’enseignement pour tous les locuteurs. Les résultats suggèrent que le caractère quasi-natif des réponses des apprenants avancés est davantage lié à de meilleures connaissances et un degré supérieur d’enseignement explicite qu’à des connaissances implicites résultant de leur séjour à l’étranger. The possible transformation of the explicit knowledge developed during classroom learning of a second language into implicit knowledge of that language remains an open issue. In this study, we investigated the relationship between the explicit and implicit processing of morphosyntactic violations of English as an L2. ERP responses were obtained from 24 French learners (12 Intermediate and 12 Advanced) and 12 Native controls (NS) while participants evaluated the grammaticality of orally presented sentences containing subject-verb agreement violations. Results show that NS and Advanced speakers outperformed the Intermediate ones on the behavioural task. A P600 effect was obtained for all groups. Additionally, NS and Advanced learners exhibited an early negativity after violations while there was no significant effect in Intermediate speakers. The presence and amplitude of this early negativity was correlated with the structure-specific proficiency of Intermediate speakers and with the time of instruction of all learners. Results suggest that the superior native-likeness of the early responses obtained in Advanced learners is due more to their better proficiency and superior degree of explicit instruction than to the direct opportunity for implicit knowledge that their stay abroad represented. |
Author | Pélissier, Maud Ferragne, Emmanuel Krzonowski, Jennifer |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Maud surname: Pélissier fullname: Pélissier, Maud – sequence: 2 givenname: Jennifer surname: Krzonowski fullname: Krzonowski, Jennifer – sequence: 3 givenname: Emmanuel surname: Ferragne fullname: Ferragne, Emmanuel |
BackLink | https://hal.science/hal-01370543$$DView record in HAL |
BookMark | eNp9kstuFDEQRVsoSISQL2BjiRWLIX70c4VGYUIijQRCsLaq7epuTxy7sd0J80N8Jx4mQoEF3tgq3XtKrrovixPnHRbFa0bfMVqxizhF5d1wwSlrREvz4c-KU85rtuIlL0-evF8U5zHusoJx1gkqToufHzxG0mNKGAj-mK1RJpFb5x8s6hGJHwiQOx_myce9S6CSUSSmsKi0BCTjAgFcQjxokDhI5h5X1twiQYsqBT9P-2i89aNRYMkcvMIYjRvfk7Ujmy-fM2zRe_Jg0kSuAjo1EYsQHIZ4aL5xozVxelU8H8BGPH-8z4pvV5uvl9er7aePN5fr7UqJiqdVX2tUoLnotaINg150FLBlHaea855D3dKKNvUwqFL3rB862pSqpzB0PaqmFGfFzZGrPezkHMwdhL30YOTvgg-jhJBHYFGyErlWnYZatyXUqtWNZi22JaMcOsUz6-2RNYH9C3W93spDjTLR0KoU9yxr3xy1eUDfF4xJ7vwSXP6q5LRqG06bps0qcVSp4GMMOPzBMioPWZCPWZBPspBd3T-uvOK8KO9SAGP_6_0FwFzBmA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lingua_2022_103243 |
Cites_doi | 10.1177/0267658309337637 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01072 10.36366/frontiers.v10i1.130 10.1002/9781405198431 10.1017/S1366728913000370 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8 10.1017/S136672890700291X 10.1037/h0058700 10.1177/0267658308090186 10.1017/S1366728901000232 10.3758/BF03192770 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067) 10.1038/35094573 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.004 10.1017/S0272263105050102 10.1162/089892999563328 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002 10.1191/0267658305sr256oa 10.1017/S1366728912000302 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 10.1162/0898929055002436 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.231 10.1017/S027226311500008X 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2030 10.1044/jshd.2404.330 10.1080/016909600386084 |
ContentType | Journal Article Conference Proceeding |
Copyright | 2017. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. Attribution |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2017. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: Attribution |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 0-V 3V. 7XB 88J 8FK ABUWG AFKRA ALSLI AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO GNUQQ HEHIP M2R M2S PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PRQQA Q9U 1XC BXJBU IHQJB VOOES DOA |
DOI | 10.1051/shsconf/20173800002 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】 ProQuest Central (Corporate) ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Social Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central ProQuest One ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Central Student Sociology Collection Social Science Database Sociology Database ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) HAL-SHS: Archive ouverte en Sciences de l'Homme et de la Société HAL-SHS: Archive ouverte en Sciences de l'Homme et de la Société (Open Access) Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Central Student ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Sociology Collection ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Sociology Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition Sociology Collection ProQuest Social Science Journals ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Central (Alumni) |
DatabaseTitleList | Publicly Available Content Database |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Social Sciences (General) |
EISSN | 2261-2424 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_14e2dc9da6d84a6c8d7d18e84102a9c2 oai_HAL_hal_01370543v1 10_1051_shsconf_20173800002 |
Genre | Conference Proceeding |
GroupedDBID | 0-V 5VS AAFWJ AAOGA AAYXX ABUWG ABZDU ACACO ACRPL ADBBV ADNMO AFKRA AFPKN AGQPQ ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ARALO ASOEW AZQEC BCNDV BENPR BPHCQ CCPQU CITATION DWQXO EBS EJD GI~ GNUQQ GROUPED_DOAJ HEHIP HMHOC IPNFZ KQ8 M2R M2S M~E OK1 PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC RIG 3V. 7XB 8FK PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQUKI PRINS PRQQA Q9U 1XC BXJBU IHQJB PUEGO VOOES |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c352t-b6decad23bdc071ab390ae81920d22b2a6805076ffc4db1bf9074cb0af9bec743 |
IEDL.DBID | BENPR |
ISSN | 2261-2424 2416-5182 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:31:47 EDT 2025 Thu Sep 04 07:23:32 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 02:28:33 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:10:52 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:17:16 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Language | English |
License | Attribution: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c352t-b6decad23bdc071ab390ae81920d22b2a6805076ffc4db1bf9074cb0af9bec743 |
Notes | ObjectType-Conference Proceeding-1 SourceType-Conference Papers & Proceedings-1 content type line 21 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-9926-8159 0000-0001-6639-9665 0000-0002-3323-6605 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.proquest.com/docview/2058720778?pq-origsite=%requestingapplication% |
PQID | 2058720778 |
PQPubID | 2040545 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_14e2dc9da6d84a6c8d7d18e84102a9c2 hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_01370543v1 proquest_journals_2058720778 crossref_primary_10_1051_shsconf_20173800002 crossref_citationtrail_10_1051_shsconf_20173800002 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2017-01-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2017-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2017 text: 2017-01-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Les Ulis |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Les Ulis |
PublicationSeriesTitle | Connaissances et Usages en L2 / Knowledge and Usage in L2 |
PublicationTitle | SHS web of conferences |
PublicationYear | 2017 |
Publisher | EDP Sciences |
Publisher_xml | – name: EDP Sciences |
References | Tanner (R39) 1954; 61 Segalowitz (R36) 2004; 10 Ojima (R29) 2005; 17 Andringa (R3) 2015; 37 Mueller (R27) 2005; 21 Tokowicz (R41) 2005; 27 Hahne (R19) 2001; 4 R40 R21 R43 R42 Carhart (R7) 1959; 24 Delorme (R11) 2004; 134 R23 R22 R26 R28 Oldfield (R30) 1971; 9 Marian (R25) 2007; 50 Rossi (R34) 2006; 18 R1 R2 Weber-Fox (R46) 1996; 8 R4 R5 R6 Lopez-Calderón (R24) 2014; 8 Ping Li (R31) 2006; 38 R9 Hahne (R18) 1999; 11 Pulvermüller (R32) 2008; 104 Ullman (R44) 2001; 2 R10 Sabourin (R35) 2008; 24 R12 R33 R14 Friederici (R16) 2002; 6 R13 R38 R15 R37 van Hell (R45) 2010; 26 Kaan (R20) 2000; 15 R17 Chen (R8) 2007; 10 |
References_xml | – ident: R13 – volume: 26 start-page: 43 issue: 1 year: 2010 ident: R45 publication-title: Second Language Research doi: 10.1177/0267658309337637 – ident: R26 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01072 – volume: 10 start-page: 1 year: 2004 ident: R36 publication-title: Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad doi: 10.36366/frontiers.v10i1.130 – ident: R21 – ident: R33 doi: 10.1002/9781405198431 – ident: R37 doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000370 – ident: R23 – ident: R28 – volume: 134 start-page: 9 year: 2004 ident: R11 publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 – ident: R40 – volume: 6 start-page: 78 issue: 2 year: 2002 ident: R16 publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8 – ident: R42 – volume: 10 start-page: 161 issue: 2 year: 2007 ident: R8 publication-title: Bilingualism: Language and Cognition doi: 10.1017/S136672890700291X – volume: 61 start-page: 401 issue: 6 year: 1954 ident: R39 publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/h0058700 – volume: 24 start-page: 397 issue: 3 year: 2008 ident: R35 publication-title: Second Language Research doi: 10.1177/0267658308090186 – volume: 4 start-page: 123 issue: 2 year: 2001 ident: R19 publication-title: Bilingualism: Language and Cognition doi: 10.1017/S1366728901000232 – volume: 38 start-page: 202 issue: 2 year: 2006 ident: R31 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/BF03192770 – volume: 50 start-page: 940 issue: 4 year: 2007 ident: R25 publication-title: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067) – volume: 2 start-page: 717 issue: 10 year: 2001 ident: R44 publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/35094573 – volume: 8 start-page: 213 year: 2014 ident: R24 publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 – ident: R14 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.004 – volume: 27 start-page: 173 issue: 2 year: 2005 ident: R41 publication-title: Studies in Second Language Acquisition doi: 10.1017/S0272263105050102 – volume: 11 start-page: 194 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: R18 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/089892999563328 – ident: R1 – ident: R10 – volume: 104 start-page: 244 issue: 3 year: 2008 ident: R32 publication-title: Brain and Language doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002 – volume: 21 start-page: 152 issue: 2 year: 2005 ident: R27 publication-title: Second Language Research doi: 10.1191/0267658305sr256oa – ident: R5 – ident: R9 – ident: R12 – ident: R38 doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000302 – volume: 9 start-page: 97 issue: 1 year: 1971 ident: R30 publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 – ident: R22 – ident: R43 – volume: 17 start-page: 1212 issue: 8 year: 2005 ident: R29 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/0898929055002436 – volume: 8 start-page: 231 year: 1996 ident: R46 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.231 – volume: 37 start-page: 185 year: 2015 ident: R3 publication-title: Studies in Second Language Acquisition doi: 10.1017/S027226311500008X – volume: 18 start-page: 2030 issue: 12 year: 2006 ident: R34 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2030 – volume: 24 start-page: 330 year: 1959 ident: R7 publication-title: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, doi: 10.1044/jshd.2404.330 – ident: R2 – volume: 15 start-page: 159 issue: 2 year: 2000 ident: R20 publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes doi: 10.1080/016909600386084 – ident: R4 – ident: R6 – ident: R15 – ident: R17 |
SSID | ssj0001219303 |
Score | 1.9905248 |
Snippet | The possible transformation of the explicit knowledge developed during classroom learning of a second language into implicit knowledge of that language remains... |
SourceID | doaj hal proquest crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Enrichment Source Index Database |
StartPage | 2 |
SubjectTerms | Cognitive science Explicit knowledge Humanities and Social Sciences Linguistics Violations |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Nb9QwELVQT1wQn2KhRSPEASSiOo7j2Ce0QFcrBAghKvUW-ZNWlKTqLgj-EL-TGce72nKAC1fLSRz7ZeZ5nHnD2BODLDlxKyvRKFXJoFVlbOuqugvID0SSjaWN4rv3anks35y0JzulvuifsEkeeJq4w1pGEbwJVgUtrfI6dKHWUUv0jNb4bH254TubqSm6gsSENxuZobY-XFGax5Bws193jSYzLa64oqzYjw7mlP6H_MMsZ1-zuMluFJII82lwt9i1ONxmsymTFsrXuIKnRTL62R326_WIDS6n5kD8QWfSZ2vYxstgTGDh64hzOq5-DuucGAWTdOy3ywifESc0w5H6RBiyGHh1fvYlQimTkwMgGzsJF1N2AXq9FzAf4OjjB8gytUBRXVhQAuEp5HoUSC7p4SVZ-C47Xhx9erWsSgWGyiMxW1dOhehtEI0LHrmIdY3hNpKGGg9COGGV5kgoVUpeBle7RFtt77hNBrGB5OQe2xvGId5nULchmNTaOkUjE_KeFnHBrQ7cBIW0c8bEZjF6X-TJqUrGeZ-Pydu6LyvY76zgjD3fXnQxqXP8vftLWuVtV5LWzg0IuL4Arv8X4GbsMWLkyj2W87c9tZF8I1Lg5ju-zf4GQn0xCiscSas7wbtOP_gfA3nIrtO7TfGgfbaHoIkHyJDW7lH-GH4D-eIRyg priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals |
Title | Does better explicit knowledge of a morphosyntactic structure guarantee more native-like electrophysiological processing? An ERP study with French learners of English |
URI | https://www.proquest.com/docview/2058720778 https://hal.science/hal-01370543 https://doaj.org/article/14e2dc9da6d84a6c8d7d18e84102a9c2 |
Volume | 38 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELagvXDjUcRCqUaIA0hY5Ok4p6qFXRYE1aqiUm-Rn23VkizNguDCz-F3MuM425ZDLzk4Xm8Sf5n5PM58w9jLGlmyT1TBs1wIXlgpeK1KzdPKIj_IfJErWih-ORDzo-LTcXkcA259_KxytInBUNvOUIwcF-mlrLKkquTu8junqlG0uxpLaNxlm2iCJeJ8c396sDi8FmVBghLKI6OnErxENj1KD5Xp255SP1qPY6dVLsl0ZzfcU1DxR6dzSt9I_meqg_-Z3WdbV5l5sFj7nAfsjmsfssmQYwvxPe3hVRSTfv2I_X3fYYMOSTvgftFu9dkK1pE06Dwo-Nbh0-763-0qpEzBICr749LBCSKInr2jPg7aIBPOL87OHcQCOiE0MlpQWA55B3htu7DXwvRwAUHAFijeCzO6gVMIlSqQdtKfxzTiLXY0m359N-exNgM3SNlWXAvrjLJZrq1BlqJ0XifKkbpaYrNMZ0rIBKmm8N4UVqfa0yLc6ET5GlGDtOUx22i71j1hkJbW1r5UqXd14ZERlYiYREmb1FYgIZ2wbJySxkThcqqfcdGEDfQybeI8NtfmccLerH-0HHQ7bu--T3O97kqi26Ghuzxp4juMqySXWVNbJawslDDSVjaVThZI0lRtcJAXiJQbY8z3PjfURsKOSI7zn3g32yOQmmgu-uYK3E9vP_2M3aOrHmJA22wD4eCeIyta6Z0I_Z0QVcDjh49__gHBxhCU |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9QwELbK9gA3HkUsFBghkEDCInESxzmgqqW72tLtalW1Um_Bie22oiRLd3n0D3HkNzLjJNuWQ2-9er1OrBnPfDPOfMPY6wxRsgt0zEUkJY-NkjzTScHD1CA-EC6ONAWKexM5Oow_HyVHK-xvVwtDn1V2NtEbalOXlCPHID1RqQjSVG3MvnPqGkW3q10LjUYtdu3FLwzZ5h93tlG-b4QYDg4-jXjbVYCXCDYWvJDGltqIqDAl-lddYNSvLfGCBUaIQmipAgRJ0rkyNkVYOAofyyLQLsP9osPFde-w1ZgqWntsdWswme5fyeogIPLtmNEzSp4geu-ojpLww5xKTSqHewnTSJGrENfcoe8agE7uhL7J_M81eH83vM_WLisBYbr0cQ_Yiq0esn5T0wutXZjD25a8-t0j9me7xoHCFwmB_U2346cLWGbuoHag4VuN0q3nF9XCl2hBQ2L749zCMWosydrSHAuVpyXnZ6dfLbQNe3wqprPYMGvqHPDdNmCzgsH-FDxhLlB-GYa0gRPwnTEQ5tLD27LlNXZ4K1J7zHpVXdknDMLEmMwlOnQ2ix0isAQ1NNDKBJmRCID7THQiycuWKJ36dZzl_sI-CfNWjvkVOfbZ--WfZg1PyM3Tt0jWy6lE8u0H6vPjvLUZGJVZYcrMaGlUrGWpTGpCZVWMoFBnJS7yCjXl2hqjzXFOY0QkiWA8-om7We8UKW_N0zy_PExPb_75Jbs7Otgb5-Odye4zdo920OSf1lkPVcM-R0S2KF60xwDYl9s-ef8AAcRNHw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=proceeding&rft.title=SHS+web+of+conferences&rft.atitle=Does+better+explicit+knowledge+of+a+morphosyntactic+structure+guarantee+more+native-like+electrophysiological+processing%3F+An+ERP+study+with+French+learners+of+English&rft.au=P%C3%A9lissier%2C+Maud&rft.au=Krzonowski%2C+Jennifer&rft.au=Ferragne%2C+Emmanuel&rft.date=2017-01-01&rft.pub=EDP+Sciences&rft.issn=2416-5182&rft.eissn=2261-2424&rft.volume=38&rft_id=info:doi/10.1051%2Fshsconf%2F20173800002 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2261-2424&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2261-2424&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2261-2424&client=summon |