Are Faster Participants Always Faster? Assessing Reliability of Participants’ Mean Response Speed in Picture Naming
Studies of language production often make use of picture naming tasks to investigate the cognitive processes involved in speaking, and many of these studies report a wide range of individual variability in how long speakers need to prepare the name of a picture. It has been assumed that this variabi...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of cognition Vol. 7; no. 1; p. 12 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
Ubiquity Press
01.01.2024
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Studies of language production often make use of picture naming tasks to investigate the cognitive processes involved in speaking, and many of these studies report a wide range of individual variability in how long speakers need to prepare the name of a picture. It has been assumed that this variability can be linked to inter-individual differences in cognitive skills or abilities (e.g., attention or working memory); therefore, several studies have tried to explain variability in language production tasks by correlating production measures with scores on cognitive tests. This approach, however, relies on the assumption that participants are reliable over time in their picture naming speed (i.e., that faster speakers are consistently fast). The current study explicitly tested this assumption by asking participants to complete a simple picture naming task twice with one to two weeks in between sessions. In one experiment, we show that picture naming speed has excellent within-task reliability and good test-retest reliability, at least when participants perform the same task in both sessions. In a second experiment with slight task variations across sessions (a speeded and non-speeded picture naming task), we replicated the high split-half reliability and found moderate consistency over tasks. These findings are as predicted under the assumption that the speed of initiating responses for speech production is an intrinsic property or capacity of an individual. We additionally discuss the consequences of these results for the statistical power of correlational designs. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Studies of language production often make use of picture naming tasks to investigate the cognitive processes involved in speaking, and many of these studies report a wide range of individual variability in how long speakers need to prepare the name of a picture. It has been assumed that this variability can be linked to inter-individual differences in cognitive skills or abilities (e.g., attention or working memory); therefore, several studies have tried to explain variability in language production tasks by correlating production measures with scores on cognitive tests. This approach, however, relies on the assumption that participants are reliable over time in their picture naming speed (i.e., that faster speakers are consistently fast). The current study explicitly tested this assumption by asking participants to complete a simple picture naming task twice with one to two weeks in between sessions. In one experiment, we show that picture naming speed has excellent within-task reliability and good test-retest reliability, at least when participants perform the same task in both sessions. In a second experiment with slight task variations across sessions (a speeded and non-speeded picture naming task), we replicated the high split-half reliability and found moderate consistency over tasks. These findings are as predicted under the assumption that the speed of initiating responses for speech production is an intrinsic property or capacity of an individual. We additionally discuss the consequences of these results for the statistical power of correlational designs. Studies of language production often make use of picture naming tasks to investigate the cognitive processes involved in speaking, and many of these studies report a wide range of individual variability in how long speakers need to prepare the name of a picture. It has been assumed that this variability can be linked to inter-individual differences in cognitive skills or abilities (e.g., attention or working memory); therefore, several studies have tried to explain variability in language production tasks by correlating production measures with scores on cognitive tests. This approach, however, relies on the assumption that participants are reliable over time in their picture naming speed (i.e., that faster speakers are consistently fast). The current study explicitly tested this assumption by asking participants to complete a simple picture naming task twice with one to two weeks in between sessions. In one experiment, we show that picture naming speed has excellent within-task reliability and good test-retest reliability, at least when participants perform the same task in both sessions. In a second experiment with slight task variations across sessions (a speeded and non-speeded picture naming task), we replicated the high split-half reliability and found moderate consistency over tasks. These findings are as predicted under the assumption that the speed of initiating responses for speech production is an intrinsic property or capacity of an individual. We additionally discuss the consequences of these results for the statistical power of correlational designs.Studies of language production often make use of picture naming tasks to investigate the cognitive processes involved in speaking, and many of these studies report a wide range of individual variability in how long speakers need to prepare the name of a picture. It has been assumed that this variability can be linked to inter-individual differences in cognitive skills or abilities (e.g., attention or working memory); therefore, several studies have tried to explain variability in language production tasks by correlating production measures with scores on cognitive tests. This approach, however, relies on the assumption that participants are reliable over time in their picture naming speed (i.e., that faster speakers are consistently fast). The current study explicitly tested this assumption by asking participants to complete a simple picture naming task twice with one to two weeks in between sessions. In one experiment, we show that picture naming speed has excellent within-task reliability and good test-retest reliability, at least when participants perform the same task in both sessions. In a second experiment with slight task variations across sessions (a speeded and non-speeded picture naming task), we replicated the high split-half reliability and found moderate consistency over tasks. These findings are as predicted under the assumption that the speed of initiating responses for speech production is an intrinsic property or capacity of an individual. We additionally discuss the consequences of these results for the statistical power of correlational designs. |
Author | Fuhrmeister, Pamela Bürki, Audrey Elbuy, Shereen |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Pamela orcidid: 0000-0003-0507-1185 surname: Fuhrmeister fullname: Fuhrmeister, Pamela – sequence: 2 givenname: Shereen orcidid: 0000-0002-3547-0841 surname: Elbuy fullname: Elbuy, Shereen – sequence: 3 givenname: Audrey orcidid: 0000-0002-1390-2826 surname: Bürki fullname: Bürki, Audrey |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38223223$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpVkc1q3DAURkVJaNI09A2Kdu1mEknXsuVVGULTBtIk9GctZOk6aPBYrq5NmV1fI6-XJ6knMw0tCCSko_MJfa_YQZ96ZOyNFGcaoDhfJX8GUL1gx0rLYlEYJQ7-WR-xU6KVEEJJrUQJL9kRGKVgHsdsWmbkl45GzPzO5TH6OLh-JL7sfrkN7Y8-8CUREsX-nn_FLromdnHc8NT-d-nx9wP_gq6fGRpST8i_DYiBx57fRT9Oc9SNW8-S1-ywdR3h6X4-YT8uP36_-Ly4vv10dbG8Xngo1LiodTDGh7ZpClkYbHwFJZaIPtQBWylqaHRoZAAE46SXwrsKtUJRBSO0qeGEXe28IbmVHXJcu7yxyUX7tJHyvX16foe2DFW51UFbQeFbMDqI1hlVVaWTAcPser9zDTn9nJBGu47ksetcj2kiq2pZKG0AtrFv9-jUrDE8B__99hl4twN8TkQZ22dECrvt1M6d2rlT-AOfNZT7 |
Cites_doi | 10.1177/00131640021970475 10.1037/met0000275 10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.001 10.3758/s13421-013-0332-7 10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00052-9 10.3758/s13423-016-1044-3 10.1177/1747021818806491 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1 10.1080/17470218.2014.964736 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02256 10.3758/BF03196772 10.1080/13825585.2016.1257100 10.1111/psyp.12885 10.1080/17470218.2013.850521 10.3758/BF03200540 10.3758/s13423-018-1558-y 10.3758/BF03196853 10.2307/1423649 10.3758/BF03195559 10.1075/ml.17020.kla 10.21500/20112084.807 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.006 10.1177/2515245918771304 10.1080/17470218.2017.1310261 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.082 10.1073/pnas.0908921106 10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.340 10.3758/BF03200741 10.1177/2515245919879695 10.1080/17470218.2015.1093007 10.1006/jmla.1996.0026 10.1016/j.bandl.2021.104993 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.016 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.07.009 10.1177/1745691614551642 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118647 10.1080/17470218.2012.670252 10.3758/BF03200722 10.1016/j.jmp.2015.06.004 10.3758/BF03194070 10.1177/1747021817747266 10.3758/BF03195534 10.3758/s13421-018-0856-y 10.1037/1040-3590.9.1.15 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright: © 2024 The Author(s). |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright: © 2024 The Author(s). |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 DOA |
DOI | 10.5334/joc.337 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 2514-4820 |
EndPage | 12 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_6d763b5d3f734cf385d0fa82776a1ded 38223223 10_5334_joc_337 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | .0O AAFWJ AAPRH AAYXX ABDBF ACCQO ADBBV AFPKN ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BCNDV CITATION GROUPED_DOAJ HYE IAO IER IHR ISR ITC M~E OK1 PGMZT RPM H13 NPM 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c342t-95d88cdfbb4148ebc736e6eecd9def1093b5db1d3e38a1c10ca7e52e07d805893 |
IEDL.DBID | DOA |
ISSN | 2514-4820 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:13:47 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 22:23:21 EDT 2025 Thu Jan 02 22:36:49 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:36:41 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | picture naming language production reliability individual differences |
Language | English |
License | Copyright: © 2024 The Author(s). |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c342t-95d88cdfbb4148ebc736e6eecd9def1093b5db1d3e38a1c10ca7e52e07d805893 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-3547-0841 0000-0003-0507-1185 0000-0002-1390-2826 |
OpenAccessLink | https://doaj.org/article/6d763b5d3f734cf385d0fa82776a1ded |
PMID | 38223223 |
PQID | 2914258339 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 1 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_6d763b5d3f734cf385d0fa82776a1ded proquest_miscellaneous_2914258339 pubmed_primary_38223223 crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_337 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2024-01-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2024-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2024 text: 2024-01-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Journal of cognition |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Cogn |
PublicationYear | 2024 |
Publisher | Ubiquity Press |
Publisher_xml | – name: Ubiquity Press |
References | (key20240111073325_B60) 1999; 112 (key20240111073325_B14) 1999; 31 (key20240111073325_B38) 2018; 1 (key20240111073325_B3) 2021; 47 (key20240111073325_B29) 2014; 8 (key20240111073325_B9) 2017; 80 (key20240111073325_B33) 2015; 10 (key20240111073325_B34) 2015; 68 (key20240111073325_B37) 1999; 31 (key20240111073325_B22) 2022 (key20240111073325_B47) 2019; 2 (key20240111073325_B54) 2021; 26 (key20240111073325_B61) 2014 (key20240111073325_B31) 2015 (key20240111073325_B67) 2021; 221 (key20240111073325_B36) 2018; 13 (key20240111073325_B66) 2018 (key20240111073325_B39) 2012; 44 (key20240111073325_B20) 2019; 9 (key20240111073325_B49) 2016; 23 key20240111073325_B45 (key20240111073325_B62) 2014; 8 (key20240111073325_B35) 2000; 129 (key20240111073325_B7) 2018; 25 (key20240111073325_B13) 2009; 106 (key20240111073325_B55) 2013; 41 (key20240111073325_B57) 2012; 65 (key20240111073325_B18) 1998; 24 (key20240111073325_B5) 2007; 14 (key20240111073325_B16) 1997; 9 (key20240111073325_B41) 1977 (key20240111073325_B46) 1994 (key20240111073325_B53) 2019; 26 (key20240111073325_B8) 2017; 54 (key20240111073325_B21) 2019; 47 (key20240111073325_B11) 2012; 3 (key20240111073325_B64) 2000; 60 (key20240111073325_B40) 2012; 59 (key20240111073325_B48) 2013; 142 key20240111073325_B50 (key20240111073325_B43) 2016; 72 (key20240111073325_B26) 2013; 30 (key20240111073325_B58) 2016; 69 (key20240111073325_B52) 2004; 32 (key20240111073325_B6) 2018; 9 (key20240111073325_B51) 2016; 146 (key20240111073325_B63) 2014; 67 (key20240111073325_B1) 2004; 36 (key20240111073325_B27) 2020 (key20240111073325_B56) 2014; 1586 (key20240111073325_B44) 1996; 35 (key20240111073325_B65) 2003; 35 (key20240111073325_B24) 2014; 9 (key20240111073325_B28) 2018; 50 (key20240111073325_B32) 2017; 3 (key20240111073325_B2) 2010; 3 (key20240111073325_B23) 2018; 71 (key20240111073325_B19) 2021; 16 (key20240111073325_B4) 2021; 44 (key20240111073325_B17) 2018; 71 (key20240111073325_B59) 1996; 28 (key20240111073325_B30) 2020; 46 (key20240111073325_B10) 2021; 245 (key20240111073325_B12) 2005; 12 (key20240111073325_B15) 2007; 57 (key20240111073325_B25) 1999; 73 (key20240111073325_B42) 2019; 72 |
References_xml | – volume: 60 start-page: 224 issue: 2 year: 2000 ident: key20240111073325_B64 article-title: Measurement error in “big five factors” personality assessment: Reliability generalization across studies and measures publication-title: Educational and Psychological Measurement doi: 10.1177/00131640021970475 – volume: 47 start-page: 466 issue: 3 year: 2021 ident: key20240111073325_B3 article-title: Concurrent speech planning does not eliminate repetition priming from spoken words: Evidence from linguistic dual-tasking publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 26 start-page: 103 issue: 1 year: 2021 ident: key20240111073325_B54 article-title: Toward a principled bayesian workflow in cognitive science publication-title: Psychological Methods doi: 10.1037/met0000275 – volume: 57 start-page: 195 issue: 2 year: 2007 ident: key20240111073325_B15 article-title: Time pressure and phonological advance planning in spoken production publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.001 – volume: 41 start-page: 1200 issue: 8 year: 2013 ident: key20240111073325_B55 article-title: Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming publication-title: Memory & Cognition doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0332-7 – volume: 16 issue: 10 year: 2021 ident: key20240111073325_B19 article-title: Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors publication-title: PLoS One – volume: 44 start-page: 978 issue: 4 year: 2012 ident: key20240111073325_B39 article-title: Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 english words publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4 – volume: 73 start-page: B27 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: key20240111073325_B25 article-title: Age-of-acquisition and frequency effects in speeded word naming publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00052-9 – volume: 23 start-page: 1898 issue: 6 year: 2016 ident: key20240111073325_B49 article-title: Typing is writing: Linguistic properties modulate typing execution publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1044-3 – volume: 72 start-page: 1667 issue: 7 year: 2019 ident: key20240111073325_B42 article-title: Age-related effects in compound production: Evidence from a double-object picture naming task publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1177/1747021818806491 – volume: 50 start-page: 1166 issue: 3 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B28 article-title: The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1 – volume: 68 start-page: 710 issue: 4 year: 2015 ident: key20240111073325_B34 article-title: Sustained attention in language production: An individual differences investigation publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.964736 – volume: 46 start-page: 1048 issue: 6 year: 2020 ident: key20240111073325_B30 article-title: Shared lexical access processes in speaking and listening? An individual differences study publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – start-page: 159 year: 1977 ident: key20240111073325_B41 article-title: The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data publication-title: Biometrics – volume: 9 start-page: 2256 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B6 article-title: Exploring different types of inhibition during bilingual language production publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02256 – year: 2020 ident: key20240111073325_B27 publication-title: Learning from the reliability paradox: How theoretically informed generative models can advance the social, behavioral, and brain sciences – volume: 12 start-page: 769 issue: 5 year: 2005 ident: key20240111073325_B12 article-title: Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03196772 – volume: 25 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B7 article-title: Behavioral evidence for a differential modulation of semantic processing and lexical production by aging: A full linear mixed-effects modeling approach publication-title: Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition doi: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1257100 – volume-title: Research methods in psychology-2nd canadian edition year: 2015 ident: key20240111073325_B31 – volume: 54 start-page: 1370 issue: 9 year: 2017 ident: key20240111073325_B8 article-title: Electrophysiological characterization of facilitation and interference in the picture-word interference paradigm publication-title: Psychophysiology doi: 10.1111/psyp.12885 – volume: 80 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2017 ident: key20240111073325_B9 article-title: brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan publication-title: Journal of Statistical Software – volume: 67 start-page: 1176 issue: 6 year: 2014 ident: key20240111073325_B63 article-title: SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for british english publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.850521 – volume: 10 issue: 9 year: 2015 ident: key20240111073325_B33 article-title: The role of sustained attention in the production of conjoined noun phrases: An individual differences study publication-title: PloS One – volume: 28 start-page: 516 issue: 4 year: 1996 ident: key20240111073325_B59 article-title: Naming times for the snodgrass and vanderwart pictures publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03200540 – volume-title: Psychometric theory 3E year: 1994 ident: key20240111073325_B46 – volume: 24 start-page: 515 issue: 2 year: 1998 ident: key20240111073325_B18 article-title: Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 26 start-page: 452 issue: 2 year: 2019 ident: key20240111073325_B53 article-title: A psychometrics of individual differences in experimental tasks publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1558-y – volume: 32 start-page: 212 issue: 2 year: 2004 ident: key20240111073325_B52 article-title: Seriality of phonological encoding in naming objects and reading their names publication-title: Memory & Cognition doi: 10.3758/BF03196853 – volume: 112 start-page: 521 issue: 4 year: 1999 ident: key20240111073325_B60 article-title: Word substitution errors in a speeded picture-word task publication-title: The American Journal of Psychology doi: 10.2307/1423649 – volume: 8 start-page: 390 year: 2014 ident: key20240111073325_B62 article-title: ERP correlates of word production predictors in picture naming: A trial by trial multiple regression analysis from stimulus onset to response publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroscience – year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B66 article-title: Penncontroller for internet based experiments (ibex) – ident: key20240111073325_B45 – volume: 36 start-page: 140 issue: 1 year: 2004 ident: key20240111073325_B1 article-title: Predictors of picture naming speed publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03195559 – volume: 13 start-page: 159 issue: 2 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B36 article-title: An investigation of the role of working memory capacity and naming speed in phonological advance planning in language production publication-title: The Mental Lexicon doi: 10.1075/ml.17020.kla – volume: 3 start-page: 12 issue: 2 year: 2010 ident: key20240111073325_B2 article-title: Analyzing reaction times publication-title: International Journal of Psychological Research doi: 10.21500/20112084.807 – volume: 142 start-page: 332 issue: 3 year: 2013 ident: key20240111073325_B48 article-title: Working memory capacity and dual-task interference in picture naming publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.006 – volume: 1 start-page: 270 issue: 2 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B38 article-title: Rejecting or accepting parameter values in bayesian estimation publication-title: Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/2515245918771304 – volume: 71 start-page: 808 issue: 4 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B17 article-title: MultiPic: A standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six european languages publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1310261 – volume: 59 start-page: 3881 issue: 4 year: 2012 ident: key20240111073325_B40 article-title: Time course of word production in fast and slow speakers: A high density ERP topographic study publication-title: NeuroImage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.082 – volume: 106 start-page: 21442 issue: 50 year: 2009 ident: key20240111073325_B13 article-title: The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908921106 – volume: 129 start-page: 340 issue: 3 year: 2000 ident: key20240111073325_B35 article-title: The task dependence of staged versus cascaded processing: An empirical and computational study of stroop interference in speech perception publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.340 – volume: 9 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: key20240111073325_B20 article-title: Interference in speaking while hearing and vice versa publication-title: Scientific Reports – volume: 3 issue: 1 year: 2017 ident: key20240111073325_B32 article-title: Sustained attention ability affects simple picture naming publication-title: Collabra: Psychology – volume: 31 start-page: 650 issue: 4 year: 1999 ident: key20240111073325_B14 article-title: Naming times for the snodgrass and vanderwart pictures in spanish publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03200741 – volume: 2 start-page: 378 issue: 4 year: 2019 ident: key20240111073325_B47 article-title: Psychological science needs a standard practice of reporting the reliability of cognitive-behavioral measurements publication-title: Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/2515245919879695 – volume: 69 start-page: 1719 issue: 9 year: 2016 ident: key20240111073325_B58 article-title: Executive control in spoken noun-phrase production: Contributions of updating, inhibiting, and shifting publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1093007 – volume: 3 start-page: 37 year: 2012 ident: key20240111073325_B11 article-title: Measurement and reliability of response inhibition publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology – volume: 35 start-page: 477 issue: 4 year: 1996 ident: key20240111073325_B44 article-title: Lexical access in phrase and sentence production: Results from picture–word interference experiments publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0026 – volume: 221 start-page: 104993 year: 2021 ident: key20240111073325_B67 article-title: Cross-linguistic interference in late language learners: An ERP study publication-title: Brain and Language doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2021.104993 – volume: 44 year: 2021 ident: key20240111073325_B4 publication-title: Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [computer program]. Version 6.1 – volume: 146 start-page: 240 year: 2016 ident: key20240111073325_B51 article-title: Language production is facilitated by semantic richness but inhibited by semantic density: Evidence from picture naming publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.016 – volume: 1586 start-page: 130 year: 2014 ident: key20240111073325_B56 article-title: Electrophysiological evidence that inhibition supports lexical selection in picture naming publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.07.009 – volume: 9 start-page: 641 issue: 6 year: 2014 ident: key20240111073325_B24 article-title: Beyond power calculations: Assessing type s (sign) and type m (magnitude) errors publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/1745691614551642 – volume: 245 start-page: 118647 year: 2021 ident: key20240111073325_B10 article-title: Trial and error: A hierarchical modeling approach to test-retest reliability publication-title: NeuroImage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118647 – ident: key20240111073325_B50 – volume: 30 start-page: 476 issue: 7–8 year: 2013 ident: key20240111073325_B26 article-title: Semantic interference in a randomized naming task: Effects of age, order, and category publication-title: Cognitive Neuropsychology – volume: 65 start-page: 1927 issue: 10 year: 2012 ident: key20240111073325_B57 article-title: Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.670252 – volume: 31 start-page: 429 issue: 3 year: 1999 ident: key20240111073325_B37 article-title: The reliability and stability of the turner and engle working memory task publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03200722 – volume: 72 start-page: 19 year: 2016 ident: key20240111073325_B43 article-title: Harold jeffreys’s default bayes factor hypothesis tests: Explanation, extension, and application in psychology publication-title: Journal of Mathematical Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2015.06.004 – volume: 14 start-page: 313 issue: 2 year: 2007 ident: key20240111073325_B5 article-title: Simon says: Reliability and the role of working memory and attentional control in the simon task publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03194070 – volume: 71 start-page: 2562 issue: 12 year: 2018 ident: key20240111073325_B23 article-title: No lexical competition without priming: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1177/1747021817747266 – volume: 35 start-page: 550 issue: 4 year: 2003 ident: key20240111073325_B65 article-title: The reliability and stability of verbal working memory measures publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03195534 – volume-title: Essentials of psychological testing year: 2014 ident: key20240111073325_B61 – volume: 47 start-page: 145 issue: 1 year: 2019 ident: key20240111073325_B21 article-title: A close call: Interference from semantic neighbourhood density and similarity in language production publication-title: Memory & Cognition doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0856-y – volume: 8 start-page: 150 year: 2014 ident: key20240111073325_B29 article-title: The speed-accuracy tradeoff: History, physiology, methodology, and behavior publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroscience – year: 2022 ident: key20240111073325_B22 article-title: Behavioral and EEG evidence for inter-individual variability in late encoding stages of word production publication-title: Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience – volume: 9 start-page: 15 issue: 1 year: 1997 ident: key20240111073325_B16 article-title: A short form of the WISC–III for clinical use publication-title: Psychological Assessment doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.9.1.15 |
SSID | ssj0002152063 |
Score | 2.2431657 |
Snippet | Studies of language production often make use of picture naming tasks to investigate the cognitive processes involved in speaking, and many of these studies... |
SourceID | doaj proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | 12 |
SubjectTerms | individual differences language production picture naming reliability |
Title | Are Faster Participants Always Faster? Assessing Reliability of Participants’ Mean Response Speed in Picture Naming |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38223223 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2914258339 https://doaj.org/article/6d763b5d3f734cf385d0fa82776a1ded |
Volume | 7 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3JTsMwELWgp14QO2WpjMQ1bRMnsXNCBVEhpJZKUKm3yMtEKoK06iLUG7_B7_EljOO0KgfEhWscO9a8eBZ7_IaQK4TUZEooj2thvBBNpCcVJF5UXAoJQ50VRNrdXnw_CB-G0XCj1JfNCXP0wE5wzdjgClCRYRln2JOJyLQyKQLOY-kbMFb7os3bCKasDrbVWtH4uluy9rZp82WsG8xWO98wPwVL_--uZWFiOrtkp_QNadvNaY9sQb5PqmsVtTwgi_YUaEdacgPal2VGdD6f0fbru1zOyqZr6o5y0SpRm3LsqLiXdJz96PT18Um7IHN8p8iTBfo0QVtGRzntj4qTBdqTbzjIIRl07p5v772ycIKnWRjMvSQyQmjEQIUY7YDSnMUQA2iTGMgsgRQKVPmGARPS135LSw5RAC1uhC0zyI5IJR_ncEIoRjwqyRLBJYtDHEgpZgw6EQGXCHICNUJX8kwnjh8jxbjCijxFkaco8hq5sXJeN1tC6-IBwpyWMKd_wVwjlyuUUlwA9lRD5jBezNIg8VHvCMaSGjl28K0_xdD9QY3FTv9jCmekGqBH4_ZfzkllPl3ABXokc1Un243WY734Bb8BXdfjoQ |
linkProvider | Directory of Open Access Journals |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Are+Faster+Participants+Always+Faster%3F+Assessing+Reliability+of+Participants%27+Mean+Response+Speed+in+Picture+Naming&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+cognition&rft.au=Fuhrmeister%2C+Pamela&rft.au=Elbuy%2C+Shereen&rft.au=B%C3%BCrki%2C+Audrey&rft.date=2024-01-01&rft.eissn=2514-4820&rft.volume=7&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=12&rft_id=info:doi/10.5334%2Fjoc.337&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F38223223&rft.externalDocID=38223223 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2514-4820&client=summon |