Visual Motion Interferes with Tactile Motion Perception
Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual apparent motion on judgments of the direction of tactile apparent motion. When visual motion was presented at the same time as, but in a direction o...
Saved in:
Published in | Perception (London) Vol. 35; no. 3; pp. 351 - 367 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
London, England
SAGE Publications
01.01.2006
Pion |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual apparent motion on judgments of the direction of tactile apparent motion. When visual motion was presented at the same time as, but in a direction opposite to, tactile motion, accuracy in judging the direction of tactile apparent motion was substantially reduced. This reduction in performance is referred to as ‘the congruency effect’. Similar effects were observed when the visual display was placed either near to the tactile display or at some distance from the tactile display (experiment 1). In experiment 2, the relative alignment between the visual and tactile directions of motion was varied. The size of the congruency effect was similar at 0° and 45° alignments but much reduced at a 90° alignment. In experiment 3, subjects made confidence ratings of their judgments of the direction of the tactile motion. The results indicated that the congruency effect was not due to subjects being unsure of the direction of motion and being forced to guess. In experiment 4, static visual stimuli were shown to have no effect on the judgments of direction of the tactile stimuli. The extent to which the congruency effect reflects capture effects and is the result of perceptual versus post-perceptual processes is discussed. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual apparent motion on judgments of the direction of tactile apparent motion. When visual motion was presented at the same time as, but in a direction opposite to, tactile motion, accuracy in judging the direction of tactile apparent motion was substantially reduced. This reduction in performance is referred to as ‘the congruency effect’. Similar effects were observed when the visual display was placed either near to the tactile display or at some distance from the tactile display (experiment 1). In experiment 2, the relative alignment between the visual and tactile directions of motion was varied. The size of the congruency effect was similar at 0° and 45° alignments but much reduced at a 90° alignment. In experiment 3, subjects made confidence ratings of their judgments of the direction of the tactile motion. The results indicated that the congruency effect was not due to subjects being unsure of the direction of motion and being forced to guess. In experiment 4, static visual stimuli were shown to have no effect on the judgments of direction of the tactile stimuli. The extent to which the congruency effect reflects capture effects and is the result of perceptual versus post-perceptual processes is discussed. Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual apparent motion on judgments of the direction of tactile apparent motion. When visual motion was presented at the same time as, but in a direction opposite to, tactile motion, accuracy in judging the direction of tactile apparent motion was substantially reduced. This reduction in performance is referred to as 'the congruency effect'. Similar effects were observed when the visual display was placed either near to the tactile display or at some distance from the tactile display (experiment 1). In experiment 2, the relative alignment between the visual and tactile directions of motion was varied. The size of the congruency effect was similar at 0 degrees and 45 degrees alignments but much reduced at a 90 degrees alignment. In experiment 3, subjects made confidence ratings of their judgments of the direction of the tactile motion. The results indicated that the congruency effect was not due to subjects being unsure of the direction of motion and being forced to guess. In experiment 4, static visual stimuli were shown to have no effect on the judgments of direction of the tactile stimuli. The extent to which the congruency effect reflects capture effects and is the result of perceptual versus post-perceptual processes is discussed.Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual apparent motion on judgments of the direction of tactile apparent motion. When visual motion was presented at the same time as, but in a direction opposite to, tactile motion, accuracy in judging the direction of tactile apparent motion was substantially reduced. This reduction in performance is referred to as 'the congruency effect'. Similar effects were observed when the visual display was placed either near to the tactile display or at some distance from the tactile display (experiment 1). In experiment 2, the relative alignment between the visual and tactile directions of motion was varied. The size of the congruency effect was similar at 0 degrees and 45 degrees alignments but much reduced at a 90 degrees alignment. In experiment 3, subjects made confidence ratings of their judgments of the direction of the tactile motion. The results indicated that the congruency effect was not due to subjects being unsure of the direction of motion and being forced to guess. In experiment 4, static visual stimuli were shown to have no effect on the judgments of direction of the tactile stimuli. The extent to which the congruency effect reflects capture effects and is the result of perceptual versus post-perceptual processes is discussed. Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual apparent motion on judgments of the direction of tactile apparent motion. When visual motion was presented at the same time as, but in a direction opposite to, tactile motion, accuracy in judging the direction of tactile apparent motion was substantially reduced. This reduction in performance is referred to as 'the congruency effect'. Similar effects were observed when the visual display was placed either near to the tactile display or at some distance from the tactile display (experiment 1). In experiment 2, the relative alignment between the visual and tactile directions of motion was varied. The size of the congruency effect was similar at 0 degrees and 45 degrees alignments but much reduced at a 90 degrees alignment. In experiment 3, subjects made confidence ratings of their judgments of the direction of the tactile motion. The results indicated that the congruency effect was not due to subjects being unsure of the direction of motion and being forced to guess. In experiment 4, static visual stimuli were shown to have no effect on the judgments of direction of the tactile stimuli. The extent to which the congruency effect reflects capture effects and is the result of perceptual versus post-perceptual processes is discussed. |
Author | Craig, James C |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: James C surname: Craig fullname: Craig, James C email: craigj@indiana.edu organization: Department of Psychology, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=17679249$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16619951$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpd0F1LwzAUBuAgE_fh_oIMQW-ketKkSXspw4_BRC-mtyFLTzSja2fSIv57O_cFuzoHzsML5-2TTlmVSMiQwi0Fkd6tEsb4CelRLtKIx4x1SA8Y0AhAiC7ph7AAoDxL2BnpUiFoliW0R-SHC40uRi9V7apyNClr9BY9htGPq79GM21qV-Du_Ibe4Gq9npNTq4uAw-0ckPfHh9n4OZq-Pk3G99PIMBbXEXKeZtJCEsscNAWQOYtRJFojAOcyBZOkxoDBPI3BxomldM4wntsMuOGWDcj1Jnflq-8GQ62WLhgsCl1i1QQlZCqBA2vhxRY28yXmauXdUvtftXu1BVdboIPRhfW6NC4cnBQyi3nWupuNM74KwaNVxtV6_XPttSsUBbUuXP0Xfkjd633gkbvcuKA_US2qxpdtbUfoD45shyI |
CODEN | PCTNBA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00342_2017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijpsycho_2015_04_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2010_07_064 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_006_0829_4 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_009_1763_z crossref_primary_10_1068_p5844 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainresbull_2007_09_004 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_015_4429_z crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_008_1637_9 crossref_primary_10_5406_amerjpsyc_123_4_0413 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_09493_2 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00192_2006 crossref_primary_10_5406_amerjpsyc_124_4_0413 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_017_1476_5 crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_20897 crossref_primary_10_1177_0301006619886237 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2022_108209 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2009_03_035 crossref_primary_10_1109_TOH_2024_3482570 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_70831_3 crossref_primary_10_1080_13467581_2023_2300395 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_010_2420_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2023_108696 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_01989_1 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4850_y crossref_primary_10_4036_iis_2015_A_03 crossref_primary_10_4036_iis_2015_A_04 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01692_w |
Cites_doi | 10.1111/1467-9280.00270 10.3758/CABN.4.2.241 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501010.x 10.3758/CABN.4.2.182 10.1109/TMMS.1970.299963 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524861.003.0004 10.1068/p5255 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524861.003.0007 10.3758/BF03194814 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00740-5 10.1167/3.9.771 10.3758/BF03208826 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00130-2 10.1038/35784 10.3758/BF03209769 10.3758/BF03207017 10.1068/p5293 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524861.001.0001 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301008.x 10.1016/S0166-4115(99)80034-X 10.1037/0096-1523.30.2.330 10.7551/mitpress/3422.003.0007 10.3758/BF03205819 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00185-4 10.3758/BF03194730 10.7551/mitpress/3422.001.0001 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00053 10.1007/s00221-002-1085-x 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00068-X |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2006 SAGE Publications 2006 INIST-CNRS |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2006 SAGE Publications – notice: 2006 INIST-CNRS |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1068/p5334 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Anatomy & Physiology Psychology |
EISSN | 1468-4233 |
EndPage | 367 |
ExternalDocumentID | 16619951 17679249 10_1068_p5334 10.1068_p5334 |
Genre | Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIDCD NIH HHS grantid: DC00095 |
GroupedDBID | --- -TM -~X .GJ 0R~ 123 4.4 53G 54M AACKU AADIR AADUE AAGGD AAGLT AAJPV AAKTJ AANSI AAPEO AAQXI AARIX AATAA AAVDI AAWLO AAZLU ABAWP ABCCA ABCJG ABDBF ABDPE ABEIX ABETI ABFXH ABIDT ABIVO ABJNI ABKRH ABNCE ABPNF ABQKF ABQNX ABQPY ABQXT ABRHV ABUJY ABYTW ABZEH ACABN ACAEP ACCVJ ACDXX ACFUR ACFZE ACGFS ACHQT ACJER ACNXM ACOFE ACOXC ACROE ACSIQ ACUFS ACUHS ACUIR ADEBD ADEIA ADMHG ADNON ADPEE ADRRZ ADTOS ADUKL AEDXQ AENEX AEOBU AEONT AESMA AESZF AETEA AEUHG AEWDL AEWHI AEXNY AFEET AFFNX AFHKK AFKRG AFMOU AFQAA AFUIA AFVCE AGDVU AGKLV AGNHF AGNWV AHDMH AHHCN AHOJL AHWHD AJUZI AJXGE ALFTD ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ARTOV AUTPY AYPQM BDZRT BMVBW BPACV CBRKF CCGJY CEADM CS3 DOPDO DU5 DV7 DV8 EBS EJD F5P FA8 FHBDP GROUPED_SAGE_PREMIER_JOURNAL_COLLECTION H13 HZ~ H~9 IL9 J8X NIF O9- P2P Q1R ROL SASJQ SAUOL SCNPE SFC SJN SSDHQ TN5 UQL WHG XJT YNT ZCA ZKB ZPLXX ZPPRI ~32 AAEJI AAPII AAYXX ACCVC AJGYC AJVBE AMNSR CITATION IQODW M4V AAMGE ACTQU AEUIJ AIOMO CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM VXZ 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c332t-e44897f0527d0a1007d32e65aae0044780c58cc0ced820f25f11b3e2bf904c4f3 |
ISSN | 0301-0066 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 09:02:55 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 01:46:27 EST 2025 Wed Apr 23 03:04:28 EDT 2025 Tue Aug 05 12:01:22 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:27 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 22:32:01 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 3 |
Keywords | Human Tactile sensitivity Stimulus movement Vision Interference Perception Experimental study Intermodal perception |
Language | English |
License | https://journals.sagepub.com/page/policies/text-and-data-mining-license CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c332t-e44897f0527d0a1007d32e65aae0044780c58cc0ced820f25f11b3e2bf904c4f3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
PMID | 16619951 |
PQID | 67870403 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 17 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_67870403 pubmed_primary_16619951 pascalfrancis_primary_17679249 crossref_citationtrail_10_1068_p5334 crossref_primary_10_1068_p5334 sage_journals_10_1068_p5334 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2006-01-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2006-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2006 text: 2006-01-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | London, England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: London, England – name: London – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Perception (London) |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Perception |
PublicationYear | 2006 |
Publisher | SAGE Publications Pion |
Publisher_xml | – name: SAGE Publications – name: Pion |
References | Craig 2003; 65 Soto-Faraco, Spence, Kingstone 2004; 30 Hofbauer, Wuerger, Meyer, Roehrbein, Schill, Zetzsche 2004; 4 Caclin, Soto-Faraco, Kingstone, Spence 2002; 64 Kirman 1974; 15 Craig 2005; 34 Igarashi, Kitagawa, Ichihara 2004; 4 Pick, Warren, Hay 1969; 6 Soto-Faraco, Spence, Lloyd, Kingstone 2004; 13 Gray, Tan 2002; 145 Pavani, Spence, Driver 2000; 11 Bertelson, Aschersleben 2003; 50 Botvinick, Cohen 1998; 391 Soto-Faraco, Lyons, Gazzaniga, Spence, Kingstone 2002; 14 Rinker, Craig 1994; 56 Strybel, Vatakis 2004; 33 Meyer, Wuerger 2001; 12 Hotting, Röder 2004; 15 Wuerger, Roehrbein, Meyer, Hofbauer, Schill, Zetzsche 2003; 3 Driver, Spence 2000; 10 Bertelson, Aschersleben 1998; 5 Soto-Faraco, Kingstone, Spence 2003; 41 bibr26-p5334 bibr16-p5334 bibr11-p5334 bibr21-p5334 bibr31-p5334 bibr18-p5334 bibr7-p5334 bibr24-p5334 bibr14-p5334 bibr9-p5334 bibr3-p5334 bibr12-p5334 bibr27-p5334 Soto-Faraco S (bibr23-p5334) 2004 bibr17-p5334 bibr1-p5334 bibr6-p5334 bibr4-p5334 bibr15-p5334 bibr25-p5334 bibr8-p5334 bibr19-p5334 bibr10-p5334 bibr20-p5334 bibr30-p5334 Sherrick C E (bibr22-p5334) 1968 bibr13-p5334 Stein B E (bibr29-p5334) 1993 bibr2-p5334 bibr28-p5334 bibr5-p5334 |
References_xml | – volume: 11 start-page: 353 year: 2000 end-page: 359 article-title: “Visual capture of touch: Out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves” publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 3 start-page: 771a year: 2003 article-title: “Auditory and visual motion signals have to be co-localized to be effectively integrated” publication-title: Journal of Vision – volume: 15 start-page: 1 year: 1974 end-page: 6 article-title: “Tactile apparent motion: The effects of interstimulus onset interval and stimulus duration” publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics – volume: 65 start-page: 779 year: 2003 end-page: 788 article-title: “The effect of hand position and pattern motion on temporal order judgments” publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics – volume: 14 start-page: 139 year: 2002 end-page: 146 article-title: “The ventriloquist in motion: Illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities” publication-title: Cognitive Brain Research – volume: 4 start-page: 182 year: 2004 end-page: 192 article-title: “Vision of pictorial hand modulates visual-tactile interactions” publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 391 start-page: 756 year: 1998 article-title: “Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see” publication-title: Nature – volume: 12 start-page: 2557 year: 2001 end-page: 2560 article-title: “Cross-modal integration of auditory and visual motion signals” publication-title: NeuroReport – volume: 145 start-page: 50 year: 2002 end-page: 55 article-title: “Dynamic and predictive links between touch and vision” publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 34 start-page: 357 year: 2005 end-page: 370 article-title: “The trajectory effect in intermodal temporal order judgments” publication-title: Perception – volume: 56 start-page: 356 year: 1994 end-page: 362 article-title: “The effect of spatial orientation on the perception of moving tactile stimuli” publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics – volume: 13 start-page: 29 year: 2004 end-page: 33 article-title: “Moving multisensory research along: Motion perception across sensory modalities” publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science – volume: 5 start-page: 482 year: 1998 end-page: 489 article-title: “Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location” publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 50 start-page: 147 year: 2003 end-page: 155 article-title: “Temporal ventriloquism: crossmodal interaction on the time dimension: 1. Evidence from auditory-visual temporal order judgment” publication-title: International Journal of Psychophysiology – volume: 4 start-page: 241 year: 2004 end-page: 250 article-title: “Catching audio-visual mice: Predicting the arrival time of auditory-visual motion signals” publication-title: Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 10 start-page: R731 year: 2000 end-page: R735 article-title: “Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence” publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 30 start-page: 330 year: 2004 end-page: 345 article-title: “Cross-modal dynamic capture: Congruency effects in the perception of motion across sensory modalities” publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 33 start-page: 1033 year: 2004 end-page: 1048 article-title: “A comparison of auditory and visual apparent motion presented individually and with crossmodal moving distractors” publication-title: Perception – volume: 41 start-page: 1847 year: 2003 end-page: 1862 article-title: “Multisensory contributions to the perception of motion” publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 64 start-page: 616 year: 2002 end-page: 630 article-title: “Tactile ‘capture’ of audition” publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics – volume: 15 start-page: 60 year: 2004 end-page: 64 article-title: “Hearing cheats touch, but less in congenitally blind than in sighted individuals” publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 6 start-page: 203 year: 1969 end-page: 205 article-title: “Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction” publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics – ident: bibr19-p5334 doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00270 – ident: bibr13-p5334 doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.2.241 – ident: bibr14-p5334 doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501010.x – ident: bibr15-p5334 doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.2.182 – ident: bibr5-p5334 doi: 10.1109/TMMS.1970.299963 – ident: bibr17-p5334 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524861.003.0004 – ident: bibr30-p5334 doi: 10.1068/p5255 – ident: bibr4-p5334 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524861.003.0007 – ident: bibr9-p5334 doi: 10.3758/BF03194814 – ident: bibr11-p5334 doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00740-5 – ident: bibr31-p5334 doi: 10.1167/3.9.771 – ident: bibr2-p5334 doi: 10.3758/BF03208826 – ident: bibr3-p5334 doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00130-2 – ident: bibr6-p5334 doi: 10.1038/35784 – ident: bibr21-p5334 doi: 10.3758/BF03209769 – volume-title: The Merging of the Senses year: 1993 ident: bibr29-p5334 – ident: bibr20-p5334 doi: 10.3758/BF03207017 – ident: bibr10-p5334 doi: 10.1068/p5293 – ident: bibr28-p5334 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524861.001.0001 – ident: bibr27-p5334 doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301008.x – ident: bibr1-p5334 doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(99)80034-X – ident: bibr26-p5334 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.2.330 – start-page: 331 volume-title: The Skin Senses year: 1968 ident: bibr22-p5334 – start-page: 49 volume-title: The Handbook of Multisensory Processes year: 2004 ident: bibr23-p5334 doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3422.003.0007 – ident: bibr16-p5334 doi: 10.3758/BF03205819 – ident: bibr24-p5334 doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00185-4 – ident: bibr7-p5334 doi: 10.3758/BF03194730 – ident: bibr8-p5334 doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3422.001.0001 – ident: bibr18-p5334 doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00053 – ident: bibr12-p5334 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1085-x – ident: bibr25-p5334 doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00068-X |
SSID | ssj0014953 |
Score | 1.9668188 |
Snippet | Previous studies have demonstrated that visual apparent motion can alter the judgment of auditory apparent motion. We investigated the effect of visual... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref sage |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 351 |
SubjectTerms | Analysis of Variance Biological and medical sciences Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Judgment Male Motion Perception - physiology Multimodal perception Orientation Perception Photic Stimulation Psychological Tests Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Self-Assessment Tactile perception Touch - physiology Vision |
Title | Visual Motion Interferes with Tactile Motion Perception |
URI | https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1068/p5334 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16619951 https://www.proquest.com/docview/67870403 |
Volume | 35 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1ba9swFD5sKZRCGVu6i9et88PYmzfFtmT7MZSWMpqug2RkT0aSpVHonNC4D-2v79HFdkIDu7wYI8uy8SdLn3S-cw7AR0mrgvHY5HchWZSOeBFxKVikUiE0VZIKG1JocsHOZunXOZ33oS6sd0kjPsv7rX4l_4MqliGuxkv2H5DtGsUCPEd88YgI4_GvMP5xtbLeHwuvWDQppxWun_3uqvFZuFbt5WUnYVknpJdd6Xpyj2534PiGX_3q1LR-S3XbJoF3jjIFxCU4aQc-FyfEA5ysjWKJiwH7aHQlzHgMLI33bj99tCbzi2_l6ez8vJyezKdPYSdG2h4PYGf88_L7pLPrGDWrs-u419mFfd_wF9vsBjvYX_IVdlTtMoxsWwJsyO8sI5g-h2eeyodjh8sLeKLqIRyMa94sft-Fn0IrrrVWiyHsTryGYQh73ZxzdwCZgzB0GIU9hKGBMPQQtpd7CF_C7PRkenwW-VwWkUySuMG-n-ZFpgmNs4pwI02pklgxyrkyNvUsJ5LmUhKpKuRkOqZ6NBKJioUuSCpTnbyCQb2o1RsIRa6RY1UqkyRPKxNYOjFSXqbjlI4yIQP8A_03LKUP9G7yjVyXVnDA8tJ-6gCOumpLF9nkUYUNAPpaGcvMsj2ADy0iJQ5KxtLEa7W4XZXMTAMpSQJ47YDq70U-WCCrD-DQIFf6v2m1-ei3f2z5EPb6fv4OBs3NrXqP9LARR77PPQDgEWc_ |
linkProvider | SAGE Publications |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV1LT-MwEB5BkQAJ8SgLhGcOaG-BNPEjOVarReURHlJZwSmKHVtaAaEi7YH99etx0lRhV4iz7cTxOPY3M9_MABxLmscsC7C-i8890stiL5OCeYoIoamSVNiUQsk1G9yTiwf6UCuKGAtTr2B5grQqMyN7WDeKJotORxg7Og8LEUYgdGCh_3h7lzR-A2RLVn4DoySb23QRVloDW7fPyigrzULoqoLF_yBmi95lb5yzNbiZzrUimjydTMZmmn8-pHH8-sesw2oNPt1-tVs2YE4VXdjsF0bxfnl3v7uWDmrt7F1YTGqvexeWm1PyfRP4r9_lxDwkseV_XGtRxJjB0kWTrjvEQIlnNW2-bXgz3-D-7Ofwx8Crqy94MgyDsZEWiWKufRrw3M-QTJGHgWI0yxR6gXnkSxpJ6UuVGxShA6p7PRGqQOjYJ5LocAs6xWuhdsAVkTaoIFdc-hHJMRVyiORTpgNCe1xIx-yZWiqprFOTY4WM59S6yFmU2oVy4LDpNqpycfzToSXSWS_OOCqaDhxNZZya3wh9I1mhXidlyvDgIn7owHYl-tlYg2Big0Md2EMxplOhtl-9-2nrESwNhslVenV-fbkHyzMzzj50xm8TdWCAzVgc1rv5L2XO80o |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LT-MwEB6xRaqQELBlF7K8ckDcAqnjR3KsFioeW9SVAMEpih1bWgGhIu0Bfj0eJ22VBSFx9iOOx_a8vpkB2FcsT3hGsL5LKALazZIgU5IHmkppmFZMupRCg0t-ek3Pb9ntApBpLEy9g-Uhwqrsitxjjbd7lJtK2eTx0QjjR7_BomVOhLRgsXc3_DuY-Q4QMVn5DqyibDlqG5YbAxscaHmUlXYzTFXF4iMxswHxclynv1pFApYuWSGCTe4PJ2O71Nf_Ujl-7YfWYKUWQv1edWq-w4IuOrDeK6wC_vjiH_gOFurs7R1oD2rveweWZq_lyzqIm3_lxE4ycGWAfGdZxNjB0kfTrn-FARMPeto8nOFnfsB1_-Tq92lQV2EIVBSRsaUajRNhQkZEHmYIqsgjojnLMo3eYBGHisVKhUrnVpowhJluV0aaSJOEVFET_YRW8VToTfBlbKx0kGuhwpjmmBI5QhAqN4SyrpDKs2enpkyq6hTlWCnjIXWuch6nbqM82J11G1U5Od51aJB13ktwgQqnB3tTOqf2OqGPJCv006RMOT5gNIw82KjIPx9rJZnEyqMebCEp0ylhm5_-9WnrHrSHx_30z9nlxRYsza0529AaP0_0jpVvxnK3PtBvNmD1vw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Visual+motion+interferes+with+tactile+motion+perception&rft.jtitle=Perception+%28London%29&rft.au=Craig%2C+James+C&rft.date=2006-01-01&rft.issn=0301-0066&rft.volume=35&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=351&rft_id=info:doi/10.1068%2Fp5334&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0301-0066&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0301-0066&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0301-0066&client=summon |