Instructional sequences in science teaching: considering element interactivity when sequencing inquiry-based investigation activities and explicit instruction

The use of investigations in science teaching is both common and commonly advocated for in science education literature. We suggest that the use of investigations should differ depending on the complexity of the subject matter. That complexity can vary depending on both the nature of the information...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inEuropean journal of psychology of education Vol. 39; no. 4; pp. 3791 - 3801
Main Authors Zhang, Lin, Sweller, John
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Dordrecht Springer Netherlands 01.12.2024
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0256-2928
1878-5174
DOI10.1007/s10212-024-00799-5

Cover

Abstract The use of investigations in science teaching is both common and commonly advocated for in science education literature. We suggest that the use of investigations should differ depending on the complexity of the subject matter. That complexity can vary depending on both the nature of the information and students’ expertise levels. The present study used Cognitive Load Theory and tested a hypothesis that the sequential order of having investigations first followed by explicit instruction only is more effective than that of having explicit instruction first when students have acquired sufficiently high levels of knowledge. This hypothesis was tested with 85 middle school students. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two sequences. The results indicated that for sufficiently knowledgeable students, a dis-ordinal knowledge level by sequence interaction was obtained. There was an advantage to the explicit instruction first approach for the less knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was high but an advantage to the investigation first approach for the more knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was lower. The results were discussed in light of a recent debate between the authors and De Jong et al. ( Educational Research Review , 39, 1-14, 2023 ) on the role of investigations and explicit instruction in science education.
AbstractList The use of investigations in science teaching is both common and commonly advocated for in science education literature. We suggest that the use of investigations should differ depending on the complexity of the subject matter. That complexity can vary depending on both the nature of the information and students’ expertise levels. The present study used Cognitive Load Theory and tested a hypothesis that the sequential order of having investigations first followed by explicit instruction only is more effective than that of having explicit instruction first when students have acquired sufficiently high levels of knowledge. This hypothesis was tested with 85 middle school students. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two sequences. The results indicated that for sufficiently knowledgeable students, a dis-ordinal knowledge level by sequence interaction was obtained. There was an advantage to the explicit instruction first approach for the less knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was high but an advantage to the investigation first approach for the more knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was lower. The results were discussed in light of a recent debate between the authors and De Jong et al. ( Educational Research Review , 39, 1-14, 2023 ) on the role of investigations and explicit instruction in science education.
The use of investigations in science teaching is both common and commonly advocated for in science education literature. We suggest that the use of investigations should differ depending on the complexity of the subject matter. That complexity can vary depending on both the nature of the information and students’ expertise levels. The present study used Cognitive Load Theory and tested a hypothesis that the sequential order of having investigations first followed by explicit instruction only is more effective than that of having explicit instruction first when students have acquired sufficiently high levels of knowledge. This hypothesis was tested with 85 middle school students. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two sequences. The results indicated that for sufficiently knowledgeable students, a dis-ordinal knowledge level by sequence interaction was obtained. There was an advantage to the explicit instruction first approach for the less knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was high but an advantage to the investigation first approach for the more knowledgeable students for whom element interactivity was lower. The results were discussed in light of a recent debate between the authors and De Jong et al. (Educational Research Review, 39, 1-14, 2023) on the role of investigations and explicit instruction in science education.
Author Zhang, Lin
Sweller, John
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Lin
  orcidid: 0000-0002-8421-879X
  surname: Zhang
  fullname: Zhang, Lin
  email: lzhang@providence.edu
  organization: Department of Elementary & Special Education, Providence College
– sequence: 2
  givenname: John
  surname: Sweller
  fullname: Sweller, John
  organization: School of Education, University of New South Wales
BookMark eNp9UcFOJCEUJBtNdtT9gT2R7Bl90NDd7M0Y3Z3ExIueCUO_VkxLj8Co8zN-q_TMqIkHT1BJVb16rw7IXhgDEvKbwzEHaE4SB8EFAyFZgVoz9YPMeNu0TPFG7pEZCFUzoUX7kxykdA8gqkpXM_I6DynHlct-DHagCR9XGBwm6gNNzk9_mtG6Ox9u_1I3huQ7jAVQHPABQy7EjNEWgyef1_T5DsO7y8Ty4XHl45otbMKuoCdM2d_aaRzdiXyZZkNH8WU5eOcnx49IR2S_t0PCX7v3kNxcnF-f_WeXV__mZ6eXzFVcZ9ZLKcHV1jkudNdiLWUNUtfIbVuhbfq27YRcKL1opBISVGOFctBZqG0PFqtD8mfru4xjyZ6yuR9XsVwkmYoLaEBpIQur3bJcHFOK2JsSd7NLjtYPhoOZ2jDbNkxpw2zaMKpIxRfpMvoHG9ffi6qtKC2nk2P8TPWN6g2MLqQv
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1007_s11251_025_09706_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11251_024_09665_9
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_025_09993_3
Cites_doi 10.1007/s11251-013-9282-5
10.1207/s1532690xci2202_1
10.1080/10508406.2011.591717
10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3
10.1037/edu0000018
10.1007/s10648-016-9359-1
10.1007/s11251-012-9248-z
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.03.007
10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.004
10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5
10.1007/s10648-019-09500-5
10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5
10.1007/s11251-009-9093-x
10.1007/s10648-023-09782-w
10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.009
10.3886/ICPSR35626.v1
10.1007/s10648-021-09646-1
10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.12.003
10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.001
10.1111/bjep.12035
10.1080/07370000802212669
10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
10.1002/tea.21909
10.1016/j.edurev.2023.100536
10.1016/j.edurev.2023.100584
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Instituto Universitário de Ciências Psicológicas, Sociais e da Vida 2024. Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
Copyright_xml – notice: Instituto Universitário de Ciências Psicológicas, Sociais e da Vida 2024. Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
AHOVV
DOI 10.1007/s10212-024-00799-5
DatabaseName CrossRef
Education Research Index
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
DatabaseTitleList

DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Education
EISSN 1878-5174
EndPage 3801
ExternalDocumentID 10_1007_s10212_024_00799_5
GroupedDBID -EM
-W8
.GO
0-V
06D
0R~
0S8
0VY
1N0
2.D
203
2KG
2LR
2VQ
30V
3V.
4.4
406
408
40D
53G
5GY
67Z
8A4
8TC
95.
96X
AACDK
AAEWM
AAGJD
AAHNG
AAHSB
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANTL
AANZL
AARHV
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABBHK
ABCFB
ABDBF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABQBU
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABUWG
ABXPI
ABXSQ
ACAOD
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACOKC
ACPIV
ACUHS
ACZOJ
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADNHR
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADULT
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETCA
AEUPB
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFDYH
AFDYV
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFQWF
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AIKWM
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
ANMIH
AOCGG
ARALO
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
B0M
BBWZM
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
CAG
CCPQU
CJNVE
CMRWG
COF
CSCUP
DDRTE
DNIVK
DPUIP
DU5
DWQXO
EAD
EAP
EAS
EBLON
EBS
EDJ
EIOEI
EJD
EMK
EPS
ESBYG
ESX
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FYJPI
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GQ6
GQ7
H13
HF~
HG6
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
IKXTQ
IPSME
IWAJR
IXD
J-C
J0Z
JAAYA
JBMMH
JBSCW
JENOY
JHFFW
JKQEH
JLEZI
JLXEF
JPL
JSODD
JST
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M0P
M4Y
N9A
NB0
NDZJH
NF0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9J
P2P
P9Q
PQEDU
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSYQQ
PT4
R9I
RHV
ROL
RPD
RSV
S1Z
S27
S3B
SA0
SCLPG
SDA
SHS
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
STPWE
T13
TSG
U2A
UG4
UOJIU
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
W48
WK8
Z45
ZMTXR
ZWUKE
~8M
~A9
AAPKM
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFOHR
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
ATHPR
AYFIA
CITATION
PHGZM
PHGZT
ABRTQ
AHOVV
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c319t-f4440c6acc129d8e64460496e1a83ea7f88d24b59b74524057a25c0da06af0ae3
IEDL.DBID AGYKE
ISSN 0256-2928
IngestDate Fri Jul 25 09:10:13 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:55:31 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:31:11 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:37:19 EST 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Keywords Investigation activities
Element interactivity, Instructional sequence
Explicit instruction
Cognitive Load Theory
Language English
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c319t-f4440c6acc129d8e64460496e1a83ea7f88d24b59b74524057a25c0da06af0ae3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
ORCID 0000-0002-8421-879X
PQID 3120705924
PQPubID 2034548
PageCount 11
ParticipantIDs proquest_journals_3120705924
crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s10212_024_00799_5
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10212_024_00799_5
springer_journals_10_1007_s10212_024_00799_5
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 20241200
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-12-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 12
  year: 2024
  text: 20241200
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace Dordrecht
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Dordrecht
PublicationSubtitle A Journal of Education and Development
PublicationTitle European journal of psychology of education
PublicationTitleAbbrev Eur J Psychol Educ
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher Springer Netherlands
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer Netherlands
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References DeCaro, Rittle-Johnson (CR6) 2012; 113
Kapur (CR14) 2010; 38
Sweller, van Merriënboer, Paas (CR24) 2019; 31
Matlen, Klahr (CR18) 2013; 41
Glogger-Frey, Fleischer, Grüny, Kappich, Renkl (CR8) 2015; 39
Chen, Kalyuga, Sweller (CR2) 2015; 107
Chen, Kalyuga, Sweller (CR3) 2017; 29
Sweller (CR22) 2010; 22
Sweller, Ayres, Kalyuga (CR23) 2010
Weaver, Chastain, DeCaro, DeCaro (CR27) 2018; 52
De Jong, Lazonder, Chinn, Fischer, Gobert, Hmelo-Silver, Koedinger, Krajcik, Kyza, Linn, Pedaste, Scheiter, Zacharia (CR5) 2023; 39
Jacobson, Markauskaite, Portolese, Kapur, Lai, Roberts (CR10) 2017; 52
Hsu, Kalyuga, Sweller (CR9) 2015; 3
Fyfe, DeCaro, Rittle-Johnson (CR7) 2014; 84
Loibl, Rummel (CR17) 2014; 42
Kapur, Bielaczyc (CR15) 2012; 21
Kirschner, Sweller, Clark (CR16) 2006; 41
Schwartz, Martin (CR20) 2004; 22
CR25
Ashman, Kalyuga, Sweller (CR1) 2020; 32
Kapur (CR13) 2008; 26
CR21
Zhang, Kirschner, Cobern, Sweller (CR28) 2022; 34
Kalyuga (CR11) 2007; 19
van Gog, Kester, Paas (CR26) 2011; 36
Chen, Paas, Sweller (CR4) 2023; 35
Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, Sweller (CR12) 2003; 38
(CR19) 2013
O Chen (799_CR3) 2017; 29
S Kalyuga (799_CR11) 2007; 19
C-Y Hsu (799_CR9) 2015; 3
K Loibl (799_CR17) 2014; 42
BJ Matlen (799_CR18) 2013; 41
J Sweller (799_CR24) 2019; 31
M Kapur (799_CR14) 2010; 38
799_CR21
MJ Jacobson (799_CR10) 2017; 52
799_CR25
P Kirschner (799_CR16) 2006; 41
T van Gog (799_CR26) 2011; 36
G Ashman (799_CR1) 2020; 32
ER Fyfe (799_CR7) 2014; 84
I Glogger-Frey (799_CR8) 2015; 39
J Sweller (799_CR23) 2010
NGSS Lead States (799_CR19) 2013
L Zhang (799_CR28) 2022; 34
M Kapur (799_CR13) 2008; 26
O Chen (799_CR4) 2023; 35
S Kalyuga (799_CR12) 2003; 38
M Kapur (799_CR15) 2012; 21
T De Jong (799_CR5) 2023; 39
J Sweller (799_CR22) 2010; 22
O Chen (799_CR2) 2015; 107
MS DeCaro (799_CR6) 2012; 113
JP Weaver (799_CR27) 2018; 52
DL Schwartz (799_CR20) 2004; 22
References_xml – volume: 42
  start-page: 305
  year: 2014
  end-page: 326
  ident: CR17
  article-title: The impact of guidance during problem-solving prior to instruction on students’ inventions and learning outcomes
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-013-9282-5
– volume: 22
  start-page: 129
  issue: 2
  year: 2004
  end-page: 184
  ident: CR20
  article-title: Inventing to prepare for future learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction
  publication-title: Cognition and Instruction
  doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci2202_1
– volume: 21
  start-page: 45
  issue: 1
  year: 2012
  end-page: 83
  ident: CR15
  article-title: Designing for productive failure
  publication-title: Journal of the Learning Sciences
  doi: 10.1080/10508406.2011.591717
– volume: 19
  start-page: 509
  year: 2007
  end-page: 539
  ident: CR11
  article-title: Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3
– volume: 107
  start-page: 689
  issue: 3
  year: 2015
  end-page: 704
  ident: CR2
  article-title: The worked example effect, the generation effect, and element interactivity
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/edu0000018
– volume: 29
  start-page: 393
  year: 2017
  end-page: 405
  ident: CR3
  article-title: The expertise reversal effect is a variant of the more general element interactivity effect
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-016-9359-1
– volume: 41
  start-page: 621
  issue: 3
  year: 2013
  end-page: 634
  ident: CR18
  article-title: Sequential effects of high and low instructional guidance on children’s acquisition of experimentation skills: Is it all in the timing?
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-012-9248-z
– volume: 52
  start-page: 1
  year: 2017
  end-page: 14
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Designs for learning about climate change as a complex system
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.03.007
– volume: 36
  start-page: 212
  issue: 3
  year: 2011
  end-page: 218
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example pairs on novices’ learning
  publication-title: Contemporary Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.004
– volume: 22
  start-page: 123
  year: 2010
  end-page: 138
  ident: CR22
  article-title: Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5
– volume: 32
  start-page: 229
  year: 2020
  end-page: 247
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Problem-solving or explicit instruction: Which should go first when element interactivity is high?
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09500-5
– ident: CR25
– volume: 31
  start-page: 261
  issue: 2
  year: 2019
  end-page: 292
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 Years Later
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5
– ident: CR21
– volume: 38
  start-page: 523
  year: 2010
  end-page: 550
  ident: CR14
  article-title: Productive failure in mathematical problem solving
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9093-x
– year: 2010
  ident: CR23
  publication-title: Cognitive load theory
– year: 2013
  ident: CR19
– volume: 35
  start-page: 63
  year: 2023
  ident: CR4
  article-title: A cognitive load theory approach to defining and measuring task complexity through element interactivity
  publication-title:  Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-023-09782-w
– volume: 113
  start-page: 552
  issue: 4
  year: 2012
  end-page: 568
  ident: CR6
  article-title: Exploring mathematics problems prepares children to learn from instruction
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.009
– volume: 3
  start-page: 37
  issue: 1
  year: 2015
  end-page: 53
  ident: CR9
  article-title: When should guidance be presented during physics instruction?
  publication-title: Archives of Scientific Psychology
  doi: 10.3886/ICPSR35626.v1
– volume: 34
  start-page: 1157
  issue: 2
  year: 2022
  end-page: 1176
  ident: CR28
  article-title: There is an evidence crisis in science educational policy
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-021-09646-1
– volume: 39
  start-page: 1
  year: 2023
  end-page: 14
  ident: CR5
  article-title: Let’s talk evidence – the case for combining inquiry-based and direct instruction
  publication-title: Educational Research Review
– volume: 52
  start-page: 36
  year: 2018
  end-page: 47
  ident: CR27
  article-title: Reverse the routine: problem solving before instruction improves conceptual knowledge in undergraduate physics
  publication-title: Contemporary Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.12.003
– volume: 38
  start-page: 23
  year: 2003
  end-page: 31
  ident: CR12
  article-title: The expertise reversal effect
  publication-title: Educational Psychologist
  doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4
– volume: 39
  start-page: 72
  year: 2015
  end-page: 87
  ident: CR8
  article-title: Inventing a solution and studying a worked solution prepare differently for learning from direct instruction
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.001
– volume: 84
  start-page: 502
  issue: 3
  year: 2014
  end-page: 519
  ident: CR7
  article-title: An alternative time for telling: When conceptual instruction prior to problem solving improves mathematical knowledge
  publication-title: British Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1111/bjep.12035
– volume: 26
  start-page: 379
  year: 2008
  end-page: 424
  ident: CR13
  article-title: Productive failure
  publication-title: Cognition and Instruction
  doi: 10.1080/07370000802212669
– volume: 41
  start-page: 75
  year: 2006
  end-page: 86
  ident: CR16
  article-title: Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential and inquiry-based teaching
  publication-title: Educational Psychologist
  doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
– volume: 38
  start-page: 23
  year: 2003
  ident: 799_CR12
  publication-title: Educational Psychologist
  doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4
– volume: 26
  start-page: 379
  year: 2008
  ident: 799_CR13
  publication-title: Cognition and Instruction
  doi: 10.1080/07370000802212669
– volume: 41
  start-page: 621
  issue: 3
  year: 2013
  ident: 799_CR18
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-012-9248-z
– ident: 799_CR21
  doi: 10.1002/tea.21909
– volume: 52
  start-page: 1
  year: 2017
  ident: 799_CR10
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.03.007
– volume: 39
  start-page: 72
  year: 2015
  ident: 799_CR8
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.001
– volume: 3
  start-page: 37
  issue: 1
  year: 2015
  ident: 799_CR9
  publication-title: Archives of Scientific Psychology
  doi: 10.3886/ICPSR35626.v1
– volume: 22
  start-page: 123
  year: 2010
  ident: 799_CR22
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5
– volume: 29
  start-page: 393
  year: 2017
  ident: 799_CR3
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-016-9359-1
– volume: 38
  start-page: 523
  year: 2010
  ident: 799_CR14
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9093-x
– volume: 84
  start-page: 502
  issue: 3
  year: 2014
  ident: 799_CR7
  publication-title: British Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1111/bjep.12035
– volume-title: Next generation science standards: For states, by states
  year: 2013
  ident: 799_CR19
– volume: 113
  start-page: 552
  issue: 4
  year: 2012
  ident: 799_CR6
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.009
– volume: 22
  start-page: 129
  issue: 2
  year: 2004
  ident: 799_CR20
  publication-title: Cognition and Instruction
  doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci2202_1
– volume: 36
  start-page: 212
  issue: 3
  year: 2011
  ident: 799_CR26
  publication-title: Contemporary Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.004
– volume: 52
  start-page: 36
  year: 2018
  ident: 799_CR27
  publication-title: Contemporary Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.12.003
– volume: 41
  start-page: 75
  year: 2006
  ident: 799_CR16
  publication-title: Educational Psychologist
  doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
– volume-title: Cognitive load theory
  year: 2010
  ident: 799_CR23
– volume: 34
  start-page: 1157
  issue: 2
  year: 2022
  ident: 799_CR28
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-021-09646-1
– volume: 35
  start-page: 63
  year: 2023
  ident: 799_CR4
  publication-title:  Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-023-09782-w
– volume: 39
  start-page: 1
  year: 2023
  ident: 799_CR5
  publication-title: Educational Research Review
  doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2023.100536
– volume: 21
  start-page: 45
  issue: 1
  year: 2012
  ident: 799_CR15
  publication-title: Journal of the Learning Sciences
  doi: 10.1080/10508406.2011.591717
– volume: 31
  start-page: 261
  issue: 2
  year: 2019
  ident: 799_CR24
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5
– volume: 42
  start-page: 305
  year: 2014
  ident: 799_CR17
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-013-9282-5
– volume: 32
  start-page: 229
  year: 2020
  ident: 799_CR1
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09500-5
– ident: 799_CR25
  doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2023.100584
– volume: 19
  start-page: 509
  year: 2007
  ident: 799_CR11
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3
– volume: 107
  start-page: 689
  issue: 3
  year: 2015
  ident: 799_CR2
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/edu0000018
SSID ssj0023393
Score 2.380256
Snippet The use of investigations in science teaching is both common and commonly advocated for in science education literature. We suggest that the use of...
SourceID proquest
crossref
springer
SourceType Aggregation Database
Enrichment Source
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 3791
SubjectTerms Cognitive load
Direct Instruction
Education
Educational Policy
Educational Psychology
Inquiry method
Instructional Materials
Memory
Meta-analysis
Pedagogic Psychology
Science education
Students
Teaching
Title Instructional sequences in science teaching: considering element interactivity when sequencing inquiry-based investigation activities and explicit instruction
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10212-024-00799-5
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3120705924
Volume 39
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8QwEB50vXjxLa4vcvCmkW4e3dTbIusTPbmgp5KmCS5KV3BF8cf4W520aVdFhT2GToY0r5kh33wDsMeNcwbDLsqEzagwSUa19U-IIo9czqXjZRWFq-v4bCAubuVtSAp7rtHu9ZNkeVN_SXZjHkbABMVmklA5C3OyoxLVgrne6d1lvwm0OK_IdtGcU5YwFZJlftfy3SBNvMwfD6OlvTlZhEE90gpm8nD4Ms4OzfsPEsdpf2UJFoIDSnrVjlmGGVus-NrNAeexCh_nE1JZFGyw1mRYkGAvyThgMI-ICQU_sUFsBUUnnoKiTL7ydSnI670tai1ealiBj6m3nzm2Gp6PUUFCJ4zeiS5yYt_86_rQa2yGtAaDk_7N8RkNVRyoweM9pk4IEZlYG4OuRa4sOmAxhiWx7WjFre46pXImMplkXSGZ9x81kybKdRRrF2nL16FVjAq7AcQIzXViVJf7_GGHoZrSSnDV9STyTLk2dOqlTE2gOPeVNh7TCTmzn_kUZz4tZz6Vbdhv-jxVBB__Sm_XOyQNh_055R2GF6fESLYNB_WCTz7_rW1zOvEtmGd-z5Rgmm1o4cTbHXSJxtluOAG7MDtgvU-ZewVs
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LT8MwDLZgO8CFN2I8c-AGQV2Sdik3hBgbj52YBKcqTRMxgQoSQyB-DL8Vp007QIDEMWpipXnZlj9_Btjl2lqNbhdlwqRU6DilyrgQosgCm_HQ8qKKwuUg6g3F2XV47ZPCniq0exWSLF7qT8luzMEImKDYjGMaTkNToA8eNKB5dHpzflI7WpyXZLuozimLmfTJMj9L-aqQJlbmt8BooW-68zCsZlrCTO4OnsfpgX77RuL4319ZgDlvgJKj8sQswpTJl1ztZo_zWIb3_oRUFjvWWGsyyonXl2TsMZiHRPuCn9ggpoSiE0dBUSRfuboU5OXW5JUU12tUgo-p058Ztmqej4ec-EHovROVZ8S8uuj6yEmsp7QCw-7J1XGP-ioOVOP1HlMrhAh0pLRG0yKTBg2wCN2SyLSV5EZ1rJQZE2kYpx0RMmc_KhbqIFNBpGygDF-FRv6QmzUgWiiuYi073OUPW3TVpJKCy44jkWfStqBdbWWiPcW5q7Rxn0zImd3KJ7jySbHySdiCvXrMY0nw8WfvzeqEJP6yPyW8zfDhDNGTbcF-teGTz79LW_9f9x2Y6V1dXiQX_cH5Bswyd34KYM0mNHATzBaaR-N029-GD6m5B2A
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8QwEA66gngRn7g-c_CmwW6Stqm3RV12feHBBW8lTRNckK7ggv4af6szbdqqqOAxdDKUTtKZYb75hpBDYZwzkHYxLm3GpEkypi2WEGUeuFyETpRTFG5uo-FYXj6ED5-6-Eu0e12SrHoakKWpmJ085-7kU-MbR0gBlwyWScLCebIAv-MenvQx7zcplxAV7S44dsYTrnzbzM86vrqmNt78ViItPc9ghSz7kJH2KxuvkjlbrOG0ZY_MWCfvo5YGFgQbdDSdFNR7ODrzqMlTavyITlhQW4HHKZJGlO1SOEmCvj7aotaCUpMKLszQ4-Wwapg5pgX1myDfprrIqX3DevgENTavtEHGg4v7syHzcxeYgQs5Y05KGZhIGwPBQK4shEwRJBKR7WklrI6dUjmXWZhksQw5RnyahybIdRBpF2grNkmnmBZ2i1AjtdCJUbHAjl8HyZXSSgoVI-07V65LevUnT40nJcfZGE9pS6eMZkrBTGlppjTskqNmz3NFyfGn9G5tydRfz5dU9Dj86kLIPbvkuLZu-_h3bdv_Ez8gi3fng_R6dHu1Q5Y4nrUSCbNLOmADuwfxzCzbL4_sB-XN7ps
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Instructional+sequences+in+science+teaching%3A+considering+element+interactivity+when+sequencing+inquiry-based+investigation+activities+and+explicit+instruction&rft.jtitle=European+journal+of+psychology+of+education&rft.au=Zhang%2C+Lin&rft.au=Sweller%2C+John&rft.date=2024-12-01&rft.pub=Springer+Netherlands&rft.issn=0256-2928&rft.eissn=1878-5174&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=3791&rft.epage=3801&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs10212-024-00799-5&rft.externalDocID=10_1007_s10212_024_00799_5
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0256-2928&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0256-2928&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0256-2928&client=summon