Human Eye-Head Gaze Shifts in a Distractor Task. II. Reduced Threshold for Initiation of Early Head Movements
MRC Group in Sensory-Motor Neuroscience, Department of Physiology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada Corneil, Brian D. and Douglas P. Munoz. Human Eye-Head Gaze Shifts in a Distractor Task. II. Reduced Threshold for Initiation of Early Head Movements. J. Neurophysiol. 82: 14...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of neurophysiology Vol. 82; no. 3; pp. 1406 - 1421 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Am Phys Soc
01.09.1999
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | MRC Group in Sensory-Motor Neuroscience, Department of Physiology,
Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
Corneil, Brian D. and
Douglas P. Munoz.
Human Eye-Head Gaze Shifts in a Distractor Task. II. Reduced
Threshold for Initiation of Early Head Movements. J. Neurophysiol. 82: 1406-1421, 1999. This study was
motivated by the observation of early head movements
(EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were
presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying
stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same
location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically
opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the
target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs
directed either to the side of the target ( correct EHMs)
or the side of the distractor ( incorrect EHMs). During
EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze
stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5° in amplitude and
reached peak velocities generally <50°/s. These metrics increased
for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs
initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements.
These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that
initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor.
Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction
latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting
that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay
gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction
analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous
paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a
distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM.
A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can
initiate an incorrect EHM. |
---|---|
AbstractList | This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements (EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs directed either to the side of the target (correct EHMs) or the side of the distractor (incorrect EHMs). During EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5 degrees in amplitude and reached peak velocities generally <50 degrees /s. These metrics increased for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements. These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor. Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM. A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can initiate an incorrect EHM.This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements (EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs directed either to the side of the target (correct EHMs) or the side of the distractor (incorrect EHMs). During EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5 degrees in amplitude and reached peak velocities generally <50 degrees /s. These metrics increased for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements. These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor. Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM. A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can initiate an incorrect EHM. This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements (EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs directed either to the side of the target (correct EHMs) or the side of the distractor (incorrect EHMs). During EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5 degrees in amplitude and reached peak velocities generally <50 degrees /s. These metrics increased for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements. These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor. Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM. A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can initiate an incorrect EHM. MRC Group in Sensory-Motor Neuroscience, Department of Physiology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada Corneil, Brian D. and Douglas P. Munoz. Human Eye-Head Gaze Shifts in a Distractor Task. II. Reduced Threshold for Initiation of Early Head Movements. J. Neurophysiol. 82: 1406-1421, 1999. This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements (EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs directed either to the side of the target ( correct EHMs) or the side of the distractor ( incorrect EHMs). During EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5° in amplitude and reached peak velocities generally <50°/s. These metrics increased for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements. These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor. Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM. A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can initiate an incorrect EHM. This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements(EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs directed either to the side of the target ( correct EHMs) or the side of the distractor ( incorrect EHMs). During EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5° in amplitude and reached peak velocities generally <50°/s. These metrics increased for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements. These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor. Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM. A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can initiate an incorrect EHM. This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements (EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a visual or auditory target, along with an accompanying stimulus of the other modality, that either appeared at the same location as the target (enhancer condition) or at the diametrically opposite location (distractor condition). Gaze shifts generated to the target in the distractor condition sometimes were preceded by EHMs directed either to the side of the target (correct EHMs) or the side of the distractor (incorrect EHMs). During EHMs, the eyes performed compensatory eye movements to keep gaze stable. Incorrect EHMs were usually between 1 and 5 degree in amplitude and reached peak velocities generally <50 degree /s. These metrics increased for more eccentric distractors. The dynamics of incorrect EHMs initially followed a trajectory typical of much larger head movements. These results suggest that incorrect EHMs are head movements that initially were planned to orient to the peripheral distractor. Furthermore gaze shifts preceded by incorrect EHMs had longer reaction latencies than gaze shifts not preceded by incorrect EHMs, suggesting that the processes leading to incorrect EHMs also serve to delay gaze-shift initiation. These results demonstrate a form of distraction analogous to the incorrect gaze shifts (IGSs) described in the previous paper and suggest that a motor program encoding a gaze shift to a distractor is capable of initiating either an IGS or an incorrect EHM. A neural program not strong enough to initiate an IGS nevertheless can initiate an incorrect EHM. |
Author | Munoz, Douglas P Corneil, Brian D |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 fullname: Corneil, Brian D – sequence: 2 fullname: Munoz, Douglas P |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10482758$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkc1u1DAURi1URKeFB2CDvIJVgn-SOF6iMu2MVIQEw9ryJDeNh8QebAcIT19PpxSEBKxs2efcK33fGTqxzgJCzynJKS3Z653NqZQyr1nOc1qQ6hFapHeW0VLWJ2hBSLpzIsQpOgthRwgRJWFP0CklRc1EWS_QuJpGbfFyhmwFusVX-gfgj73pYsDGYo3fmhC9bqLzeKPD5xyv1zn-AO3UQIs3vYfQu6HFXfpfWxONjsZZ7Dq81H6Y8d3Qd-4rjGBjeIoed3oI8Oz-PEefLpebi1V2_f5qffHmOms4lTHTmrCa646RqmCcclJtu1JQKCuqdS23dVk0lGqmJW1lseVMF5UUICTljEO35efo5XHu3rsvE4SoRhMaGAZtwU1BCUIKImT5X5AKXleiLBL44h6ctiO0au_NqP2sfiaZAHoEGu9C8ND9hqhDW2pn1aEtVTPF1aGt5Ig_nMbEuwBT5Gb4p8mPZm9u-m_Gg9r3czBucDezupyGYQPfY_IeDLVvu2S9-ruVlvyafwuBmLeu |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_0911902107 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00460_2017 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_01171_2004 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_01188_2011 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_90223_2008 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_01_00224_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0896_6273_04_00267_3 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00222_2012 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2007_11_014 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_26479_2 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_020_05905_9 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1460_9568_2010_07496_x crossref_primary_10_1007_s10162_013_0401_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2005_10_029 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_005_0036_8 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_90815_2008 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0042_6989_01_00158_4 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00422_006_0049_9 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_2002_88_4_2000 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00495_2010 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/BF00234137 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08566.1997 10.1152/jn.1990.64.6.1873 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90222-0 10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00007-0 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012650 10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1642 10.1016/0957-4271(95)00012-7 10.1007/BF00230001 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.332 10.1152/jn.1997.78.4.2221 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2313 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1981.tb30899.x 10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.509 10.1152/jn.1994.72.6.2648 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.511 10.1007/BF00230391 10.1038/355542a0 10.1152/jn.1984.52.6.1030 10.1152/jn.1988.59.3.997 10.1126/science.171.3966.82 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90003-2 10.1152/jn.1994.72.6.2665 10.1152/jn.1987.58.3.427 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2328 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016043 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-17-07015.1998 10.1007/BF00237071 10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.766 10.1152/jn.1995.73.4.1632 10.1007/BF00229557 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1108 10.1016/S0957-4271(96)00181-4 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90238-7 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90439-0 10.1007/BF00234475 10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00034-2 10.1152/jn.1993.70.2.559 10.1152/jn.1999.81.3.1284 10.1017/S0952523800002911 10.1152/jn.1986.56.6.1542 10.1007/BF00243222 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017689 10.1152/jn.1987.57.1.35 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2558 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1390 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1669 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1597 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019653 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90286-X 10.1007/PL00005663 10.3109/00016488009131715 10.1152/jn.1998.79.6.3060 10.1152/jn.1996.76.2.927 10.1126/science.2003221 10.1152/jn.1987.57.1.22 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90169-8 10.1152/jn.1980.43.1.207 10.1007/BF00241974 10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1624 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90071-X 10.1080/01616412.1993.11740176 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90228-X 10.1007/BF00230204 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1193 10.1126/science.173.3995.452 10.1126/science.152.3729.1644 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-24-08193.1996 10.1007/BF00237182 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90104-7 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1406 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE CrossRef Neurosciences Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1522-1598 |
EndPage | 1421 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10482758 10_1152_jn_1999_82_3_1406 jn_82_3_1406 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Comparative Study |
GroupedDBID | - 08R 0VX 1Z7 2WC 39C 3O- 41 53G 55 5GY 5VS AALRV ABFLS ABIVO ABPTK ABUFD ABZEH ACGFS ACNCT ADACO ADBBV ADBIT ADKLL AENEX AETEA AFFNX ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BAWUL C1A CS3 DIK DL DU5 DZ E3Z EBS EJD F5P FH7 FRP GJ GX1 H~9 KQ8 L7B MVM NEJ O0- OHT OK1 P2P RAP RHF RHI RPL SJN UHB UPT UQL VH1 WH7 WOQ WOW X X7M ZA5 ZGI ZXP ZY4 --- -DZ -~X .55 .GJ 18M 1CY 41~ AAYXX ABCQX ABHWK ABJNI ABKWE ACGFO ADFNX ADHGD ADIYS AFOSN AI. AIZAD BKKCC BTFSW CITATION EMOBN H13 ITBOX RPRKH TR2 W8F XSW YBH YQT YSK 29L 4.4 8M5 ABTAH CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM VXZ XJT XOL 7TK 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c319t-aa0283af2064231306bf571e561aa89b854c11a2a91d94b32a4697e791323efb3 |
ISSN | 0022-3077 |
IngestDate | Thu Jul 10 23:46:32 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 09:37:47 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:41:02 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:17 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:33:46 EDT 2025 Tue Jan 05 17:56:56 EST 2021 Mon May 06 12:29:12 EDT 2019 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 3 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c319t-aa0283af2064231306bf571e561aa89b854c11a2a91d94b32a4697e791323efb3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 |
PMID | 10482758 |
PQID | 17386754 |
PQPubID | 23462 |
PageCount | 16 |
ParticipantIDs | crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_1999_82_3_1406 crossref_citationtrail_10_1152_jn_1999_82_3_1406 pubmed_primary_10482758 proquest_miscellaneous_70040795 highwire_physiology_jn_82_3_1406 proquest_miscellaneous_17386754 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 1900 |
PublicationDate | 1999-Sep |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 1999-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 1999 text: 1999-Sep |
PublicationDecade | 1990 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Journal of neurophysiology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Neurophysiol |
PublicationYear | 1999 |
Publisher | Am Phys Soc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Am Phys Soc |
References | B20 B21 B65 B22 B66 B23 B67 B24 B68 B25 B26 Whittington D. A. (B81) 1981; 41 Istvan P. J. (B40) 1994; 20 B28 B29 B71 B73 B30 B74 B31 B32 B76 B77 B34 B78 B35 B79 B36 B37 B38 B39 B1 B2 B3 B4 B5 Corneil B. D. (B12) 1998; 24 B9 Büttner-Ennever J. A. (B6) 1997; 23 B80 B82 B83 B84 B41 B85 B42 B86 B43 B44 B45 B46 B47 B48 Corneil B. D. (B10) 1997; 75 B49 Corneil B. D. (B11) 1998; 3 Corneil B. D. (B13) 1995; 21 B50 B51 B52 B53 B54 B55 B56 B57 B14 B58 B15 B59 B16 B17 B18 B19 Corneil B. D. (B8) 1997; 23 B60 B61 B62 B63 |
References_xml | – volume: 20 start-page: 141 year: 1994 ident: B40 publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – ident: B17 doi: 10.1007/BF00234137 – ident: B20 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08566.1997 – ident: B73 doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.6.1873 – ident: B3 doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90222-0 – ident: B25 doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00007-0 – ident: B1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012650 – ident: B53 doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1642 – ident: B28 doi: 10.1016/0957-4271(95)00012-7 – ident: B65 doi: 10.1007/BF00230001 – ident: B68 doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.332 – volume: 75 start-page: Avi year: 1997 ident: B10 publication-title: Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. Abstr. – volume: 23 start-page: 756 year: 1997 ident: B8 publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – ident: B30 doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.4.2221 – volume: 24 start-page: 417 year: 1998 ident: B12 publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – ident: B57 doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2313 – volume: 21 start-page: 1197 year: 1995 ident: B13 publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – ident: B84 doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1981.tb30899.x – volume: 23 start-page: 1296 year: 1997 ident: B6 publication-title: Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. – ident: B38 doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.509 – ident: B15 doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.6.2648 – ident: B21 doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.511 – ident: B26 doi: 10.1007/BF00230391 – ident: B31 doi: 10.1038/355542a0 – ident: B37 doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.52.6.1030 – ident: B61 doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.3.997 – ident: B82 doi: 10.1126/science.171.3966.82 – ident: B71 doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90003-2 – ident: B16 doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.6.2665 – ident: B39 doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.3.427 – ident: B22 doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2328 – volume: 41 start-page: 358 year: 1981 ident: B81 publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. – ident: B44 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016043 – ident: B19 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-17-07015.1998 – ident: B67 doi: 10.1007/BF00237071 – ident: B76 doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.766 – ident: B63 doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.4.1632 – ident: B32 doi: 10.1007/BF00229557 – ident: B80 doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1108 – ident: B83 doi: 10.1016/S0957-4271(96)00181-4 – ident: B36 doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90238-7 – ident: B43 doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90439-0 – ident: B48 doi: 10.1007/BF00234475 – ident: B50 doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00034-2 – ident: B56 doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.2.559 – ident: B62 doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.3.1284 – ident: B45 doi: 10.1017/S0952523800002911 – ident: B74 doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.6.1542 – ident: B58 doi: 10.1007/BF00243222 – ident: B60 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017689 – ident: B42 doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.57.1.35 – ident: B18 doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2558 – ident: B9 doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1390 – ident: B23 doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1669 – ident: B47 doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1597 – ident: B66 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019653 – ident: B29 doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90286-X – ident: B34 doi: 10.1007/PL00005663 – ident: B77 doi: 10.3109/00016488009131715 – ident: B59 doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.6.3060 – ident: B24 doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.2.927 – volume: 3 start-page: E1 year: 1998 ident: B11 publication-title: Neural Control Move. Abstr. – ident: B55 doi: 10.1126/science.2003221 – ident: B41 doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.57.1.22 – ident: B85 doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90169-8 – ident: B46 doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.1.207 – ident: B51 doi: 10.1007/BF00241974 – ident: B52 doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1624 – ident: B78 doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90071-X – ident: B49 doi: 10.1080/01616412.1993.11740176 – ident: B86 doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90228-X – ident: B79 doi: 10.1007/BF00230204 – ident: B54 doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1193 – ident: B5 doi: 10.1126/science.173.3995.452 – ident: B2 doi: 10.1126/science.152.3729.1644 – ident: B14 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-24-08193.1996 – ident: B35 doi: 10.1007/BF00237182 – ident: B4 doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90104-7 |
SSID | ssj0007502 |
Score | 1.7288134 |
Snippet | MRC Group in Sensory-Motor Neuroscience, Department of Physiology,
Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
Corneil, Brian D. and
Douglas P.... This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements(EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a... This study was motivated by the observation of early head movements (EHMs) occasionally generated before gaze shifts. Human subjects were presented with a... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref highwire |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 1406 |
SubjectTerms | Attention - physiology Differential Threshold Eye Movements - physiology Fixation, Ocular - physiology Head - physiology Humans Movement - physiology Reaction Time - physiology Time Factors |
Title | Human Eye-Head Gaze Shifts in a Distractor Task. II. Reduced Threshold for Initiation of Early Head Movements |
URI | http://jn.physiology.org/cgi/content/abstract/82/3/1406 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10482758 https://www.proquest.com/docview/17386754 https://www.proquest.com/docview/70040795 |
Volume | 82 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Pb9MwFLZgXLggYPwoP31AHCgJiR0n8XEM0AYCgdRJu1l2Yo8OmlRtetj-ep4dJ2lHi4BLVCWOk_r78vSe7fc9hF7kihFVlkXAODVBwkgR8CjXQcESrblKmZJO7fNLenSSfDxlp129e59d0qiwuNyaV_I_qMI5wNVmyf4Dsn2ncAJ-A75wBITh-FcYtzPw-kIHYFLL8Zm81OPl96lp3CZXaVdfmoUrqDNu5PJHOD4-DmFAy5Vd9G8AxaVdfHI7Dad2E1HvPmone-w6ndVOUbwVfNrixjpBTDc_sjFBf1gvKj-__HbYVfx5VdVuxvqdzysrfQLesKFqY_9_5IuveBuakzWu0DWDCPFbut1SM6v8el7ZdEkAloQ0vNoWBns-c9BBzJiTrNV4vyKP3V26jm4QiBSsqfv0bRCMB4doEIyHl_YL2_D0N7892wrI-t42vZROOXp3FOK8kcltdMuPPz5oOXEHXdPVXbR_UMmmnl3gl_hrD8g-mjma4I4m2NIEtzTB0wpLPNAEO5pgoAn2NME9TTDQBA80wbXBjibYddrT5B46-fB-cngU-CobQQHmtwmktC6mNMSGohRcmlQZlsUaHGspc65ylhRxLInkcckTRYlMUp7pjMeUUG0UvY_2qrrSDxFOS5oYSiPCC3COlM5pJlWkuKEGPvw4HaGoG1VReAl6Wwnlp3ChKCPivBIWE5ETQYXFZIRe9bfMW_2VPzV-3UElBuILO5kzAdbADX1TMS_NCOFtzaHbtR6fd4gLsLt2MU1Wul4tRWyr5WYs2d3CFo6IMs5G6EFLlbU_0LLs0c4rj9HN4dt7gvaaxUo_Be-3Uc8cv38B3wOs9A |
linkProvider | Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Human+eye-head+gaze+shifts+in+a+distractor+task.+II.+Reduced+threshold+for+initiation+of+early+head+movements&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+neurophysiology&rft.au=Corneil%2C+B+D&rft.au=Munoz%2C+D+P&rft.date=1999-09-01&rft.issn=0022-3077&rft.volume=82&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=1406&rft_id=info:doi/10.1152%2Fjn.1999.82.3.1406&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F10482758&rft.externalDocID=10482758 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0022-3077&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0022-3077&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0022-3077&client=summon |