An Asymmetric Effect: Physical and Simulated Confederate’s Mere Presence Induce a Preference for Deontological Over Utilitarian Judgment
People may behave differently in a shared physical context due to the mere presence of others. The study examined whether individual moral judgments were subject to the confederate’s presence. Experiment 1 supported the hypothesis that the confederate’s presence, relative to the control group, incre...
Saved in:
Published in | Psychological reports Vol. 126; no. 5; pp. 2446 - 2464 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Los Angeles, CA
SAGE Publications
01.10.2023
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | People may behave differently in a shared physical context due to the mere presence of others. The study examined whether individual moral judgments were subject to the confederate’s presence. Experiment 1 supported the hypothesis that the confederate’s presence, relative to the control group, increased deontological judgment, disapproving of sacrificing a person’s lifetime or interest for preserving the greater good of others. Experiment 2 investigated whether the results extend to mental space. The result revealed that simulating a positive interaction with the confederate significantly increased the preference for deontological judgments relative to the control group. However, the effect disappeared if the participants were required to simulate only the person from the scenario that did not include any additional background contexts. These results demonstrated that the confederate’s physical presence and simulated confederate’s presence always preferred deontological judgments over utilitarian judgments. The findings suggested that the asymmetric moral effect occurred in the physical realm and mental space. |
---|---|
AbstractList | People may behave differently in a shared physical context due to the mere presence of others. The study examined whether individual moral judgments were subject to the confederate’s presence. Experiment 1 supported the hypothesis that the confederate’s presence, relative to the control group, increased deontological judgment, disapproving of sacrificing a person’s lifetime or interest for preserving the greater good of others. Experiment 2 investigated whether the results extend to mental space. The result revealed that simulating a positive interaction with the confederate significantly increased the preference for deontological judgments relative to the control group. However, the effect disappeared if the participants were required to simulate only the person from the scenario that did not include any additional background contexts. These results demonstrated that the confederate’s physical presence and simulated confederate’s presence always preferred deontological judgments over utilitarian judgments. The findings suggested that the asymmetric moral effect occurred in the physical realm and mental space. People may behave differently in a shared physical context due to the mere presence of others. The study examined whether individual moral judgments were subject to the confederate's presence. Experiment 1 supported the hypothesis that the confederate's presence, relative to the control group, increased deontological judgment, disapproving of sacrificing a person's lifetime or interest for preserving the greater good of others. Experiment 2 investigated whether the results extend to mental space. The result revealed that simulating a positive interaction with the confederate significantly increased the preference for deontological judgments relative to the control group. However, the effect disappeared if the participants were required to simulate only the person from the scenario that did not include any additional background contexts. These results demonstrated that the confederate's physical presence and simulated confederate's presence always preferred deontological judgments over utilitarian judgments. The findings suggested that the asymmetric moral effect occurred in the physical realm and mental space.People may behave differently in a shared physical context due to the mere presence of others. The study examined whether individual moral judgments were subject to the confederate's presence. Experiment 1 supported the hypothesis that the confederate's presence, relative to the control group, increased deontological judgment, disapproving of sacrificing a person's lifetime or interest for preserving the greater good of others. Experiment 2 investigated whether the results extend to mental space. The result revealed that simulating a positive interaction with the confederate significantly increased the preference for deontological judgments relative to the control group. However, the effect disappeared if the participants were required to simulate only the person from the scenario that did not include any additional background contexts. These results demonstrated that the confederate's physical presence and simulated confederate's presence always preferred deontological judgments over utilitarian judgments. The findings suggested that the asymmetric moral effect occurred in the physical realm and mental space. |
Author | Xu, Wenming Yu, Xinyue Guo, Jiayi Wang, Ruiming |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Wenming orcidid: 0000-0003-4264-1001 surname: Xu fullname: Xu, Wenming organization: Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou , China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China – sequence: 2 givenname: Xinyue orcidid: 0000-0001-7471-1209 surname: Yu fullname: Yu, Xinyue organization: Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou , China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China – sequence: 3 givenname: Jiayi surname: Guo fullname: Guo, Jiayi organization: Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou , China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China – sequence: 4 givenname: Ruiming orcidid: 0000-0002-3277-0953 surname: Wang fullname: Wang, Ruiming organization: Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou , China; School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35428414$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kcFO3DAQhq0KVJaFB-il8rGXQCZOnKS31RYoaBFIBYlb5NjjbVBiU9uptDfOvAGvx5PUYWkvSD2NZv7vH-mf2Sc7xhok5BOkRwBleZymjGV1DlkGaVXWafWBzKAoqoTXcLdDZpOeTMAe2ff-PraQsvIj2WNFnlU55DPytDB04TfDgMF1kp5ojTJ8pdc_N76ToqfCKPqjG8ZeBFR0aY1GhS42L4_Pnl6iQ3rt0KORSM-NGmMR00RHZZpp6-g3tCbY3q5fN179RkdvQ9d3QbhOGHoxqvWAJhyQXS16j4dvdU5uT09ult-T1dXZ-XKxSmRWZyEpay4ZhxwVIG_bgrUcY85USSaYKAA4rwVI0LkUuqg4Q1nKeBBVgRZ5i2xOvmz3Pjj7a0QfmqHzEvteGLSjbzJeAK9qYGVEP7-hYzugah5cNwi3af7eLwKwBaSz3sfY_xBIm-lHzbsfRc_R1uPFGpt7OzoT4_7H8Ad05JKI |
Cites_doi | 10.1038/nn.3648 10.1080/00224545.2020.1860882 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.004 10.1196/annals.1440.001 10.1177/1745691620904083 10.3329/dujbs.v27i1.46405 10.1177/1368430213510573 10.1177/0956797617740973 10.1037/xhp0000153 10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1 10.1162/jocn_a_00797 10.1073/pnas.1108996108 10.1177/0956797611434965 10.1017/S0140525X11002202 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.014 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.008 10.1002/ejsp.126 10.1037/pspi0000194 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2348-13.2013 10.18637/jss.v082.i13 10.1080/016396201750065793 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02096.x 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.01058 10.1016/j.neures.2012.01.003 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.06.004 10.1037/pspa0000170 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.010 10.1177/0146167211422365 10.1098/rstb.2015.0100 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.770891 10.2307/796133 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.11.004 10.1007/s40806-016-0080-6 10.1080/10781910903484776 10.1521/soco.2015.33.6.562 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 10.1016/j.socec.2020.101561 10.1073/pnas.1402461111 10.1177/0022022100031006003 10.1037/xge0000165 10.1177/1745691614556679 10.1007/BF03395769 10.1037/a0031021 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s) 2022 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s) 2022 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1177/00332941221087908 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef PubMed |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1558-691X |
EndPage | 2464 |
ExternalDocumentID | 35428414 10_1177_00332941221087908 10.1177_00332941221087908 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Educational Science Planning Project of Guangdong Province grantid: 2019GXJK199 – fundername: National Social Science Foundation grantid: 19ZDA360 – fundername: Guangdong Planning Office of Philosophy and Social Science grantid: GD20YDXZXL14 – fundername: Guangdong Province Special Fund for Cultural Prosperity and Development(2019) |
GroupedDBID | --- --Z -DZ -TM -~X .-4 .GJ 0R~ 123 186 3EH 53G 54M 5WV 6TJ 85S 9M8 AADUE AAGGD AAGLT AAIKC AAJPV AAKTJ AAMNW AANSI AAOVH AAPEO AAQXI AARIX AATAA AAWLO AAWTL ABAWP ABCCA ABCJG ABCQX ABDPE ABEIX ABFNE ABFXH ABIDT ABIVO ABJNI ABKRH ABPNF ABPPZ ABQKF ABQPY ABQXT ABRHV ABUJY ABYTW ABZEH ACABN ACDXX ACFUR ACFZE ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACJER ACKIV ACNCT ACOFE ACONW ACOXC ACROE ACSIQ ACTDY ACUFS ACUIR ADEBD ADEIA ADGDL ADLEJ ADMHG ADNWM ADPEE ADRRZ ADTOS ADUKL ADXHL AEDXQ AEGXH AEOBU AEONT AESMA AESZF AETEA AEWDL AEWHI AEXNY AFEET AFFDN AFFNX AFHKK AFKRG AFMOU AFQAA AFUIA AGDVU AGKLV AGNHF AGNWV AHDMH AHOJL AHQJS AHWHD AIAGR AJUZI AKVCP ALFTD ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ARTOV AUTPY AYPQM BDZRT BMVBW BPACV C45 CBRKF CCGJY CEADM CS3 DOPDO DU5 DV7 DV8 EBD EBS EJD EMOBN F5P FHBDP GROUPED_SAGE_PREMIER_JOURNAL_COLLECTION H13 HF~ HZ~ H~9 J8X L7B LPU MVM NHB O-3 O9- OHT OVD P2P PQQKQ Q1R RGH ROL RXW S10 SASJQ SAUOL SCNPE SFC SJN SKT SSDHQ SV3 TAE TEORI TN5 UBW UBZ UHB UKR UQL VQG VQP WH7 XJT XKC XOL XZL YNT YR5 YRY YYQ YZZ ZCA ZCG ZGI ZHY ZPLXX ZPPRI ZUP ZVN ZXP ~32 ~34 AAYXX ACCVC AJGYC AMNSR CITATION AAPII AJVBE NPM 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c292t-796c3614ed1e6bb53b6e0330dc3a3a511669a1c1f4caf5863ec7c691d81fa4be3 |
ISSN | 0033-2941 1558-691X |
IngestDate | Thu Jul 10 18:36:45 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:12:46 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 05:24:19 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 22:31:26 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 5 |
Keywords | utilitarian judgment deontological judgment asymmetric effect moral judgment |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c292t-796c3614ed1e6bb53b6e0330dc3a3a511669a1c1f4caf5863ec7c691d81fa4be3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0001-7471-1209 0000-0002-3277-0953 0000-0003-4264-1001 |
PMID | 35428414 |
PQID | 2651689137 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 19 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2651689137 pubmed_primary_35428414 crossref_primary_10_1177_00332941221087908 sage_journals_10_1177_00332941221087908 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2023-10-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2023-10-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2023 text: 2023-10-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Los Angeles, CA |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Los Angeles, CA – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Psychological reports |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Psychol Rep |
PublicationYear | 2023 |
Publisher | SAGE Publications |
Publisher_xml | – name: SAGE Publications |
References | Baayen, Davidson, Bates 2008; 59 Everett, Pizarro, Crockett 2016; 145 Schacter, Addis, Buckner 2008; 1124 Baumard, André, Sperber 2013; 36 Gaesser, Shimura, Cikara 2020; 118 Izuma 2012; 72 Polman, Ruttan 2012; 38 Lee, Sul, Kim 2018; 39 Freundlieb, Kovács, Sebanz 2018; 29 Baron, Miller 2000; 31 Greene 2007; 11 Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber, Ric 2005; 9 Miles, Crisp 2014; 17 Bacchini, De Angelis, Dragone, Esposito, Affuso 2021; 12 Foot 1967; 5 Valdesolo, Desteno 2008; 44 Gaesser, Schacter 2014; 111 Samia, Khanam 2018; 27 Strijkers, Bertrand, Grainger 2015; 27 Barr, Levy, Scheepers, Tily 2013; 68 Bostyn, Roets 2016; 8 Husnu, Crisp 2010; 16 Galinsky, Maddux, Gilin, White 2008; 19 Macko 2021; 161 Freundlieb, Kovács, Sebanz 2016; 42 Caruso, Gino 2011; 118 Andersson, Erlandsson, Västfjäll, Tinghög 2020; 87 Uhlmann, Pizarro, Diermeier 2015; 10 Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, Christensen 2017; 82 Amit, Greene 2012; 23 Gaesser, Keeler, Young 2018; 171 Conway, Gawronski 2013; 104 Fu, Lu, Kou 2015; 47 Gaesser, Horn, Young 2015; 33 Mata 2019; 117 Schein 2020; 15 Laner, Benin, Ventrone 2001; 22 Chekroun, Brauer 2002; 32 Rockenbach, Milinski 2011; 108 Sacco, Brown, Lustgraaf, Hugenberg 2017; 3 Biocca 1997; 3 Surtees, Apperly, Samson 2016; 150 Milinski 2016; 371 Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker 2015; 67 Genevsky, Västfjäll, Slovic, Knutson 2013; 33 Thomson 1985; 94 Aarts, Verhage, Veenvliet, Dolan, van der Sluis 2014; 17 McDougall, Chery, Terrance, Weatherly 2011; 61 bibr45-00332941221087908 bibr32-00332941221087908 bibr25-00332941221087908 bibr7-00332941221087908 bibr38-00332941221087908 bibr2-00332941221087908 bibr8-00332941221087908 bibr37-00332941221087908 bibr1-00332941221087908 bibr18-00332941221087908 bibr24-00332941221087908 bibr29-00332941221087908 bibr3-00332941221087908 Foot P. (bibr17-00332941221087908) 1967; 5 bibr44-00332941221087908 bibr9-00332941221087908 bibr23-00332941221087908 bibr13-00332941221087908 bibr49-00332941221087908 bibr33-00332941221087908 bibr19-00332941221087908 bibr43-00332941221087908 bibr22-00332941221087908 bibr35-00332941221087908 Piaget J. (bibr39-00332941221087908) 1948 bibr48-00332941221087908 bibr28-00332941221087908 Biocca F. (bibr11-00332941221087908) 1997; 3 bibr15-00332941221087908 bibr42-00332941221087908 bibr5-00332941221087908 bibr27-00332941221087908 bibr34-00332941221087908 bibr21-00332941221087908 bibr4-00332941221087908 bibr14-00332941221087908 bibr41-00332941221087908 bibr31-00332941221087908 Bostyn D. H. (bibr12-00332941221087908) 2016; 8 bibr47-00332941221087908 bibr30-00332941221087908 bibr40-00332941221087908 bibr20-00332941221087908 bibr36-00332941221087908 bibr10-00332941221087908 bibr6-00332941221087908 bibr46-00332941221087908 bibr50-00332941221087908 bibr16-00332941221087908 bibr26-00332941221087908 |
References_xml | – volume: 17 start-page: 491 issue: 4 year: 2014 end-page: 496 article-title: A solution to dependency: Using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data publication-title: Nature Neuroscience – volume: 23 start-page: 861 issue: 8 year: 2012 end-page: 868 article-title: You see, the ends don't justify the means: Visual imagery and moral judgment publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 31 start-page: 703 issue: 6 year: 2000 end-page: 725 article-title: Limiting the scope of moral obligations to help a cross-cultural investigation publication-title: Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology – volume: 36 start-page: 59 issue: 1 year: 2013 end-page: 78 article-title: A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice publication-title: The Behavioral and Brain Sciences – volume: 145 start-page: 772 issue: 6 year: 2016 end-page: 787 article-title: Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology General – volume: 118 start-page: 280 issue: 2 year: 2011 end-page: 285 article-title: Blind ethics: Closing one's eyes polarizes moral judgments and discourages dishonest behavior publication-title: Cognition – volume: 8 start-page: 1 issue: 3 year: 2016 end-page: 8 article-title: An asymmetric moral conformity effect: Subjects conform to deontological but not consequentialist majorities publication-title: Social Psychological and Personality Science – volume: 10 start-page: 72 issue: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 81 article-title: A person-centered approach to moral judgment publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science – volume: 3 start-page: 125 issue: 2 year: 2017 end-page: 132 article-title: The adaptive utility of deontology: Deontological moral decision–making fosters perceptions of trust and likeability publication-title: Evolutionary Psychological Science – volume: 68 start-page: 255 issue: 3 year: 2013 end-page: 278 article-title: Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 47 start-page: 1058 issue: 8 year: 2015 end-page: 1066 article-title: Effects of a stranger’s presence and behavior on moral hypocrisy publication-title: Acta Psychologica Sinica – volume: 161 start-page: 664 issue: 6 year: 2021 end-page: 682 article-title: Contingencies of self-worth and the strength of deontological and utilitarian inclinations publication-title: The Journal of Social Psychology – volume: 9 start-page: 184 issue: 3 year: 2005 end-page: 211 article-title: Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion publication-title: Personality and Social Psychology Review – volume: 108 start-page: 18307 issue: 45 year: 2011 end-page: 18312 article-title: To qualify as a social partner, humans hide severe punishment, although their observed cooperativeness is decisive publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America – volume: 12 start-page: 770891 year: 2021 article-title: Individual and environmental correlates of adolescents’ moral decision-making in moral dilemmas publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology – volume: 44 start-page: 1334 issue: 5 year: 2008 end-page: 1338 article-title: The duality of virtue: Deconstructing the moral hypocrite publication-title: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology – volume: 33 start-page: 562 issue: 6 year: 2015 end-page: 584 article-title: When can imagining the self-increase willingness to help others? investigating whether the self-referential nature of episodic simulation fosters prosociality publication-title: Social Cognition – volume: 16 start-page: 97 issue: 1 year: 2010 end-page: 108 article-title: Imagined intergroup contact: A new technique for encouraging greater inter–ethnic contact in Cyprus publication-title: Peace and Conflict Journal of Peace Psychology – volume: 32 start-page: 853 issue: 6 year: 2002 end-page: 867 article-title: The bystander effect and social control behavior: The effect of the presence of others on people’s reactions to norm violations publication-title: European Journal of Social Psychology – volume: 94 start-page: 1395 issue: 6 year: 1985 end-page: 1415 article-title: The trolley problem publication-title: The Yale Law Journal – volume: 118 start-page: 683 issue: 4 year: 2020 end-page: 705 article-title: Episodic simulation reduces intergroup bias in prosocial intentions and behavior publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology – volume: 27 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2018 end-page: 7 article-title: Number of bystander, time pressure and gender effects on altruistic behavior publication-title: Dhaka University Journal of Biological Sciences – volume: 59 start-page: 390 issue: 4 year: 2008 end-page: 412 article-title: Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 11 start-page: 322 issue: 8 year: 2007 end-page: 324 article-title: Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 67 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 48 article-title: Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 publication-title: Journal of Statistical Software – volume: 27 start-page: 1542 issue: 8 year: 2015 end-page: 1551 article-title: Seeing the same words differently: The time course of automaticity and top-down intention in reading publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 22 start-page: 23 issue: 1 year: 2001 end-page: 42 article-title: Bystander attitudes towards victims of violence: Who is worth helping? publication-title: Deviant Behavior – volume: 42 start-page: 401 issue: 3 year: 2016 end-page: 412 article-title: When do humans spontaneously adopt another’s visuospatial perspective? publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance – volume: 15 start-page: 207 issue: 2 year: 2020 end-page: 215 article-title: The importance of context in moral judgments publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science – volume: 87 start-page: 101561 year: 2020 article-title: Prosocial and moral behavior under decision reveal in a public environment publication-title: Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics – volume: 104 start-page: 216 issue: 2 year: 2013 end-page: 235 article-title: Deontological and utilitarian inclinations in moral decision making: A process dissociation approach publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology – volume: 111 start-page: 4415 issue: 12 year: 2014 end-page: 4420 article-title: Episodic simulation and episodic memory can increase intentions to help others publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences – volume: 3 start-page: 12 issue: 2 year: 1997 end-page: 26 article-title: Cyborg’s dilemma: Embodiment in virtual environments publication-title: Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication – volume: 150 start-page: 43 year: 2016 end-page: 52 article-title: I’ve got your number: Spontaneous perspective-taking in an interactive task publication-title: Cognition – volume: 82 start-page: 1 issue: 13 year: 2017 end-page: 26 article-title: LmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models publication-title: Journal of Statistical Software – volume: 17 start-page: 3 issue: 1 year: 2014 end-page: 26 article-title: A meta-analytic test of the imagined contact hypothesis publication-title: Group Processes and Intergroup Relations – volume: 38 start-page: 129 issue: 1 year: 2012 end-page: 139 article-title: Effects of anger, guilt, and envy on moral hypocrisy publication-title: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin – volume: 29 start-page: 614 issue: 4 year: 2018 end-page: 622 article-title: Reading your mind while you are reading-evidence for spontaneous visuospatial perspective taking during a semantic categorization task publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 61 start-page: 411 issue: 3 year: 2011 end-page: 424 article-title: The effect of male confederate presence, betting, and accuracy of play on males’ gambling on blackjack publication-title: Psychological Record – volume: 171 start-page: 180 year: 2018 end-page: 193 article-title: Moral imagination: Facilitating prosocial decision-making through scene imagery and theory of mind publication-title: Cognition – volume: 117 start-page: 1061 issue: 6 year: 2019 end-page: 1082 article-title: Social metacognition in moral judgment: Decisional conflict promotes perspective taking publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology – volume: 72 start-page: 283 issue: 4 year: 2012 end-page: 288 article-title: The social neuroscience of reputation publication-title: Neuroscience Research – volume: 1124 start-page: 39 issue: 1 year: 2008 end-page: 60 article-title: Episodic simulation of future events: Concepts, data, and applications publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences – volume: 39 start-page: 611 issue: 6 year: 2018 end-page: 621 article-title: Social observation increases deontological judgments in moral dilemmas publication-title: Evolution and Human Behavior – volume: 19 start-page: 378 issue: 4 year: 2008 end-page: 384 article-title: Why it pays to get inside the head of your opponent: The differential effects of perspective taking and empathy in negotiations publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 33 start-page: 17188 issue: 43 year: 2013 end-page: 17196 article-title: Neural underpinnings of the identifiable victim effect: Affect shifts preferences for giving publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 371 start-page: 20150100 issue: 1687 year: 2016 article-title: Reputation, a universal currency for human social interactions publication-title: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences – volume: 5 start-page: 5 year: 1967 end-page: 15 article-title: The problem of abortion and the doctrine of double effect publication-title: Oxford Review – ident: bibr1-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1038/nn.3648 – ident: bibr33-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1080/00224545.2020.1860882 – ident: bibr27-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.004 – ident: bibr44-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.001 – ident: bibr45-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/1745691620904083 – ident: bibr43-00332941221087908 doi: 10.3329/dujbs.v27i1.46405 – ident: bibr36-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/1368430213510573 – ident: bibr19-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/0956797617740973 – ident: bibr18-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1037/xhp0000153 – ident: bibr38-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1 – ident: bibr46-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00797 – ident: bibr41-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108996108 – ident: bibr2-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/0956797611434965 – ident: bibr10-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11002202 – ident: bibr8-00332941221087908 doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 – ident: bibr47-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.014 – volume: 5 start-page: 5 year: 1967 ident: bibr17-00332941221087908 publication-title: Oxford Review – ident: bibr13-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.008 – ident: bibr14-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1002/ejsp.126 – ident: bibr24-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1037/pspi0000194 – ident: bibr4-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 – ident: bibr26-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2348-13.2013 – ident: bibr30-00332941221087908 doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13 – ident: bibr31-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1080/016396201750065793 – ident: bibr25-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02096.x – volume: 8 start-page: 1 issue: 3 year: 2016 ident: bibr12-00332941221087908 publication-title: Social Psychological and Personality Science – ident: bibr20-00332941221087908 doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.01058 – ident: bibr29-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2012.01.003 – ident: bibr32-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.06.004 – ident: bibr34-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1037/pspa0000170 – ident: bibr50-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.010 – ident: bibr40-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/0146167211422365 – ident: bibr37-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0100 – ident: bibr9-00332941221087908 – volume: 3 start-page: 12 issue: 2 year: 1997 ident: bibr11-00332941221087908 publication-title: Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication – ident: bibr5-00332941221087908 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.770891 – volume-title: The moral judgement of the child year: 1948 ident: bibr39-00332941221087908 – ident: bibr48-00332941221087908 doi: 10.2307/796133 – ident: bibr22-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.11.004 – ident: bibr42-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1007/s40806-016-0080-6 – ident: bibr28-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1080/10781910903484776 – ident: bibr21-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1521/soco.2015.33.6.562 – ident: bibr7-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 – ident: bibr3-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2020.101561 – ident: bibr23-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402461111 – ident: bibr6-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/0022022100031006003 – ident: bibr16-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1037/xge0000165 – ident: bibr49-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1177/1745691614556679 – ident: bibr35-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1007/BF03395769 – ident: bibr15-00332941221087908 doi: 10.1037/a0031021 |
SSID | ssj0031037 |
Score | 2.3448458 |
Snippet | People may behave differently in a shared physical context due to the mere presence of others. The study examined whether individual moral judgments were... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref sage |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Publisher |
StartPage | 2446 |
Title | An Asymmetric Effect: Physical and Simulated Confederate’s Mere Presence Induce a Preference for Deontological Over Utilitarian Judgment |
URI | https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/00332941221087908 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35428414 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2651689137 |
Volume | 126 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Li9swEBZpFkoupe-mL1QoLHRxiSVbtnoLfS0LabdlF9JTkGx58cHeJbFb0l_Qn90Z2bKTdANtL8YosQzzfR7NSPMg5CW4XGCmTiIv5ZH0AsEzT8dm4mUsZVrCgiRsutjskzg-D07m4Xww-LGZXVLp18nPa_NK_gdVGANcMUv2H5DtJoUBuAd84QoIw_WvMJ6WIN51UWBXrOSoKUSMLv6pE76Ny8wLbNGFO7mY3YeVNrElXrQ6mpmlsTEY9uvGJh6YPYAjrvgsxiC-M1jiwKnIz9-xbGFla3svUTmc1OlFFz3TWrnbWrU9lnDQzmsb12fKwq2ati8YDs7zcl13TPtYN6dCuVrn_b5_o5u-1nn3eLtnwfroN1hyWj0bxp6QtluO052cMxn4DHzQOJK22MM1-t2eMGMDur3_BTFfFRZwHoJvFTQ5qjtFtd1PN8gBA_-CDcnB9Nvpl5lbxLH7WtQegtv6XLtvHJGbbo5ti-YPN2UrRNBaLWe3ya3W3aDThjt3yMCUd8mow2d9j_yalrQnEW1I9IY6ClGgEO0oRDcodLiiSCDqCEQbAlFFewJRIBDdIhBFAtENAlFHoPvk_MP7s7fHXtuew0uYZJUXSZFwsO5M6huhdci1MCCmSZpwxRUY8kJI5Sd-FiQqC2PBTRIlgHka-5kKtOEPyLC8LM0jQmUWZ1oxFUVG4IaGMjpIUbap1DBfOCavnIgXV00VloXvCtXvQjMmLxwIC9CVeACmSnNZrxZMhL7Ac_loTB426HTTOTTH5BDhWrSf-Wr_ex7vneMJGfWsf0qG1bI2z8B2rfTzlme_AS1_lJI |
linkProvider | SAGE Publications |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LaxRBEC5kczAX49v1kbQgCMKE7emenmlvixrWmI0rZiHiYehHjQTZiWRnD_Hk2X_g3_OX2DXTs5CoIJ4GhqFf1VVdNV31fQBPQsgV3NRRnniR60QqUSW2wFFSpT61OhxIqi0Xmx6qyVzuH2fHMauSamHiCi53Ka0qjKg11mvtJqSkkRCpljwNsUqRa6rz3SioGmEAG-MPs3fT3g4TgVaHmNmylUke7zT_2MjFU-k3V_NCmld78uxtwcd-zF3CyefdVROG-_USnOP_Teo6XIsOKRt3O-gGXMH6Jmyu7eL5Lfg-rtl4eb5YEPmWYx3e8XM2ixJmpvbs_cmCeMDQs7aEkOA8G_z57ceSTfEM2awtcnLIiCkkPAy9iZWGLLjN7CUSjkK0w-xt0C82b1oA8aAgNdtf-U_0G_M2zPdeHb2YJJHCIXGpTpsk18qJ4AGg56iszYRVGOY68k4YYYKzp5Q23PFKOlNlhRLocqc09wWvjLQo7sCgPq3xHjBdFZU1qclzVBT0GrTSF5JLr21oLxvCs16E5ZcOqaPkPZj55fUdwuNeyGXQJ7okMTWerpZlqjKu6O42H8LdTvrr5kQWgrXQ4xCekiTLXq5_7-f-P3-5A1cnR9OD8uD14ZsHsEms9l3O4EMYNGcrfBR8n8Zux03-C-B290c |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3da9RAEB_kCtIX67dXv1YQBCH1djfZZPt2WI9avXqiB_Up7MdESrm09HIP9cln_4P-e_4lziabQquC-BQIYXezs_O1M_MbgOfkcpGZOsoTL3OdpEpWiS1wlFTCC6tJIam2XGy6r3bn6d5BdhAv3EItTNzB5VZIq6IVtcI6cPeJr17FGGPbgEzolAvyV4pch1rfNVJUQgxgbfxl9nHay-LQRKtDzWw7lqU8xjX_OMhlzfSbuXkp1avVPpMNKPt1d0knR1urhpb87Qqk4___2E24EQ1TNu5O0i24hvVtWL-Qj2d34Me4ZuPl2WIRmnA51uEeb7NZpDQztWefDhehHxh61pYSBljPBn9-P1-yKZ4im7XFTg5Z6BhCDxPexIpDRuYz28GApxDlMftAfMbmTQskToxSs72V_xquM-_CfPLm8-vdJLZySJzQoklyrZwkSwA9R2VtJq1C-teRd9JIQ0afUtpwx6vUmSorlESXO6W5L3hlUovyHgzq4xofANNVUVkjTJ6jCs6vQZv6IuWp15bGy4bwsidjedIhdpS8BzW_ur9DeNYTuiS-CsESU-PxalkKlXEVYrj5EO53J-BiOJmR00YzDuFFoGbZ0_bv82z-85dP4fpsZ1K-f7v_7iGsh-b2XergIxg0pyt8TCZQY5_Ec_4L9tH5vA |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=An+Asymmetric+Effect%3A+Physical+and+Simulated+Confederate%27s+Mere+Presence+Induce+a+Preference+for+Deontological+Over+Utilitarian+Judgment&rft.jtitle=Psychological+reports&rft.au=Xu%2C+Wenming&rft.au=Yu%2C+Xinyue&rft.au=Guo%2C+Jiayi&rft.au=Wang%2C+Ruiming&rft.date=2023-10-01&rft.eissn=1558-691X&rft.spage=332941221087908&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177%2F00332941221087908&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F35428414&rft.externalDocID=35428414 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0033-2941&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0033-2941&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0033-2941&client=summon |