On the Nature of Interruptions in Complex Dynamic Tasks
The formal study of interruption can improve workplace safety by providing insights into the cognitive processes underlying interrupted task performance (Boehm-Davis & Remington, 2009). Experimental studies examining the disruptive effects of interruptions have typically used basic laboratory pa...
Saved in:
Published in | Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting Vol. 60; no. 1; pp. 246 - 247 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Los Angeles, CA
SAGE Publications
01.09.2016
|
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 2169-5067 1071-1813 1071-1813 2169-5067 |
DOI | 10.1177/1541931213601055 |
Cover
Abstract | The formal study of interruption can improve workplace safety by providing insights into the cognitive processes underlying interrupted task performance (Boehm-Davis & Remington, 2009). Experimental studies examining the disruptive effects of interruptions have typically used basic laboratory paradigms (Trafton & Monk, 2007) and account for disruptive effects using prospective memory models (McDaniel, Einstein, Graham, & Rall, 2004) or activation-based models (Altmann & Trafton, 2002). However, the effects of interruptions in more complex dynamic tasks, such as air traffic control (ATC), may be different because they require operators to perform multiple task goals in a continuously evolving task environment.
The current study examined the impact of two kinds of interruption on performance in simulated ATC. Participants (n = 60) were required to accept/handoff aircraft entering/exiting their sector and to prevent aircraft pairs from conflicting (violating minimum separation standards). There were three within-subjects conditions: no-interruption (baseline control), a blank interruption (blank screen for 27s), and an n-back interruption (visual 2-back task, for 27s). Each ATC trial included two delayed-execute PM tasks: a conflict-resolution PM-task and a handoff PM-task. Both tasks were
encoded before the interruption occurred.
The conflict-resolution PM-task required participants to resolve a deferred conflict immediately after the interruption ended. Time taken to resolve this conflict was taken as resumption time. We predicted resumption time would increase for both interruption conditions due to the need to reorientate to the evolved visual ATC scene. We also predicted the n-back interruption would further increase resumption time due to blocking rehearsal.
Interruptions significantly increased resumption time. However, in contrast to previous research using basic tasks (Monk, Trafton, & Boehm-Davis, 2008) resumption time was not greater in the demanding n-back interruption relative to the blank interruption.
The handoff PM-task required participants to handoff an aircraft using a non-routine keystroke at 63s (on average) after the interruption. To be clear, the aircraft flashed for hand-off and the participant needed to remember to press the alternative handoff key instead of the routine handoff key. Prior research has examined PM in simulated ATC (Loft, 2014), and we extended it by examining how interruptions would impact PM. Based on findings in basic delayed-execute studies, we predicted interruptions would increase handoff PM-task errors. The average error rate on the handoff PM-task was 31%, but this
did not differ between the conditions.
Two important findings emerged from this study. Firstly, we did not find rehearsal to be an important factor in interruption recovery in simulated ATC. This indicates that orientating to an evolved visual scene may be the primary driver behind resumption time costs in this dynamic task. Secondly, we replicated the PM errors reported in previous simulated ATC experiments, but found that PM performance was robust to interruptions. Whilst the use of a student sample with limited training limits the generalizability of the present study, we recommend psychological models of interruption be applied cautiously, particularly in safety-critical environments. Human factors practice and research will benefit from further examining the nature of interruptions in complex task environments. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The formal study of interruption can improve workplace safety by providing insights into the cognitive processes underlying interrupted task performance (Boehm-Davis & Remington, 2009). Experimental studies examining the disruptive effects of interruptions have typically used basic laboratory paradigms (Trafton & Monk, 2007) and account for disruptive effects using prospective memory models (McDaniel, Einstein, Graham, & Rall, 2004) or activation-based models (Altmann & Trafton, 2002). However, the effects of interruptions in more complex dynamic tasks, such as air traffic control (ATC), may be different because they require operators to perform multiple task goals in a continuously evolving task environment.
The current study examined the impact of two kinds of interruption on performance in simulated ATC. Participants (n = 60) were required to accept/handoff aircraft entering/exiting their sector and to prevent aircraft pairs from conflicting (violating minimum separation standards). There were three within-subjects conditions: no-interruption (baseline control), a blank interruption (blank screen for 27s), and an n-back interruption (visual 2-back task, for 27s). Each ATC trial included two delayed-execute PM tasks: a conflict-resolution PM-task and a handoff PM-task. Both tasks were
encoded before the interruption occurred.
The conflict-resolution PM-task required participants to resolve a deferred conflict immediately after the interruption ended. Time taken to resolve this conflict was taken as resumption time. We predicted resumption time would increase for both interruption conditions due to the need to reorientate to the evolved visual ATC scene. We also predicted the n-back interruption would further increase resumption time due to blocking rehearsal.
Interruptions significantly increased resumption time. However, in contrast to previous research using basic tasks (Monk, Trafton, & Boehm-Davis, 2008) resumption time was not greater in the demanding n-back interruption relative to the blank interruption.
The handoff PM-task required participants to handoff an aircraft using a non-routine keystroke at 63s (on average) after the interruption. To be clear, the aircraft flashed for hand-off and the participant needed to remember to press the alternative handoff key instead of the routine handoff key. Prior research has examined PM in simulated ATC (Loft, 2014), and we extended it by examining how interruptions would impact PM. Based on findings in basic delayed-execute studies, we predicted interruptions would increase handoff PM-task errors. The average error rate on the handoff PM-task was 31%, but this
did not differ between the conditions.
Two important findings emerged from this study. Firstly, we did not find rehearsal to be an important factor in interruption recovery in simulated ATC. This indicates that orientating to an evolved visual scene may be the primary driver behind resumption time costs in this dynamic task. Secondly, we replicated the PM errors reported in previous simulated ATC experiments, but found that PM performance was robust to interruptions. Whilst the use of a student sample with limited training limits the generalizability of the present study, we recommend psychological models of interruption be applied cautiously, particularly in safety-critical environments. Human factors practice and research will benefit from further examining the nature of interruptions in complex task environments. The formal study of interruption can improve workplace safety by providing insights into the cognitive processes underlying interrupted task performance (Boehm-Davis & Remington, 2009). Experimental studies examining the disruptive effects of interruptions have typically used basic laboratory paradigms (Trafton & Monk, 2007) and account for disruptive effects using prospective memory models (McDaniel, Einstein, Graham, & Rall, 2004) or activation-based models (Altmann & Trafton, 2002). However, the effects of interruptions in more complex dynamic tasks, such as air traffic control (ATC), may be different because they require operators to perform multiple task goals in a continuously evolving task environment. The current study examined the impact of two kinds of interruption on performance in simulated ATC. Participants ( n = 60) were required to accept/handoff aircraft entering/exiting their sector and to prevent aircraft pairs from conflicting (violating minimum separation standards). There were three within-subjects conditions: no-interruption (baseline control), a blank interruption (blank screen for 27s), and an n-back interruption (visual 2-back task, for 27s). Each ATC trial included two delayed-execute PM tasks: a conflict-resolution PM-task and a handoff PM-task. Both tasks were encoded before the interruption occurred. The conflict-resolution PM-task required participants to resolve a deferred conflict immediately after the interruption ended. Time taken to resolve this conflict was taken as resumption time. We predicted resumption time would increase for both interruption conditions due to the need to reorientate to the evolved visual ATC scene. We also predicted the n-back interruption would further increase resumption time due to blocking rehearsal. Interruptions significantly increased resumption time. However, in contrast to previous research using basic tasks (Monk, Trafton, & Boehm-Davis, 2008) resumption time was not greater in the demanding n-back interruption relative to the blank interruption. The handoff PM-task required participants to handoff an aircraft using a non-routine keystroke at 63s (on average) after the interruption. To be clear, the aircraft flashed for hand-off and the participant needed to remember to press the alternative handoff key instead of the routine handoff key. Prior research has examined PM in simulated ATC (Loft, 2014), and we extended it by examining how interruptions would impact PM. Based on findings in basic delayed-execute studies, we predicted interruptions would increase handoff PM-task errors. The average error rate on the handoff PM-task was 31%, but this did not differ between the conditions. Two important findings emerged from this study. Firstly, we did not find rehearsal to be an important factor in interruption recovery in simulated ATC. This indicates that orientating to an evolved visual scene may be the primary driver behind resumption time costs in this dynamic task. Secondly, we replicated the PM errors reported in previous simulated ATC experiments, but found that PM performance was robust to interruptions. Whilst the use of a student sample with limited training limits the generalizability of the present study, we recommend psychological models of interruption be applied cautiously, particularly in safety-critical environments. Human factors practice and research will benefit from further examining the nature of interruptions in complex task environments. |
Author | Loft, Shayne Farrell, Simon Wilson, Micah K. Visser, Troy A. W. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Micah K. surname: Wilson fullname: Wilson, Micah K. email: micah.k.wilson@curtin.edu.au organization: University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia – sequence: 2 givenname: Simon surname: Farrell fullname: Farrell, Simon organization: University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia – sequence: 3 givenname: Troy A. W. surname: Visser fullname: Visser, Troy A. W. organization: University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia – sequence: 4 givenname: Shayne surname: Loft fullname: Loft, Shayne organization: University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia |
BookMark | eNp9jz1PwzAURS1UJNLCzug_EPCL4zgZUfiqVNGlzNGLY0NKYkd2ItF_T6oyVYLpLvdc3bMkC-usJuQW2B2AlPcgUig4JMAzBkyICxIBkxBDDnxBogSyIhYsk1dkGcKesYRLnkZEbi0dPzV9w3HymjpD13bU3k_D2DobaGtp6fqh09_08WCxbxXdYfgK1-TSYBf0zW-uyPvz0658jTfbl3X5sIkVyFTEgMY0PGuaxkDeoOIyU3md1wkiJLlidZEIjY2SEnWqpSjmu6lJFc-VqhXjfEWy067yLgSvTaXaEY_fRo9tVwGrjvrVuf4MsjNw8G2P_vAfEp-QgB-62rvJ21nt7_4PZnlqPA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1177_1541931218621382 |
Cites_doi | 10.1002/acp.1002 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.029 10.1207/s15516709cog2601_2 10.1177/0963721414545214 10.1037/a0014402 10.1518/155723408X299852 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2016 by Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2016 by Human Factors and Ergonomics Society |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION |
DOI | 10.1177/1541931213601055 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Engineering |
EISSN | 1071-1813 2169-5067 |
EndPage | 247 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10_1177_1541931213601055 10.1177_1541931213601055 |
GroupedDBID | -TM .2G .2L .2N 01A 09Z 0R~ 1~K 29P 4.4 54M 85S 88I 8AF 8FI 8FJ 8R4 8R5 AABOD AACKU AACTG AADIR AADUE AAGGD AAGLT AAJOX AAJPV AAKTJ AAMFR AANSI AAPEO AAQXI AARIX AATAA AATBZ AAWLO AAYTG ABAWP ABCCA ABCJG ABDWY ABEIX ABFWQ ABFXH ABHKI ABIDT ABJNI ABKRH ABLUO ABPNF ABQKF ABQPY ABQXT ABRHV ABUJY ABUWG ABYTW ACAEP ACDXX ACFUR ACFZE ACGBL ACGFS ACGOD ACJER ACLZU ACOFE ACOXC ACROE ACSIQ ACUAV ACUFS ACUIR ACXKE ADBBV ADDLC ADEBD ADEIA ADNON ADPEE ADRRZ ADTBJ ADTOS ADUKL ADVBO AEDXQ AEOBU AEPTA AEQLS AESMA AESZF AEUHG AEVPJ AEWDL AEWHI AEXNY AFEET AFKBI AFKRA AFKRG AFMOU AFQAA AFUIA AGDVU AGKLV AGNHF AGNWV AGWFA AHDMH AHWHD AJEFB AJUZI ALFTD ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ARTOV AUVAJ AYPQM AZFZN AZQEC BBRGL BDDNI BENPR BMVBW BPACV BPHCQ BVXVI BYIEH CBRKF CCGJY CCPQU CEADM CFDXU CORYS CS3 DD0 DD~ DE- DG~ DO- DOPDO DV7 DV8 DWQXO D~Y EBS EJD FHBDP FYUFA GNUQQ GROUPED_SAGE_PREMIER_JOURNAL_COLLECTION H13 HCIFZ HF~ HVGLF J8X K.F KQ4 M2M M2P O9- P.B PHGZM PHGZT PQQKQ PROAC PSYQQ Q1R Q2X Q7O Q7P Q7V Q7X Q82 Q83 ROL S01 SASJQ SAUOL SCNPE SFC SPV SSDHQ UKHRP ZPLXX ZPPRI ZRKOI ~32 AAYXX ACCVC AJGYC AMNSR CITATION |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c1745-1affd36dddf18dac376c8b8b2aa128c0b925eadc77ae4e7591814f4c38ccbc033 |
ISSN | 2169-5067 1071-1813 |
IngestDate | Thu Apr 24 23:11:51 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 05:23:36 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 22:29:09 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c1745-1affd36dddf18dac376c8b8b2aa128c0b925eadc77ae4e7591814f4c38ccbc033 |
PageCount | 2 |
ParticipantIDs | crossref_citationtrail_10_1177_1541931213601055 crossref_primary_10_1177_1541931213601055 sage_journals_10_1177_1541931213601055 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20160900 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 9 year: 2016 text: 20160900 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Los Angeles, CA |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Los Angeles, CA |
PublicationTitle | Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | SAGE Publications |
Publisher_xml | – name: SAGE Publications |
References | bibr3-1541931213601055 bibr6-1541931213601055 bibr5-1541931213601055 bibr4-1541931213601055 bibr2-1541931213601055 bibr1-1541931213601055 |
References_xml | – ident: bibr4-1541931213601055 doi: 10.1002/acp.1002 – ident: bibr2-1541931213601055 doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.029 – ident: bibr1-1541931213601055 doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2601_2 – ident: bibr3-1541931213601055 doi: 10.1177/0963721414545214 – ident: bibr5-1541931213601055 doi: 10.1037/a0014402 – ident: bibr6-1541931213601055 doi: 10.1518/155723408X299852 |
SSID | ssj0023734 |
Score | 1.9619328 |
Snippet | The formal study of interruption can improve workplace safety by providing insights into the cognitive processes underlying interrupted task performance... |
SourceID | crossref sage |
SourceType | Enrichment Source Index Database Publisher |
StartPage | 246 |
Title | On the Nature of Interruptions in Complex Dynamic Tasks |
URI | https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/1541931213601055 |
Volume | 60 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwELfK9gIPiE8xNpAfEBKq3OXDiZPHCTZNQNnQOhhPlePY28RIp6SVgP-F_5WLfXXTqqDBS5REF0vJ_WKf7353R8gLKW0dM8myKOSMZ1IxGRjOdMBlmpjECJsoPPyQHp7yt2fJWa_3q8Namk2Lgfq5Nq_kf7QK90CvbZbsP2jWDwo34Bz0C0fQMBxvpOMj5Cja4pzzpJC6nl17gnj7u1_p7_03rvF8fySbr03XID32C1gzpws4v_4BNuJpHev79blLX248yxPr8g-1TZnueG8aT8eXF_13A4-PlhLsQhwnl98Wsf9Plg9gUVNPYNhB_7N_5j0uEScX8gdG_9E_EaaegNUNjK11QUZhmrMkcO04BtpNwGDyMLA64u4M7ToOLCERp1t0X2q8EusXBRuWBlsRjNW2gl1qu4IuFkBPS0Th8aroLbIZCdGyADb3vhx_HPodfSwsbcG_yCIOvrs6xpLd0yENWjtmdI_cxQ0I3XNouk96unpA7nTKUj4k4qiiAATqcEUnhi7hil5WFHFFEVfU4uoROT3YH70-ZNhggynYiCYslMaUcVqWpQmzUipYbFRWZEUkJZgtKijyKIGZRgkhNdciyUEx3HAVZ0oVKojjx2SjmlT6CaGtqcvjSJV5aLgJS5nzLE2UClRUGJkGW2R3_vpjhdXn2yYoV-MQC86vfrAt8so_ce0qr_xF9mX7Rcf4bzZ_FHx6U8FtcnsB5h2yMa1n-hmYn9PiOYLgN5CSe0I |
linkProvider | SAGE Publications |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LSwMxEB60PagH32J95iCCh9R9JNnssfigalsVWqheSja7AWnZSh8g_nqT7LbWiiLeJ8swzM58Sb58A3AihNUxE5h7LsGEC4mFowhOHCIYVVQF9qFwvcGqLXLbpu2ZUV95BIdlQ6vSHtliPf27zTtxSjTmMEJkzA53XIQiN5o0BShWnh4e69Pdlh_YK2XPZSGmuiZ_3lF--8aXnjRD6LI95noNnifeZdSSbnk80o69zwk3_sv9dVjNkSeqZKmyAQtJugkrM3qEWxDcp0gDQtSwap-or5A9MByMM-YLekmRqR-95A1dZpPsUVMMu8NtaF1fNS-qOJ-sgKXegVDsCqVin8VxrFweC6mrjOQRjzwhdL-SThR6VKeYDAKRkCSgocYBRBHpcykj6fj-DhTSfprsAjIYh_iejENXEeXGIiScUSkd6UVKMKcE55PYdmQuO26mX_Q6bq40Ph-SEpxNV7xmkhu_2J6aSHcmcf_RcO-vhsewVG3Wa53aTeNuH5Y1PmIZpewACqPBODnUGGQUHeXZ9gH1MMx8 |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LSwMxEA7agujBt1ifOYjgIXYfSXb3WKylPlortFBPJZtsQFq2pduC-OtNsmmtFUW8zy7ZYTLzZefLNwBcMGZ0zBgKPRcjHDKOmCMxShzMKJFEBuaicKNJ6x183yVdy83Rd2GsB7NrTatSKzLJWu_ukZBl22Msq7KvcIcWI6NmwOMqKKo65XgFUKy8tJ4b8xOXH5i2sufSCBGVlz_7lN_e8aUuLZC6TJ2pbeXDVDMjT6jpJf3r6UQt7n1JvPHfn7ANNi0ChZU8ZHbASpLugo0FXcI9EDylUAFD2DSqn3AooflxOJ7mDBj4mkKdRwbJG6zmE-1hm2X9bB90arftmzqyExYQVycRglwmpfCpEEK6oWBcZRsexmHsMabqFnfiyCMq1HgQsAQnAYkUHsAScz_kPOaO7x-AQjpMk0MANdbBvsdF5EosXcEiHFLCucO9WDLqlEB55t8et_LjegrGoOdaxfFll5TA1fyJUS698YvtpfZ2b-b7Hw2P_mp4DtZa1Vrv8a75cAzWFUyiObPsBBQm42lyqqDIJD6zAfcB8GLO8Q |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=On+the+Nature+of+Interruptions+in+Complex+Dynamic+Tasks&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+Human+Factors+and+Ergonomics+Society+Annual+Meeting&rft.au=Wilson%2C+Micah+K.&rft.au=Farrell%2C+Simon&rft.au=Visser%2C+Troy+A.+W.&rft.au=Loft%2C+Shayne&rft.date=2016-09-01&rft.pub=SAGE+Publications&rft.issn=2169-5067&rft.eissn=1071-1813&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=246&rft.epage=247&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177%2F1541931213601055&rft.externalDocID=10.1177_1541931213601055 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2169-5067&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2169-5067&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2169-5067&client=summon |