Spatiotemporal dipole source localization of face processing ERPs in adolescents: a preliminary study
Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise temporal activation of brain sources specific to the processing of emotional facial expressions in older children and adolescents is not well known....
Saved in:
Published in | Behavioral and brain functions Vol. 5; no. 1; p. 16 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
BioMed Central Ltd
12.03.2009
BioMed Central BMC |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise temporal activation of brain sources specific to the processing of emotional facial expressions in older children and adolescents is not well known. This preliminary study aims to trace the spatiotemporal dynamics of facial emotion processing during adolescence and provide a basis for future developmental studies and comparisons with patient populations that have social-emotional deficits such as autism.
We presented pictures showing happy, angry, fearful, or neutral facial expressions to healthy adolescents (aged 10-16 years) and recorded 128-channel event-related potentials (ERPs) while they performed an emotion discrimination task. ERP components were analyzed for effects of age and emotion on amplitude and latency. The underlying cortical sources of scalp ERP activity were modeled as multiple equivalent current dipoles using Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA).
Initial global/holistic processing of faces (P1) took place in the visual association cortex (lingual gyrus) around 120 ms post-stimulus. Next, structural encoding of facial features (N170) occurred between 160-200 ms in the inferior temporal/fusiform region, and perhaps early emotion processing (Vertex Positive Potential or VPP) in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Finally, cognitive analysis of facial expressions (P2) in the prefrontal cortex and emotional reactions in somatosensory areas were observed from about 230 ms onwards. The temporal sequence of cortical source activation in response to facial emotion processing was occipital, prefrontal, fusiform, parietal for young adolescents and occipital, limbic, inferior temporal, and prefrontal for older adolescents.
This is a first report of high-density ERP dipole source analysis in healthy adolescents which traces the sequence of neural activity within the first 500 ms of categorizing emotion from faces. Our spatio-temporal brain source models showed the presence of adult-like cortical networks for face processing in adolescents, whose functional specificity to different emotions appear to be not yet fully mature. Age-related differences in brain activation patterns illustrate the continued development and maturation of distinct neural systems for processing facial expressions during adolescence and possible changes in emotion perception, experience, and reaction with age. |
---|---|
AbstractList | BACKGROUND: Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise temporal activation of brain sources specific to the processing of emotional facial expressions in older children and adolescents is not well known. This preliminary study aims to trace the spatiotemporal dynamics of facial emotion processing during adolescence and provide a basis for future developmental studies and comparisons with patient populations that have social-emotional deficits such as autism. METHODS: We presented pictures showing happy, angry, fearful, or neutral facial expressions to healthy adolescents (aged 10-16 years) and recorded 128-channel event-related potentials (ERPs) while they performed an emotion discrimination task. ERP components were analyzed for effects of age and emotion on amplitude and latency. The underlying cortical sources of scalp ERP activity were modeled as multiple equivalent current dipoles using Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA). RESULTS: Initial global/holistic processing of faces (P1) took place in the visual association cortex (lingual gyrus) around 120 ms post-stimulus. Next, structural encoding of facial features (N170) occurred between 160-200 ms in the inferior temporal/fusiform region, and perhaps early emotion processing (Vertex Positive Potential or VPP) in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Finally, cognitive analysis of facial expressions (P2) in the prefrontal cortex and emotional reactions in somatosensory areas were observed from about 230 ms onwards. The temporal sequence of cortical source activation in response to facial emotion processing was occipital, prefrontal, fusiform, parietal for young adolescents and occipital, limbic, inferior temporal, and prefrontal for older adolescents. CONCLUSION: This is a first report of high-density ERP dipole source analysis in healthy adolescents which traces the sequence of neural activity within the first 500 ms of categorizing emotion from faces. Our spatio-temporal brain source models showed the presence of adult-like cortical networks for face processing in adolescents, whose functional specificity to different emotions appear to be not yet fully mature. Age-related differences in brain activation patterns illustrate the continued development and maturation of distinct neural systems for processing facial expressions during adolescence and possible changes in emotion perception, experience, and reaction with age. Abstract Background Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise temporal activation of brain sources specific to the processing of emotional facial expressions in older children and adolescents is not well known. This preliminary study aims to trace the spatiotemporal dynamics of facial emotion processing during adolescence and provide a basis for future developmental studies and comparisons with patient populations that have social-emotional deficits such as autism. Methods We presented pictures showing happy, angry, fearful, or neutral facial expressions to healthy adolescents (aged 10–16 years) and recorded 128-channel event-related potentials (ERPs) while they performed an emotion discrimination task. ERP components were analyzed for effects of age and emotion on amplitude and latency. The underlying cortical sources of scalp ERP activity were modeled as multiple equivalent current dipoles using Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA). Results Initial global/holistic processing of faces (P1) took place in the visual association cortex (lingual gyrus) around 120 ms post-stimulus. Next, structural encoding of facial features (N170) occurred between 160–200 ms in the inferior temporal/fusiform region, and perhaps early emotion processing (Vertex Positive Potential or VPP) in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Finally, cognitive analysis of facial expressions (P2) in the prefrontal cortex and emotional reactions in somatosensory areas were observed from about 230 ms onwards. The temporal sequence of cortical source activation in response to facial emotion processing was occipital, prefrontal, fusiform, parietal for young adolescents and occipital, limbic, inferior temporal, and prefrontal for older adolescents. Conclusion This is a first report of high-density ERP dipole source analysis in healthy adolescents which traces the sequence of neural activity within the first 500 ms of categorizing emotion from faces. Our spatio-temporal brain source models showed the presence of adult-like cortical networks for face processing in adolescents, whose functional specificity to different emotions appear to be not yet fully mature. Age-related differences in brain activation patterns illustrate the continued development and maturation of distinct neural systems for processing facial expressions during adolescence and possible changes in emotion perception, experience, and reaction with age. Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise temporal activation of brain sources specific to the processing of emotional facial expressions in older children and adolescents is not well known. This preliminary study aims to trace the spatiotemporal dynamics of facial emotion processing during adolescence and provide a basis for future developmental studies and comparisons with patient populations that have social-emotional deficits such as autism. We presented pictures showing happy, angry, fearful, or neutral facial expressions to healthy adolescents (aged 10-16 years) and recorded 128-channel event-related potentials (ERPs) while they performed an emotion discrimination task. ERP components were analyzed for effects of age and emotion on amplitude and latency. The underlying cortical sources of scalp ERP activity were modeled as multiple equivalent current dipoles using Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA). Initial global/holistic processing of faces (P1) took place in the visual association cortex (lingual gyrus) around 120 ms post-stimulus. Next, structural encoding of facial features (N170) occurred between 160-200 ms in the inferior temporal/fusiform region, and perhaps early emotion processing (Vertex Positive Potential or VPP) in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Finally, cognitive analysis of facial expressions (P2) in the prefrontal cortex and emotional reactions in somatosensory areas were observed from about 230 ms onwards. The temporal sequence of cortical source activation in response to facial emotion processing was occipital, prefrontal, fusiform, parietal for young adolescents and occipital, limbic, inferior temporal, and prefrontal for older adolescents. This is a first report of high-density ERP dipole source analysis in healthy adolescents which traces the sequence of neural activity within the first 500 ms of categorizing emotion from faces. Our spatio-temporal brain source models showed the presence of adult-like cortical networks for face processing in adolescents, whose functional specificity to different emotions appear to be not yet fully mature. Age-related differences in brain activation patterns illustrate the continued development and maturation of distinct neural systems for processing facial expressions during adolescence and possible changes in emotion perception, experience, and reaction with age. BACKGROUNDDespite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise temporal activation of brain sources specific to the processing of emotional facial expressions in older children and adolescents is not well known. This preliminary study aims to trace the spatiotemporal dynamics of facial emotion processing during adolescence and provide a basis for future developmental studies and comparisons with patient populations that have social-emotional deficits such as autism. METHODSWe presented pictures showing happy, angry, fearful, or neutral facial expressions to healthy adolescents (aged 10-16 years) and recorded 128-channel event-related potentials (ERPs) while they performed an emotion discrimination task. ERP components were analyzed for effects of age and emotion on amplitude and latency. The underlying cortical sources of scalp ERP activity were modeled as multiple equivalent current dipoles using Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA). RESULTSInitial global/holistic processing of faces (P1) took place in the visual association cortex (lingual gyrus) around 120 ms post-stimulus. Next, structural encoding of facial features (N170) occurred between 160-200 ms in the inferior temporal/fusiform region, and perhaps early emotion processing (Vertex Positive Potential or VPP) in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Finally, cognitive analysis of facial expressions (P2) in the prefrontal cortex and emotional reactions in somatosensory areas were observed from about 230 ms onwards. The temporal sequence of cortical source activation in response to facial emotion processing was occipital, prefrontal, fusiform, parietal for young adolescents and occipital, limbic, inferior temporal, and prefrontal for older adolescents. CONCLUSIONThis is a first report of high-density ERP dipole source analysis in healthy adolescents which traces the sequence of neural activity within the first 500 ms of categorizing emotion from faces. Our spatio-temporal brain source models showed the presence of adult-like cortical networks for face processing in adolescents, whose functional specificity to different emotions appear to be not yet fully mature. Age-related differences in brain activation patterns illustrate the continued development and maturation of distinct neural systems for processing facial expressions during adolescence and possible changes in emotion perception, experience, and reaction with age. |
Author | Wong, Teresa Ka Wai Fung, Peter Chin Wan Chua, Siew Eng McAlonan, Grainne Mary |
AuthorAffiliation | 2 Department of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong 1 Department of Psychiatry, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 1 Department of Psychiatry, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong – name: 2 Department of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Teresa Ka Wai surname: Wong fullname: Wong, Teresa Ka Wai email: wongkwt@gmail.com organization: Department of Psychiatry, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong. wongkwt@gmail.com – sequence: 2 givenname: Peter Chin Wan surname: Fung fullname: Fung, Peter Chin Wan – sequence: 3 givenname: Grainne Mary surname: McAlonan fullname: McAlonan, Grainne Mary – sequence: 4 givenname: Siew Eng surname: Chua fullname: Chua, Siew Eng |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19284600$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp1kstv1DAQxi1URB9w5Yh845Rix47jcECqqgKVKoF4nK2JPV5cJXGws5XKX4_DrkpXiJOtmc8_f_M4JUdTnJCQl5ydc67VG95KWXVM86qpuHpCTh4CR4_ux-Q051vGhJZ1_Ywc867WUjF2QvDrDEuIC45zTDBQF-Y4IM1xmyzSIVoYwq9VMdHoqYcSnFO0mHOYNvTqy-dMw0TBlUfZ4rTktxSKAocwhgnSPc3L1t0_J089DBlf7M8z8v391bfLj9XNpw_Xlxc3Va-4XCrvuZAdCif6DrxHtLVTQoGTuhfQQldM9zX6VjTOcSmlsopxlCBb1zc1iDNyveO6CLdmTmEsFkyEYP4EYtoYSEuwAxoNfYNasFY2KL3VGmzvbFc77TxvuSysdzvWvO1HdGtxpUEH0MPMFH6YTbwztVJMNE0BXOwAfYj_ARxmbBzNOjKzjsw0hqvCeL03keLPLebFjKH0eRhgwrjNphVC1LLlrCjPd0qbYs4J_cNHnJl1Vf5Fv3pc31_5fjfEb_RTvzQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsycho_2017_07_022 crossref_primary_10_1186_1744_9081_7_8 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_00616 crossref_primary_10_1134_S0362119712060060 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2018_09_070 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_14303 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40489_021_00260_z crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2022_2038262 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2015_01_027 crossref_primary_10_4236_psych_2013_46073 crossref_primary_10_1134_S0362119711040104 crossref_primary_10_1080_87565641_2020_1789144 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_euroneuro_2015_01_009 crossref_primary_10_1097_MOP_0b013e32834cb9c9 crossref_primary_10_1080_1357650X_2017_1361963 crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_7591 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijpsycho_2014_06_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_clinph_2015_10_061 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2015_12_001 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2021_1920050 crossref_primary_10_12677_AP_2017_77114 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_06_031 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00787_020_01684_4 crossref_primary_10_1186_s11689_018_9244_y crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_016_0481_9 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_euroneuro_2010_06_002 crossref_primary_10_1093_braincomms_fcad023 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2009 Wong et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. 2009 Wong et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2009 Wong et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. 2009 Wong et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. |
DBID | NPM AAYXX CITATION 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.1186/1744-9081-5-16 |
DatabaseName | PubMed CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | PubMed CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Anatomy & Physiology Psychology |
EISSN | 1744-9081 |
EndPage | 16 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_8ab5e830745e4fc88acbdc92d8df1714 oai_biomedcentral_com_1744_9081_5_16 10_1186_1744_9081_5_16 19284600 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -A0 0R~ 23N 2VQ 2WC 3V. 4.4 53G 5GY 5VS 6J9 7X7 88E 8FI 8FJ AAFWJ AAJSJ ABDBF ABIVO ABUWG ACGFO ACGFS ACPRK ACRMQ ADBBV ADINQ ADRAZ ADUKV AENEX AFKRA AFPKN AHBYD AHMBA AHSBF AHYZX ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMKLP AMTXH AOIJS AZQEC BAPOH BAWUL BCNDV BENPR BFQNJ BMC BPHCQ BVXVI C1A C24 C6C CCPQU CS3 DIK DU5 DWQXO E3Z EBD EBLON EBS EJD ESX F5P FYUFA GNUQQ GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 H13 HMCUK HYE IAO IHR INH INR IPNFZ IPY ISR ITC KQ8 M1P M2M M48 M~E NPM O5R O5S OK1 P2P PGMZT PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ RBZ RIG RNS ROL RPM RSV SBL SOJ TR2 TUS UKHRP WOQ WOW XSB ~8M AAYXX CITATION 7X8 ABVAZ AFGXO AFNRJ 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-b614t-ff1349e3d3b9affeec2d636ad48b3a7a9846b2ef735dd14446c601e4a47db52a3 |
IEDL.DBID | RBZ |
ISSN | 1744-9081 |
IngestDate | Tue Oct 22 14:39:05 EDT 2024 Tue Sep 17 21:16:41 EDT 2024 Wed May 22 07:12:48 EDT 2024 Fri Oct 25 08:59:30 EDT 2024 Thu Sep 12 17:40:25 EDT 2024 Sat Sep 28 07:51:44 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Language | English |
License | This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-b614t-ff1349e3d3b9affeec2d636ad48b3a7a9846b2ef735dd14446c601e4a47db52a3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-16 |
PMID | 19284600 |
PQID | 733324710 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 1 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_8ab5e830745e4fc88acbdc92d8df1714 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2660355 biomedcentral_primary_oai_biomedcentral_com_1744_9081_5_16 proquest_miscellaneous_733324710 crossref_primary_10_1186_1744_9081_5_16 pubmed_primary_19284600 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2009-03-12 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2009-03-12 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 03 year: 2009 text: 2009-03-12 day: 12 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Behavioral and brain functions |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Behav Brain Funct |
PublicationYear | 2009 |
Publisher | BioMed Central Ltd BioMed Central BMC |
Publisher_xml | – name: BioMed Central Ltd – name: BioMed Central – name: BMC |
References | 15501098 - Neuroimage. 2004;23 Suppl 1:S289-99 14642472 - Neuroimage. 2003 Nov;20(3):1609-24 10827445 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2000 Jun;4(6):223-233 12372353 - Brain Cogn. 2002 Oct;50(1):73-89 12203682 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2002 Sep;17(1):1-3 12169250 - Neuroimage. 2002 Jul;16(3 Pt 1):651-62 9336221 - J Comp Neurol. 1997 Oct 20;387(2):167-78 14504419 - Neuropsychobiology. 2003;48(2):102-10 16492261 - J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006 Mar-Apr;47(3-4):296-312 15072677 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2004 Apr;16(3):415-26 1559147 - Brain. 1992 Feb;115 Pt 1:1-13 15649552 - Int J Psychophysiol. 2005 Feb;55(2):209-19 16009478 - Brain Cogn. 2005 Oct;59(1):82-95 12015233 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002 Apr;12(2):169-77 9482803 - J Neurosci. 1998 Mar 15;18(6):2188-99 17368096 - Clin Neurophysiol. 2007 May;118(5):1069-77 1559150 - Brain. 1992 Feb;115 Pt 1:15-36 11583243 - J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001 Sep;42(6):705-17 10731765 - Psychophysiology. 2000 Mar;37(2):127-52 10449260 - Brain Topogr. 1999 Summer;11(4):291-303 12371677 - Brain Topogr. 2002 Fall;15(1):51-63 10482774 - J Neurophysiol. 1999 Sep;82(3):1610-4 10491603 - Nat Neurosci. 1999 Oct;2(10):861-3 10943724 - Neuroreport. 2000 Aug 3;11(11):2571-6 12853131 - Int J Psychophysiol. 2003 Jul;49(1):67-74 15222855 - Emotion. 2004 Jun;4(2):189-200 16407774 - Neuroreport. 2006 Feb 6;17(2):215-9 14561449 - Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003 Oct;17(3):613-20 16513007 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2006 Mar;18(3):430-43 11275483 - Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2001 Apr;11(2):213-26 8019525 - J Physiol Paris. 1994;88(1):51-80 7511504 - Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994 Mar;90(3):229-41 16019117 - Brain Cogn. 2005 Oct;59(1):96-100 14534432 - Neuroreport. 2003 Oct 6;14(14):1841-5 16310813 - Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(11):2139-48 11467919 - Neuroimage. 2001 Aug;14(2):465-73 15666336 - Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2004;10(4):259-71 16472321 - Dev Sci. 2006 Mar;9(2):207-20 12943325 - Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003 Jun;3(2):97-110 11930154 - Neuroreport. 2002 Mar 25;13(4):427-31 15003769 - Clin Neurophysiol. 2004 Apr;115(4):880-6 11506548 - Neuroimage. 2001 Sep;14(3):759-67 11499988 - Neuropsychology. 2001 Jul;15(3):329-41 10943704 - Neuroreport. 2000 Aug 3;11(11):2461-5 14629922 - Int J Psychophysiol. 2003 Dec;51(1):45-58 11277569 - Neuroreport. 2001 Mar 26;12(4):709-14 16178938 - J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005 Oct;46(10):1135-8 3756376 - Br J Psychol. 1986 Aug;77 ( Pt 3):305-27 10491602 - Nat Neurosci. 1999 Oct;2(10):859-61 10639397 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Jan;10(1):69-81 11931516 - Psychol Bull. 2002 Mar;128(2):203-35 11264671 - Eur J Neurosci. 2001 Mar;13(5):987-94 10771410 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Mar;12(2):255-66 15335344 - J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004 Oct;45(7):1235-45 12705432 - Clin Neurophysiol. 2003 Mar;114(3):515-28 12464327 - Clin Neurophysiol. 2002 Dec;113(12):1903-8 10680766 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2000 Feb;9(2):93-105 10407202 - Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1999 Jul 16;8(2):125-42 1506820 - J Exp Child Psychol. 1992 Aug;54(1):1-18 15106860 - Neuroreport. 2004 Jan 19;15(1):211-6 9059786 - Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1996;46:127-37 11784442 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2001 Nov 15;13(8):1035-47 15595374 - Dev Sci. 2004 Jun;7(3):340-59 12689381 - Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003 Mar 29;358(1431):561-72 11408051 - Psychiatry Res. 2001 Jun 1;102(2):109-24 15509388 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2004 Oct;16(8):1426-42 14527602 - Neuroimage. 2003 Sep;20(1):420-8 11798271 - Neuroimage. 2002 Feb;15(2):353-72 |
References_xml | |
SSID | ssj0038422 |
Score | 2.0416138 |
Snippet | Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the precise... BACKGROUNDDespite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the... BACKGROUND: Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past, the... Abstract Background Despite extensive investigation of the neural systems for face perception and emotion recognition in adults and young children in the past,... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral biomedcentral proquest crossref pubmed |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | 16 |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LbxMxEB6hHlAvCFIey0s-IDhZTfxab28FtaqQihBQqTfLTxEpbKI2PeTfM7Y3IQtIXLja-_DujD3f2DPfALyxjqXgfaJaukAFZ5bakDxtk7eMe-mcz8nJl5_UxZX4eC2v90p95ZiwSg9cf9yxtk5GjZooZBTJa229C75jQYeUi3eX1XfabZ2pugZzLcr5AcJtQTu0egNd40yr410blTRXOR_luS9G5qmw-P8Nev4eQblnks4fwoMBS5LT-g2P4F7sJ3B02qMf_WND3pIS3Vm2zSdw_3I4RJ_A4W7R2xxB_FpCqgeGqgUJ89VyEUnd0yfF0g2ZmmSZSLLYuKq5BWjzyNmXz7dk3pM9XqgTYvGKuCjVwm42pPDXPoar87NvHy7oUHqBOrTXa5pSpi2MPHDX2ZRi9CwormwQ2nHb2g5hi2MxtVyGgD6ZUB49uyisaIOTzPIncNAv-_gMiJZtG5XNGfdTETqFkK6LyrPoJL5kJhs4GUnArCrNhsnE1-MenIMmi89k8RlpZqqBd1tx7e4rbo1Wf1z5Pktz9PTSgMpmBmUz_1K2BshWFwxOw3y2Yvu4vLs1LecITRGuNfC0qsav8XQIARBXNtCOlGY0lHFPP_9emL4RPU0RED7_H2N_AYf1JIzTGXsJB-ubu_gKAdXavS5z5yfIESKM priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – databaseName: PubMed Central dbid: RPM link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9QwELbaHlAviG55BAryAcHJ3a5fcXorVasKaVEFVOrN8hNWyiar7faw_56xk5QGOHGN7cTKfPbM2DPfIPTeWBq9c5EoYT3hjBpifHSkjM5Q5oS1LiUnz7_Iqxv--Vbc7iAx5MLkoH1nF8dNvTxuFj9zbOVq6aZDnNj0en4OSuUE9OR0F-0CQAcXvdt-meKU9uyMMyWnYHFzUoHiI4LMcsGiCnZkmTLaRhnu9UgxZf7-fxmdf8ZOPlJGl8_Q096KxGfdbA_QTmgm6PCsAQ96ucUfcI7rzAfmE_Rk3l-fT9D-w3a3PUThWw6m7rmpauwXq7YOuDvNx1nH9TmauI04Gni46rIKQNvhi6_Xd3jR4EeMUKfYQI9Q5zph6y3OzLXP0c3lxffzK9IXXSAWNPWGxJgICwPzzFYmxhAc9ZJJ47myzJSmgp9naYglE96DN8alA58ucMNLbwU17AXaa9omvEJYibIM0qRc-xPuKwnGXBWko8EK-MhMFOh0JAG96gg2dKK8HrfA6tNJkjpJUgs9kwX6OIjrYVx2aJT8q-enJM3R2_ODdv1D97DSylgRFGx4XAQenVLGWe8q6pWPqUZ8gfCABQ0LMN2qmCa093e6ZAyMUjDUCvSyg8bv-fRQK1A5As1oKuMWgHzm-O4h_vq_R75B-93FFyMzeoT2Nuv78Bbsp419l9fLL8yXICU priority: 500 providerName: National Library of Medicine – databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals (Open Access) dbid: M48 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Lb9QwELagSKgXBFseKQ_5gOBk2PUrTiWECmpVIRUhYKXeLD9hpZAsu1up--8ZO9lt0_bINbYTK-PxfGPPfIPQa2Np9M5FooT1hDNqiPHRkTI6Q5kT1rqUnHz6VZ5M-ZczcXYZ_9T_wOWtrl2qJzVd1O8u_q4_gsJ_yAqv5HsA1ZxUYNuIIBN5F92jHLz0FMbHtzcKTPF8o7Dt2xM43hx_LfO9HhiszOt_Gxi9HlN5xUgdP0QPenSJD7vl8AjdCc0I7R024Fn_WeM3OMd75oP0Ebp_2l-rj9Dudhtc76HwIwdZ95xVNfazeVsH3J3y42z7-txN3EYcDTycd9kGYAXx0fdvSzxr8BWmqANsoEeoc_2wxRpnRtvHaHp89PPzCemLMRALFnxFYkxEhoF5ZisTYwiOesmk8VxZZkpTAZCxNMSSCe_BS-PSga8XuOGlt4Ia9gTtNG0TniGsRFkGaVIO_pj7SgLIq4J0NFgBH5mIAh0MJKDnHfGGTlTYwxbQSp3Ep5P4tNATWaC3G3Ftx2VHR8kbPT8laQ7enh-0i1-611utjBVBwUbIReDRKWWc9a6iXvmYascXCG_WggbFTLctpgnt-VKXjAFYBQBXoKfd0ricTwWgAJBmgcrBohlMZdjSzH5n7m_AU2OAiPv_Y-7P0e7mhHw8eYF2Vovz8BIg1sq-yrrzD1o6KLQ priority: 102 providerName: Scholars Portal |
Title | Spatiotemporal dipole source localization of face processing ERPs in adolescents: a preliminary study |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19284600 https://search.proquest.com/docview/733324710 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-16 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC2660355 https://doaj.org/article/8ab5e830745e4fc88acbdc92d8df1714 |
Volume | 5 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1La9tAEB6aBEoupXX6UB9iD6U9LY21D61yi4tDKDiEtAHTi9gnNTiySZyD_31nV7ITOb31osM-pEWzu_PNzM63AJ-1KYKzNlAljKOcFZpqFywtg9UFs8IYG5OTJxfy_Jr_mIrpg79jJ4I_VPIbQmZOK9RcVNCh3IODIhKqRLt89Huz5zLFU7xg27ajZ3zafyevfd5TR4m1_19Qc_fE5CMVdPYSXnTYkZy2wn4Fz3wzgKPTBu3mmzX5QtJpzuQmH8DzSRc0H8DhdpNbH4H_mY5Qd4xUc-Jmy8Xck9aHT5Jm6zIzySKQoLFw2eYSoI4j46vLOzJryCMeqBOisYWfp9vBbtck8dW-huuz8a_v57S7aoEa1M8rGkKkKfTMMVPpELy3hZNMaseVYbrUFcIUU_hQMuEc2mBcWrTkPNe8dEYUmr2B_WbR-HdAlChLL3XMsD_mrpII4SovbeGNwI8MRQYnPQnUy5ZWo45E1_0aXHN1FF8dxVeLeigz-LoR17ZfMmOUfNJyFKXZe3sqwLlVd6uyVtoIr3Cb48LzYJXS1jhbFU65EG-Gz4Bs5kKNyy7GUnTjF_d3dckYQlGEZxm8bafGw3gqVPmIIzMoe5OmN5R-TTP7k5i9ES0dIwB8_z-_6AMcthEvRofFR9hf3d77TwicViaHvXJa5nAwGl9cXuXJ_YDPCVd5Wk158nX9BQOBHug |
link.rule.ids | 108,230,315,730,783,787,867,888,2109,2228,24330,24949,27936,27937,31732,33757,53804,53806,76140,76141 |
linkProvider | BioMedCentral |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1LbxMxEB6VIkEvCFIe4ekDgpNFs36st7cWtSrQVKg0UsXF8hMihU2Upof8e8beTZptuXH1Y9fyjGe-sT2fAd4bW0TvXKRKWE85Kww1PjpaRmcK5oS1LiUnD8_kyYh_vRSXWzBa5cLcJKdjIG3TKwnJvjci2ExIn2Qrvro81qx9JT8hvua0QjdHBR3Ie3Afo3Se1uv54c-VgWaK58OFdduWy_Fu_1tJ8JOO78oU___CpbevV274q-PH8KgFmuSgGfsT2Ap1D3YPagyy_yzJB5KvfuY99R48GLYn7D3YWVvE5S6EH_m-dUtfNSF-PJtOAmk2_El2g20aJ5lGEg0WzprEA3SI5Oj8-xUZ12SDNGqfGGwRJvkpsfmSZHLbpzA6Prr4fELbdxmoxdlc0BgTp2FgntnKxBiCK7xk0niuLDOlqRDT2CLEkgnvMWDj0mHYF7jhpbeiMOwZbNfTOrwAokRZBmlSOv4e95VEvFcF6YpgBf5kIPqw35GAnjUcHDqxYndrUB90Ep9O4tNCD2QfPq7Ete6XYx4l77Q8TNLsfD0XTOe_dKtkWhkrgkKbyEXg0SllnPWuKrzyMT0j3wey0gWNazQdvJg6TK-vdMkY4lbEcn143qjGzXgqxAcIOvtQdpSmM5RuTT3-nWnAEVrtIVp8-T9T9A4enlwMT_Xpl7Nvr2CnOSpjdFC8hu3F_Dq8QcS1sG_z-vkLIsAu_A |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1Lb9QwEB6VIlW9INgCXZ4-IDhZ7caPOL210FV5tKoKRRUXy09YsWRX7faw_56xk91uWm7cIsdOosx45hvb8w3AG2OL6J2LVAnrKWeFocZHR8voTMGcsNal5OTjE3l0zj9diIs1-L7IhblJTsdA2qYqCcm-NyJYTUgfZyuOF-73ztTHZvIruYMAm9MK_RwVdCDvwX0M03kq6XB28GNhoZnieXdh2bclc7w7_lYW_LjjvDLH_7-A6e3zlSsOa_gQHrRIk-w3qvEI1kLdg639GqPsP3PyluSzn3lRvQcbx-0Wew82lyZxvgXhaz5w3fJXjYkfTSfjQJoVf5L9YJvHSSaRRION0ybzAD0iOTw7vSKjmqywRu0Rgz3CONcSu5yTzG77GM6Hh9_eH9G2MAO1-DdnNMZEahiYZ7YyMYbgCi-ZNJ4ry0xpKgQ1tgixZMJ7jNi4dBj3BW546a0oDHsC6_WkDttAlCjLIE3Kx9_lvpII-KogXRGswJcMRB_2OhLQ04aEQyda7O4dVAidxKeT-LTQA9mHdwtxLcfloEfJOz0PkjQ7T88Nk8ufup3DWhkrgkKjyEXg0SllnPWuKrzyMdWR7wNZ6ILGSZp2XkwdJtdXumQMgSuCuT48bVTj5nsqBAiIOvtQdpSm8yndO_XoV-YBR2y1i3Dx2f_8otewcfphqL98PPn8HDabrTJGB8ULWJ9dXoeXiLhm9lWePn8Bmakuxw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Spatiotemporal+dipole+source+localization+of+face+processing+ERPs+in+adolescents%3A+a+preliminary+study&rft.jtitle=Behavioral+and+brain+functions&rft.au=McAlonan+Grainne&rft.au=Fung+Peter&rft.au=Wong+Teresa&rft.au=Chua+Siew&rft.date=2009-03-12&rft.pub=BMC&rft.issn=1744-9081&rft.eissn=1744-9081&rft.volume=5&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=16&rft_id=info:doi/10.1186%2F1744-9081-5-16&rft.externalDBID=DOA&rft.externalDocID=oai_doaj_org_article_8ab5e830745e4fc88acbdc92d8df1714 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1744-9081&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1744-9081&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1744-9081&client=summon |