On the effectiveness of self-paced learning

Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners’ control. Here we investigate...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inJournal of memory and language Vol. 64; no. 2; pp. 109 - 118
Main Authors Tullis, Jonathan G., Benjamin, Aaron S.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Amsterdam Elsevier Inc 01.02.2011
Elsevier
Elsevier BV
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0749-596X
1096-0821
DOI10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002

Cover

Abstract Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners’ control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy-reduction strategy—that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used.
AbstractList Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners' control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy-reduction strategy--that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used. (Contains 3 figures and 1 table.)
Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners’ control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy reduction strategy—that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used.
Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners' control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy-reduction strategy - that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used. [Copyright Elsevier Inc.]
Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners’ control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy-reduction strategy—that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used.
Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners' control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy reduction strategy-that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used.Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners' control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy reduction strategy-that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used.
Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist in which allowing learners to control learning produces higher levels of performance than restricting learners' control. Here we investigate the consequences of allowing learners to self-pace study of a list of words on later recognition, and show that learners with control of study-time allocation significantly outperformed subjects with no control, even when the total study time was equated between groups (Experiments 1 and 2). The self-pacing group also outperformed a group for which study time was automatically allocated as a function of normative item difficulty (Experiment 2). The advantage of self-pacing was apparent only in subjects who utilized a discrepancy-reduction strategy--that is, who allocated more study time to normatively difficult items. Self-pacing can improve memory performance, but only when appropriate allocation strategies are used. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
Author Benjamin, Aaron S.
Tullis, Jonathan G.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Jonathan G.
  surname: Tullis
  fullname: Tullis, Jonathan G.
  email: jtullis2@illinois.edu
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Aaron S.
  surname: Benjamin
  fullname: Benjamin, Aaron S.
BackLink http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ911668$$DView record in ERIC
http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=23840237$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21516194$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kl2L1DAYhYOsuLOrP0AQKcKiIB3zps1HERZkWb9Y2BsF70KavtlN6aRj0hnw35sy46h7MVelnOccDm_OGTkJY0BCngNdAgXxrl_2q2HJ6PwPS0rZI7IA2oiSKgYnZEFl3ZS8ET9OyVlKPaUAXLIn5JQBBwFNvSBvb0Mx3WOBzqGd_BYDplSMrkg4uHJtLHbFgCYGH-6eksfODAmf7b_n5PvH629Xn8ub209frj7clIareipFq6zKNTqgtGraSnUtbRsHlWtdXYEEo6xFWaEC7rhA1XFmOu4wU1Kwujonl7vc9aZdYWcxTNEMeh39ysRfejRe_68Ef6_vxq2uqGwYFzng9T4gjj83mCa98sniMJiA4yZpJZiUQgqayTdHSVBMgJB1PYe-eoD24yaGfAitKg4MFJ_zXv5b_dD5z8EzcLEHTLJmcNEE69NfrlI1ZZXM3Isdh9Hbg3z9tQEQQmUZdrKNY0oR3QEBqudt6F7nbeh5GxpA521kj3zgsX4ykx_nI_rhqPP9vkx-9a3HqJP1GPI2fMyr0d3oj7h_AwEgz7k
CODEN JMLAE6
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1109_TCSVT_2023_3294963
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jclinepi_2024_111635
crossref_primary_10_21815_JDE_018_122
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2018_05_007
crossref_primary_10_26102_2310_6018_2019_27_4_023
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01259
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_future_2023_03_013
crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_2281
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2017_03_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2018_03_001
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_016_0664_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijme_2022_100699
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41539_020_0061_1
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0132207
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12528_022_09342_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_iree_2014_08_003
crossref_primary_10_1093_geronb_gbae061
crossref_primary_10_3389_frvir_2021_670909
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_014_0409_y
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_021_01245_3
crossref_primary_10_1057_s41599_023_02590_1
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_015_0517_3
crossref_primary_10_1007_s44186_023_00174_w
crossref_primary_10_1177_0194599816677697
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_023_00496_9
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ins_2019_12_015
crossref_primary_10_1080_1533015X_2024_2436198
crossref_primary_10_4103_jehp_jehp_1177_23
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2018_1506457
crossref_primary_10_1111_bjep_12166
crossref_primary_10_52134_ueader_1064312
crossref_primary_10_1055_s_0041_1735868
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_diin_2018_04_021
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2012_09_012
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2023_e16348
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_021_09645_2
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_022_01382_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_profnurs_2017_08_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_fsidi_2023_301566
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02078
crossref_primary_10_1109_TCSVT_2024_3491907
crossref_primary_10_4085_1947_380X_20_87
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2015_09_010
crossref_primary_10_1108_IJILT_11_2017_0116
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_023_00511_z
crossref_primary_10_1145_3582571
crossref_primary_10_1177_2372732218788092
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41539_022_00142_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_020_09219_7
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_015_0516_4
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01074_w
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_022_02025_w
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2012_04_023
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2019_101237
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_017_9173_2
crossref_primary_10_1007_s40012_022_00367_6
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_020_09223_x
crossref_primary_10_5688_ajpe6179
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_018_0842_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2013_06_003
crossref_primary_10_18178_ijiet_2022_12_12_1758
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10758_024_09746_1
crossref_primary_10_1109_TCSVT_2023_3304567
crossref_primary_10_1177_2372732215601116
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_012_0266_2
crossref_primary_10_1027_1618_3169_a000131
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2016_10_008
crossref_primary_10_1111_bjop_12123
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2017_1311974
crossref_primary_10_1155_2021_6648550
crossref_primary_10_1080_18146627_2022_2147847
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ridd_2019_04_001
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01950_9
crossref_primary_10_1109_TPAMI_2021_3069908
crossref_primary_10_1111_mbe_12117
crossref_primary_10_1002_ets2_12270
crossref_primary_10_1109_TPAMI_2024_3401036
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2022_07_010
crossref_primary_10_3102_0034654314526064
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_020_09578_2
crossref_primary_10_1080_13562517_2014_962505
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0070906
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2016_05_084
crossref_primary_10_1080_08995605_2021_2002104
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11251_015_9356_7
crossref_primary_10_3390_su16124923
crossref_primary_10_1177_0018720813517710
crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_2096
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_017_0729_9
crossref_primary_10_52634_mier_2024_v14_i2_2699
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_012_0274_5
crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_1854
crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_372
crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence12090084
crossref_primary_10_1007_s42113_018_0006_4
crossref_primary_10_1080_09658211_2015_1069853
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2015_07_011
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_014_0478_y
crossref_primary_10_5606_fng_btd_2021_25054
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10639_024_12524_2
crossref_primary_10_1177_1474704919834502
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0152115
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neucom_2024_128366
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2015_1057148
crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence10040083
crossref_primary_10_1103_PhysRevSTPER_11_020128
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0248758
crossref_primary_10_4236_ce_2014_513135
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_019_09195_7
crossref_primary_10_1080_01587919_2016_1233050
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_022_09307_w
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2024_2555
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2302898
crossref_primary_10_1080_15391523_2016_1175856
crossref_primary_10_1080_1046560X_2022_2112552
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2014_966122
crossref_primary_10_54855_ictep_24514
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41239_019_0147_0
crossref_primary_10_1080_09658211_2023_2212429
crossref_primary_10_3390_bs14070567
crossref_primary_10_5005_jp_journals_10082_03112
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neucom_2022_07_055
crossref_primary_10_1111_jasp_12637
crossref_primary_10_1002_tl_20247
crossref_primary_10_1080_00219266_2024_2386250
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01120_7
crossref_primary_10_1080_09658211_2021_1979585
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_019_00899_4
Cites_doi 10.3758/BF03194388
10.3758/BF03210921
10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.36
10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.609
10.1037/a0016371
10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01628.x
10.1037/0278-7393.26.1.204
10.1016/0022-0965(89)90033-7
10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.001
10.3758/BF03211311
10.1080/00221325.1988.10532142
10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.66
10.3758/BF03202594
10.1080/00221325.1993.9914736
10.3758/BF03196779
10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.643
10.1037/0278-7393.2.1.21
10.2307/747487
10.1016/0001-6918(72)90012-1
10.3758/PBR.15.1.174
10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.851
10.1037/h0028369
10.1016/j.jml.2006.02.003
10.1126/science.1152408
10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.530
10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.601
10.1037/a0014351
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x
10.1111/1467-8721.01235
10.3758/BF03201175
10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55
10.1006/jmla.1994.1026
10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90389-3
10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.160
10.3758/BF03329756
10.3758/MC.37.1.52
10.1016/0010-0285(71)90024-7
10.3758/BF03195868
10.1037/0096-3445.122.1.47
10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00502.x
10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00051-6
10.3758/BF03194055
10.1037/0278-7393.18.1.116
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2010 Elsevier Inc.
2015 INIST-CNRS
Copyright © 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 2010
Copyright_xml – notice: 2010 Elsevier Inc.
– notice: 2015 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Copyright © 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
– notice: 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 2010
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
IQODW
NPM
7T9
8BM
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002
DatabaseName CrossRef
ERIC
ERIC (Ovid)
ERIC
ERIC
ERIC (Legacy Platform)
ERIC( SilverPlatter )
ERIC
ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)
Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
Pascal-Francis
PubMed
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
ComDisDome
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ERIC
PubMed
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
ComDisDome
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList ERIC

Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)

MEDLINE - Academic
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
PubMed
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: ERI
  name: ERIC
  url: https://eric.ed.gov/
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Languages & Literatures
Education
Psychology
EISSN 1096-0821
ERIC EJ911668
EndPage 118
ExternalDocumentID PMC3079256
2235570831
21516194
23840237
EJ911668
10_1016_j_jml_2010_11_002
S0749596X10000999
Genre Journal Article
Feature
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 AG026263
GroupedDBID --K
--M
--Z
-DZ
-~X
.GJ
.~1
07C
0R3
0R~
186
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29L
3EH
4.4
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
7-5
71M
85S
8P~
9JM
9JO
AABNK
AACLR
AACTN
AADFP
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFJI
AAGJA
AAGJQ
AAGUQ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AATGF
AAWTL
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABGZZ
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABMMH
ABOYX
ABPPZ
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACFII
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACIOK
ACMRT
ACNCT
ACREJ
ACRLP
ACXNI
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADFGL
ADIYS
ADMUD
ADPVW
ADSFD
AEBSH
AEBYS
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AETEA
AFFDN
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AFYLN
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJGPC
AJOXV
AKYCK
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
AOMHK
ASPBG
AVARZ
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BKOMP
BLXMC
CAG
COF
CS3
DM4
EBS
ECVKH
EFBJH
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
G8K
GBLVA
HMW
HMY
HVGLF
HYQOX
HZ~
IHE
J1W
K-O
KC5
KOM
LG5
LPU
M3U
M3X
M41
MO0
MOBAO
MVM
N9A
O-F
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OHT
OKEIE
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
PQQKQ
PRBVW
Q38
Q5E
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SDF
SDG
SES
SEW
SPCBC
SPS
SSB
SSN
SSO
SSS
SSY
SSZ
T5K
TN5
UNMZH
WH7
WUQ
XJT
XOL
XPP
XSW
XZL
YK3
YQT
YYQ
ZA5
ZCA
ZCG
ZMT
ZU3
ZXP
~G-
~OB
~OC
~OG
~PJ
~PM
~PN
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDPE
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADMHG
ADNMO
ADVLN
ADXHL
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
PMKZF
PVKVW
SSH
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
EFKBS
ERI
PET
PMFND
REK
WWN
IQODW
NPM
7T9
8BM
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-a584t-6b8c8109d10039b38db0b9f13fbf43171a8cce73e815f56e8d52ad5fe0b976243
IEDL.DBID AIKHN
ISSN 0749-596X
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 17:49:09 EDT 2025
Fri Sep 05 02:45:43 EDT 2025
Thu Sep 04 17:26:28 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 14 07:41:32 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 06:04:07 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 09:17:15 EDT 2025
Tue Sep 02 19:09:49 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:01:51 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:51:44 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:32:37 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 2
Keywords Metacognitive control
Self-pacing
Memory
Metacognition
Self learning
Human
Cognition
Time
Self administration
Experimental study
Learning
Acquisition process
Metamemory
Cognitive control
Language English
License https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0
CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-a584t-6b8c8109d10039b38db0b9f13fbf43171a8cce73e815f56e8d52ad5fe0b976243
Notes SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/3079256
PMID 21516194
PQID 835121850
PQPubID 29955
PageCount 10
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3079256
proquest_miscellaneous_862776760
proquest_miscellaneous_1826167446
proquest_journals_835121850
pubmed_primary_21516194
pascalfrancis_primary_23840237
eric_primary_EJ911668
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2010_11_002
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_jml_2010_11_002
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_jml_2010_11_002
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2011-02-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2011-02-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 02
  year: 2011
  text: 2011-02-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace Amsterdam
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Amsterdam
– name: United States
– name: New York
PublicationTitle Journal of memory and language
PublicationTitleAlternate J Mem Lang
PublicationYear 2011
Publisher Elsevier Inc
Elsevier
Elsevier BV
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Inc
– name: Elsevier
– name: Elsevier BV
References Dunlosky, Connor (b0045) 1997; 25
Dunlosky, Theide (b0065) 1998; 98
Metcalfe, Kornell (b0175) 2005; 52
Cohen (b0245) 1988
Dunlosky, Nelson (b0250) 1994; 33
Benjamin, Bjork, Schwarz (b0035) 1998; 127
Flavell (b0080) 1979; 34
Son (b0205) 2004; 30
Benjamin, Bird (b0030) 2006; 55
Dufresne, Kobasigawa (b0055) 1989; 47
Koriat, Ma’ayan, Nussinson (b0120) 2006; 135
Toppino, Cohen, Davis, Moors (b0220) 2009; 35
Maki, Serra (b0145) 1992; 18
Hacker, Bol, Horgan, Rakow (b0100) 2000; 92
Dunning, Johnson, Ehrlinger, Kruger (b0070) 2003; 12
Guttentag, Carroll (b0095) 1998; 26
Dunlosky, Nelson (b0060) 1992; 20
Matzen, Benjamin (b0155) 2009; 37
Metcalfe, Schwartz, Joaquim (b0180) 1993
Zacks (b0230) 1969; 32
Metcalfe, Finn (b0165) 2008; 15
Nelson, Dunlosky, Graf, Narens (b0185) 1994; 5
Kornell, Bjork (b0125) 2007; 14
Benjamin (b0020) 2005; 12
Dunlosky, Theide (b0255) 2004; 32
Ny Le, Denheire, Taillanter (b0135) 1972; 36
Roediger, Karpicke (b0195) 2006; 17
Son, Metcalfe (b0210) 2000; 26
Mazzoni, Cornoldi (b0150) 1993; 122
Benjamin, Diaz, Wee (b0040) 2009; 116
Metcalfe (b0160) 2009; 18
Baker, Anderson (b0005) 1982; 17
Benjamin (b0025) 2008; Vol. 48
Shaughnessy (b0200) 1981; 20
Finley, Tullis, Benjamin (b0075) 2009
Belmont, Butterfield (b0010) 1971; 2
Metcalfe, Kornell (b0170) 2003; 132
Kobasigawa, Metcalf-Heggart (b0110) 1993; 154
Zechmeister, Shaughnessy (b0235) 1980; 15
Karpicke, Roediger (b0105) 2008; 319
Koriat, Bjork, Sheffer, Bar (b0115) 2004; 143
Dufresne, Kobasigawa (b0050) 1988; 149
Green, Swets (b0090) 1966
Wilson (b0225) 1988; 20
Glanzer, Bowles (b0085) 1976; 2
Nelson, Narens (b0190) 1994
Thiede, Anderson, Therriault (b0215) 2003; 95
Kornell, Metcalfe (b0130) 2006; 32
Benjamin (b0015) 2003; 31
Macmillan, Creelman (b0140) 2005
Zacks (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0230) 1969; 32
Kornell (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0125) 2007; 14
Belmont (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0010) 1971; 2
Dunlosky (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0045) 1997; 25
Benjamin (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0030) 2006; 55
Dunlosky (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0255) 2004; 32
Finley (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0075) 2009
Cohen (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0245) 1988
Dufresne (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0055) 1989; 47
Baker (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0005) 1982; 17
Son (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0205) 2004; 30
Son (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0210) 2000; 26
Maki (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0145) 1992; 18
Benjamin (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0015) 2003; 31
Toppino (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0220) 2009; 35
Metcalfe (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0175) 2005; 52
Dunlosky (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0250) 1994; 33
Metcalfe (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0160) 2009; 18
Wilson (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0225) 1988; 20
Glanzer (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0085) 1976; 2
Ny Le (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0135) 1972; 36
Nelson (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0190) 1994
Karpicke (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0105) 2008; 319
Thiede (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0215) 2003; 95
Dunning (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0070) 2003; 12
Koriat (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0120) 2006; 135
Flavell (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0080) 1979; 34
Hacker (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0100) 2000; 92
Metcalfe (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0180) 1993
Nelson (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0185) 1994; 5
Benjamin (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0040) 2009; 116
Kornell (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0130) 2006; 32
Kobasigawa (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0110) 1993; 154
Matzen (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0155) 2009; 37
Guttentag (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0095) 1998; 26
Benjamin (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0035) 1998; 127
Dufresne (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0050) 1988; 149
Zechmeister (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0235) 1980; 15
Dunlosky (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0065) 1998; 98
Macmillan (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0140) 2005
Metcalfe (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0170) 2003; 132
Benjamin (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0020) 2005; 12
Metcalfe (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0165) 2008; 15
Shaughnessy (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0200) 1981; 20
Benjamin (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0025) 2008; Vol. 48
Koriat (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0115) 2004; 143
Green (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0090) 1966
Mazzoni (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0150) 1993; 122
Dunlosky (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0060) 1992; 20
Roediger (10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0195) 2006; 17
References_xml – volume: 18
  start-page: 159
  year: 2009
  end-page: 163
  ident: b0160
  article-title: Metacognitive judgments and control of study
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
– volume: 2
  start-page: 411
  year: 1971
  end-page: 420
  ident: b0010
  article-title: Learning strategies as determinants of memory deficiencies
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 32
  start-page: 779
  year: 2004
  end-page: 788
  ident: b0255
  article-title: Causes and constraints of the shift-to-easier-materials effect in the control of study
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
– volume: 154
  start-page: 223
  year: 1993
  end-page: 235
  ident: b0110
  article-title: Spontaneous allocation of study-time by first- and third-grade children on a simple memory task
  publication-title: The Journal of Genetic Psychology
– volume: 98
  start-page: 37
  year: 1998
  end-page: 56
  ident: b0065
  article-title: What makes people study more? An evaluation of factors that affect self-paced study
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
– volume: 12
  start-page: 83
  year: 2003
  end-page: 87
  ident: b0070
  article-title: Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
– year: 1966
  ident: b0090
  article-title: Signal detection theory and psychophysics
– volume: 37
  start-page: 52
  year: 2009
  end-page: 64
  ident: b0155
  article-title: Remembering words not presented in sentences: How study context changes patterns of false memories
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
– volume: 33
  start-page: 545
  year: 1994
  end-page: 565
  ident: b0250
  article-title: Does the sensitivity of judgments of learning (JOLs) to the effects of various study activities depend on when the JOLs occur?
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 20
  start-page: 374
  year: 1992
  end-page: 380
  ident: b0060
  article-title: Importance of the kind of cue for judgements of learning (JOLs) and the delayed JOL effect
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
– volume: 36
  start-page: 280
  year: 1972
  end-page: 289
  ident: b0135
  article-title: Regulation of study-time and interstimulus similarity in self-paced learning conditions
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
– volume: 52
  start-page: 463
  year: 2005
  end-page: 477
  ident: b0175
  article-title: A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 116
  start-page: 84
  year: 2009
  end-page: 114
  ident: b0040
  article-title: Signal detection with criterion noise: Applications to recognition memory
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 15
  start-page: 174
  year: 2008
  end-page: 179
  ident: b0165
  article-title: Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
– volume: 14
  start-page: 219
  year: 2007
  end-page: 224
  ident: b0125
  article-title: The promises and perils of self-regulated study
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1352
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1358
  ident: b0220
  article-title: Metacognitive control over the distribution of practice. When is spacing preferred?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 132
  start-page: 530
  year: 2003
  end-page: 542
  ident: b0170
  article-title: The dynamics of learning and the allocation of study time to a region of proximal learning
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 15
  start-page: 41
  year: 1980
  end-page: 44
  ident: b0235
  article-title: When you know that you know and when you think that you know but you don’t
  publication-title: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
– volume: 32
  start-page: 441
  year: 1969
  end-page: 447
  ident: b0230
  article-title: Invariance of total learning time under different conditions of practice
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 30
  start-page: 601
  year: 2004
  end-page: 604
  ident: b0205
  article-title: Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 32
  start-page: 609
  year: 2006
  end-page: 622
  ident: b0130
  article-title: Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– start-page: 195
  year: 1994
  end-page: 206
  ident: b0190
  article-title: Why investigate metacognition?
  publication-title: Metacognition: Knowing about knowing
– volume: 135
  start-page: 36
  year: 2006
  end-page: 60
  ident: b0120
  article-title: The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: Lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 5
  start-page: 207
  year: 1994
  end-page: 213
  ident: b0185
  article-title: Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 26
  start-page: 204
  year: 2000
  end-page: 221
  ident: b0210
  article-title: Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation
  publication-title: Journal of Experiment Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 2
  start-page: 21
  year: 1976
  end-page: 31
  ident: b0085
  article-title: Analysis of the word frequency effect in recognition memory
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory
– volume: 20
  start-page: 216
  year: 1981
  end-page: 230
  ident: b0200
  article-title: Memory monitoring accuracy and modification of rehearsal strategies
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
– volume: 95
  start-page: 66
  year: 2003
  end-page: 73
  ident: b0215
  article-title: Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
– volume: 31
  start-page: 297
  year: 2003
  end-page: 305
  ident: b0015
  article-title: Predicting and postdicting the effects of word frequency on memory
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
– year: 1988
  ident: b0245
  article-title: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
– volume: Vol. 48
  start-page: 175
  year: 2008
  end-page: 223
  ident: b0025
  article-title: Memory is more than just remembering: Strategic control of encoding, accessing memory, and making decisions
  publication-title: The psychology of learning and motivation: Skill and strategy in memory use
– volume: 122
  start-page: 47
  year: 1993
  end-page: 60
  ident: b0150
  article-title: Strategies in study time allocation: Why is study time sometimes not effective?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 26
  start-page: 951
  year: 1998
  end-page: 958
  ident: b0095
  article-title: Memorability judgments for high- and low-frequency words
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
– volume: 127
  start-page: 55
  year: 1998
  end-page: 68
  ident: b0035
  article-title: The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 18
  start-page: 116
  year: 1992
  end-page: 126
  ident: b0145
  article-title: The basis of test predictions for text material
  publication-title: The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 17
  start-page: 281
  year: 1982
  end-page: 294
  ident: b0005
  article-title: Effects of inconsistent information on text processing: Evidence for comprehension monitoring
  publication-title: Reading Research Quarterly
– volume: 143
  start-page: 643
  year: 2004
  end-page: 656
  ident: b0115
  article-title: Predicting one’s own forgetting: The role of experience-based and theory-based processes
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– year: 2005
  ident: b0140
  article-title: Detection theory
– volume: 92
  start-page: 160
  year: 2000
  end-page: 170
  ident: b0100
  article-title: Test prediction and performance in a classroom context
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
– volume: 55
  start-page: 126
  year: 2006
  end-page: 137
  ident: b0030
  article-title: Metacognitive control of the spacing of study repetitions
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 12
  start-page: 874
  year: 2005
  end-page: 879
  ident: b0020
  article-title: Response speeding mediates the contribution of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
– volume: 47
  start-page: 274
  year: 1989
  end-page: 296
  ident: b0055
  article-title: Children’s spontaneous allocation of study time: Differential and sufficient aspects
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
– volume: 319
  start-page: 966
  year: 2008
  end-page: 968
  ident: b0105
  article-title: The critical importance of retrieval for learning
  publication-title: Science
– start-page: 851
  year: 1993
  end-page: 861
  ident: b0180
  article-title: The cue-familiarity heuristic in metacognition
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 17
  start-page: 249
  year: 2006
  end-page: 255
  ident: b0195
  article-title: Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long term retention
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 149
  start-page: 87
  year: 1988
  end-page: 92
  ident: b0050
  article-title: Developmental differences in children’s allocation of study time
  publication-title: The Journal of Genetic Psychology
– year: 2009
  ident: b0075
  article-title: Metacognitive control of learning and remembering
  publication-title: New science of learning: Cognition, computers and collaboration in education
– volume: 34
  start-page: 906
  year: 1979
  end-page: 911
  ident: b0080
  article-title: Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-development theory
  publication-title: American Psychologist
– volume: 25
  start-page: 691
  year: 1997
  end-page: 700
  ident: b0045
  article-title: Age differences in the allocation of study time account for age differences in memory performance
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
– volume: 20
  start-page: 6
  year: 1988
  end-page: 11
  ident: b0225
  article-title: The MRC psycholinguistic database: machine readable dictionary, version 2
  publication-title: Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers
– year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0075
  article-title: Metacognitive control of learning and remembering
– volume: 31
  start-page: 297
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0015
  article-title: Predicting and postdicting the effects of word frequency on memory
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194388
– volume: 20
  start-page: 374
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0060
  article-title: Importance of the kind of cue for judgements of learning (JOLs) and the delayed JOL effect
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03210921
– volume: 135
  start-page: 36
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0120
  article-title: The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: Lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.36
– volume: 32
  start-page: 609
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0130
  article-title: Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.609
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1352
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0220
  article-title: Metacognitive control over the distribution of practice. When is spacing preferred?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/a0016371
– volume: 18
  start-page: 159
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0160
  article-title: Metacognitive judgments and control of study
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01628.x
– volume: 26
  start-page: 204
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0210
  article-title: Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation
  publication-title: Journal of Experiment Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.1.204
– volume: 47
  start-page: 274
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0055
  article-title: Children’s spontaneous allocation of study time: Differential and sufficient aspects
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(89)90033-7
– volume: 52
  start-page: 463
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0175
  article-title: A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.001
– volume: 25
  start-page: 691
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0045
  article-title: Age differences in the allocation of study time account for age differences in memory performance
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211311
– volume: 149
  start-page: 87
  year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0050
  article-title: Developmental differences in children’s allocation of study time
  publication-title: The Journal of Genetic Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/00221325.1988.10532142
– volume: 95
  start-page: 66
  issue: 1
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0215
  article-title: Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.66
– volume: 20
  start-page: 6
  year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0225
  article-title: The MRC psycholinguistic database: machine readable dictionary, version 2
  publication-title: Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers
  doi: 10.3758/BF03202594
– volume: 154
  start-page: 223
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0110
  article-title: Spontaneous allocation of study-time by first- and third-grade children on a simple memory task
  publication-title: The Journal of Genetic Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/00221325.1993.9914736
– volume: 12
  start-page: 874
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0020
  article-title: Response speeding mediates the contribution of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196779
– volume: 143
  start-page: 643
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0115
  article-title: Predicting one’s own forgetting: The role of experience-based and theory-based processes
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.643
– volume: 2
  start-page: 21
  year: 1976
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0085
  article-title: Analysis of the word frequency effect in recognition memory
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.2.1.21
– volume: 17
  start-page: 281
  year: 1982
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0005
  article-title: Effects of inconsistent information on text processing: Evidence for comprehension monitoring
  publication-title: Reading Research Quarterly
  doi: 10.2307/747487
– year: 1966
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0090
– volume: 36
  start-page: 280
  year: 1972
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0135
  article-title: Regulation of study-time and interstimulus similarity in self-paced learning conditions
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(72)90012-1
– start-page: 195
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0190
  article-title: Why investigate metacognition?
– volume: 15
  start-page: 174
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0165
  article-title: Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.174
– start-page: 851
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0180
  article-title: The cue-familiarity heuristic in metacognition
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.851
– volume: 32
  start-page: 441
  year: 1969
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0230
  article-title: Invariance of total learning time under different conditions of practice
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/h0028369
– volume: 55
  start-page: 126
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0030
  article-title: Metacognitive control of the spacing of study repetitions
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.02.003
– volume: 319
  start-page: 966
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0105
  article-title: The critical importance of retrieval for learning
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1152408
– volume: 34
  start-page: 906
  year: 1979
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0080
  article-title: Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-development theory
  publication-title: American Psychologist
  doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
– volume: 132
  start-page: 530
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0170
  article-title: The dynamics of learning and the allocation of study time to a region of proximal learning
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.530
– volume: 30
  start-page: 601
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0205
  article-title: Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.601
– year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0245
– volume: 116
  start-page: 84
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0040
  article-title: Signal detection with criterion noise: Applications to recognition memory
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/a0014351
– volume: 17
  start-page: 249
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0195
  article-title: Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long term retention
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x
– volume: 12
  start-page: 83
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0070
  article-title: Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01235
– volume: 26
  start-page: 951
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0095
  article-title: Memorability judgments for high- and low-frequency words
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03201175
– volume: 127
  start-page: 55
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0035
  article-title: The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55
– volume: 33
  start-page: 545
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0250
  article-title: Does the sensitivity of judgments of learning (JOLs) to the effects of various study activities depend on when the JOLs occur?
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1026
– volume: 20
  start-page: 216
  year: 1981
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0200
  article-title: Memory monitoring accuracy and modification of rehearsal strategies
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90389-3
– volume: 92
  start-page: 160
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0100
  article-title: Test prediction and performance in a classroom context
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.160
– volume: 15
  start-page: 41
  year: 1980
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0235
  article-title: When you know that you know and when you think that you know but you don’t
  publication-title: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
  doi: 10.3758/BF03329756
– volume: Vol. 48
  start-page: 175
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0025
  article-title: Memory is more than just remembering: Strategic control of encoding, accessing memory, and making decisions
– volume: 37
  start-page: 52
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0155
  article-title: Remembering words not presented in sentences: How study context changes patterns of false memories
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/MC.37.1.52
– volume: 2
  start-page: 411
  year: 1971
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0010
  article-title: Learning strategies as determinants of memory deficiencies
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(71)90024-7
– volume: 32
  start-page: 779
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0255
  article-title: Causes and constraints of the shift-to-easier-materials effect in the control of study
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03195868
– volume: 122
  start-page: 47
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0150
  article-title: Strategies in study time allocation: Why is study time sometimes not effective?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.1.47
– volume: 5
  start-page: 207
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0185
  article-title: Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00502.x
– volume: 98
  start-page: 37
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0065
  article-title: What makes people study more? An evaluation of factors that affect self-paced study
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00051-6
– year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0140
– volume: 14
  start-page: 219
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0125
  article-title: The promises and perils of self-regulated study
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194055
– volume: 18
  start-page: 116
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002_b0145
  article-title: The basis of test predictions for text material
  publication-title: The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.1.116
SSID ssj0011572
Score 2.4259439
Snippet Metacognitive monitoring and control must be accurate and efficient in order to allow self-guided learners to improve their performance. Yet few examples exist...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
eric
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 109
SubjectTerms Biological and medical sciences
Cognition & reasoning
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Human
Independent Study
Language Acquisition
Learning
Learning Strategies
Learning. Memory
Memorization
Memory
Metacognition
Metacognitive control
Pacing
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Self Instruction
Self-pacing
Study Habits
Time
Time Management
Title On the effectiveness of self-paced learning
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ911668
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21516194
https://www.proquest.com/docview/835121850
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1826167446
https://www.proquest.com/docview/862776760
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC3079256
Volume 64
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Bb9MwFH7ausskhKCMEQaVkRAHUGicOE58nKZNZYxxgEm9WXZsQ6eSVqQ7cOG385w4gaKxA9fm2Wn87OfvPX9-D-Bl4VKlc8Fjnige4wxRsVBGxaWuGGNVmhXC33f-cMlnV-x8ns934KS_C-NplcH2dza9tdbhl2kYzel6sZh-ws1P4FvmNOCcXdhLM8HzEewdv3s_uxwOE2je1nDy8rFv0B9utjSv62_LjuDlc3mG4Mot21MgQ99bqwbHznVFL25DpX-TK__Yrc4ewP0AM8lx9yUPYcfWY1-hObA5xnB4EQKVDXlFLobcys0Y9geb-OMRvPlYE4SIpKN9BMtIVo40duli9LetIaHwxJcDuDo7_Xwyi0N9hVgh7NjEXJdVSRNhqL-hq7PS6EQLRzOnnccVVJVVZYvMljR3ObelyVNlcmdRCm0oyx7DqF7V9gmQNDNGOKMZSzQTtNJpplOHXSGAUSUvI0j6YZVVSD7ua2AsZc8yu5aoCek1gU6JRE1E8Hposu4yb9wlzHpdya3pI3FnuKvZgdfr0P_pOdp-7v_uZEvRgwACHHS4syKCo17zMiz6RiKYpYiY8iSCF8NTXK3-CEbVdnXTSO_N-XsfjEdA_iGDPqZPscSxm8NuKv1-PeIzH3aKoNiaZIOATxa-_aRefG2ThqMtFwhvn_7fQB3BfhdM9zyeZzDafL-xzxGNbfQEdt_-pJOw5iZt_OEXLZ4yeg
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Pb9MwFH4a48AkhEYZI4wNIyEOoKz54TjxcZo2ldGNA5vUm2UnNnQqaUW6Axf-dt5rnLCisQPX5sVp_Ozn7zmfvwfwNneJNpkUoYi0CHGE6FDqSoeFKTnnZZLmks47n1-I0RU_m2STDTjuzsIQrdLH_jamr6K1_2Xoe3O4mE6HX3Dxk_iUSexxzgN4yLM0J17f4a-e50FiMkmrxSlDMu8-ba5IXtffZy29i5Q8_dbKHYuTp0I_XugGe861JS_uwqR_UytvrVWn2_DEg0x21L7HU9iw9YDqM3suxwB2x36bsmHv2LhXVm4GsNVHxJ_P4MPnmiFAZC3pw8dFNnessTMXYrZtK-bLTnzdgavTk8vjUeirK4QaQccyFKYoiziSVUznc01aVCYy0sWpM45QRayLsrR5aos4c5mwRZUlusqcRSuMoDx9Dpv1vLYvgCVpVUlXGc4jw2VcmiQ1icOmEL7oQhQBRF23qtJLj1MFjJnqOGbXCj2hyBOYkij0RADv-1sWre7Gfca885VaGzwK14X7btshv_btn5xh5Bf0dw_WHN0bILzBdDvNA9jrPK_8lG8UQtkY8VIWBfCmv4pzlT7A6NrObxpFuRyd-uAiAPYPG8wwSWBJYDO77VD683hEZ7TpFEC-Nsh6A5IKX79ST7-tJMMxkksEty__r6New6PR5flYjT9efNqDrXZbnRg9r2Bz-ePG7iMuW5qD1bz7DZtcMkQ
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=On+the+Effectiveness+of+Self-Paced+Learning&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+memory+and+language&rft.au=Tullis%2C+Jonathan+G&rft.au=Benjamin%2C+Aaron+S&rft.date=2011-02-01&rft.pub=Elsevier&rft.issn=0749-596X&rft.volume=64&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=109&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.jml.2010.11.002&rft.externalDocID=EJ911668
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0749-596X&client=summon